ORCID Carola Minkels
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the
It is desirable that (more) children continue swimming after having completed their swimming lessons to preserve their swimming skills and water safety, and as part of an active, healthy lifestyle. This may be encouraged by stirring children's intrinsic motivation for swimming during swimming lessons. However, it is currently unknown how intrinsically motivating swimming lessons are in Western countries.
This study examined to what extent swimming instructors in the Netherlands cater to the basic needs of autonomy, competence, and relatedness, which, according to Self-Determination Theory (SDT), promote intrinsic motivation. Additionally, it examined whether an SDT-based teaching program prompts instructors to better meet these needs, and to what extent the teaching program, the education and experience of the instructor, and the group size predict the employment of SDT in swimming lessons.
A total of 128 swimming lessons given by equally many instructors were observed in the Netherlands and rated on a modified version of the SDT teaching style scale to assess autonomy, competence, and relatedness support. The swimming lessons referred to four teaching programs, one of which was explicitly based on SDT.
Instructors exhibited autonomy-thwarting, weakly competence-supportive, and relatedness-supportive behaviors. The SDT-based teaching program scored higher on the provision of autonomy, competence, and relatedness in lessons. This finding was significant for autonomy. Teaching program was the only significant predictor of SDT employment by instructors.
Further improvement is desirable in catering to the basic needs, particularly autonomy, which can be achieved by deliberately implementing the principles of SDT into teaching programs for swimming.
香京julia种子在线播放
All over the world children receive swimming lessons to secure their safety in water (
Before addressing this question, it is useful to highlight the benefits of actively continuing to swim after having learned to swim. Swimming is a very healthy, whole body physical activity that requires a mix of endurance and strength (
The latter aim may be achieved by incorporating the tenets of the self-determination theory (SDT) of Ryan and Deci (
In educational settings, the extent to which autonomy, competence, and relatedness are incorporated into an activity is reflected in a teacher's teaching style. Teaching style refers to the overall education and management strategies employed by teachers and has been classified as either autonomy- or control-oriented (
Furthermore, considering that the teaching style of swimming instructors may be associated with relatively stable personality traits, it is of interest to examine whether it is possible to make an individual swimming instructor's teaching style more autonomy-oriented, if deemed necessary or desirable. Reeve (
The question is whether the finding that teaching style is malleable generalizes to swimming instructors, which might not be the case for several reasons. Swimming lessons, school classes, and physical education lessons are all pedagogical situations where teachers try to teach children new skills. However, there are relevant differences between these learning environments. First, compared to regular school classes and also most physical education lessons, it is critically important to secure children's safety in the water during swimming lessons. Consequently, it may be more difficult to apply an autonomy-oriented teaching style during swimming lessons as this may endanger children's safety. Second, children in swimming lessons are typically aged between 5 and 7 years old, which is much younger than the students in the studies of Reeve (
In the Netherlands, three basic levels of swimming competence are distinguished, referred to as A, B, and C. Each level consists of a strictly defined set of skills and criteria. Once the skills corresponding to a certain level have been acquired, children can obtain a diploma by demonstrating those skills in an exam judged by a licensed swimming instructor. Although the skills that need to be demonstrated are strictly defined, there are no regulations regarding the way they should be taught in swimming lessons. As a result, considerable variation exists in the pedagogical and didactical principles and assumptions underpinning swim teaching programs in the Netherlands. Several organizations have developed such teaching programs, among which Easyswim, ZwemABC (NRZ), Optisport, and SuperSpetters (KNZB) are the most prominent. These programs have distinct objectives. Inspired by SDT, Easyswim strives to deliver engaging swimming lessons that are tailored to the specific needs of children (
Furthermore, in the Netherlands, there are three different ways to become a swimming instructor (general PE teacher education; general swimming instructor education; and swimming instructor education tailored to a specific program). These swimming instructor education programs all differ in the extent to which they address the SDT, if at all. Therefore, we also examined whether the education background influences the extent to which swimming instructors cater to the three basic needs during swimming lessons.
Finally, there may be several other factors that affect the employment of the principles of SDT in swimming lessons. For example, in educational research, Leroy, Bressoux, Sarrazin, and Trouilloud (
The aims of the present study were threefold. The primary aim was to examine to what extent swimming instructors in the Netherlands cater to the three basic psychological needs during swimming lessons. The second aim was to examine whether a teaching program based on the principles of SDT prompts swimming instructors to better meet the three basic psychological needs in comparison with other teaching programs. The third aim was to examine to what extent the applied teaching program, the instructor's education and experience, and the group size predict the employment of SDT principles in swimming lessons. To accomplish those aims, we first had to confirm the validity and reliability of Tessier et al.'s (
Regarding the first aim, we based our expectation on the commonly held but untested opinion about the state of swimming lessons in the Netherlands, namely that swimming instructors typically tend toward need-thwarting behaviors. Based on this general opinion, we expected to find average scores on the three subscales for autonomy, competence, and relatedness below a neutral 4. Regarding the second aim, we expected that the swimming program based on the principles of the SDT would prompt swimming instructors to better meet the three basic psychological needs of autonomy, competence, and relatedness, considering that previous research in other domains has shown that it is possible to change a person's teaching style to become more autonomy-oriented (
A power analysis (G*Power, 3.1.9.6, Kiel, Germany) for a one-way omnibus ANOVA with fixed effects was conducted to determine the number of swimming instructors (and thus the number of swimming lessons) that had to be observed for the study to be statistically meaningful. The power analysis with a moderate effect size f of 0.30, an alpha error probability of 0.05, and a power of 0.8, revealed that a minimum sample size of 128 swimming instructors providing an equal number of swimming lessons was required.
Swimming instructors in the Netherlands were invited to participate in the study through the nation-wide professional networks of the Nationale Raad Zwemveiligheid (NRZ) and the national swimming center, InnoSportlab de Tongelreep at Eindhoven. Through these networks, the required sample size of 128 swimming instructors was achieved (age = 40.28 ± 14.51 years; 101 female, 27 male; mean ± standard deviation), who were affiliated to a total of 42 different swimming schools. The 128 observed swimming lessons were distributed as follows over the four teaching programs: Easyswim 32; ZwemABC 31; Optisport 33; and SuperSpetters 32. The swimming lessons were observed for the entire duration to fill out the SDT teaching style scale (see below). The lessons involved novice swimmers between 4 and 10 years of age, who were taught the leg movements of the different swimming strokes.
Ethical clearance of the study was provided by the Ethical Committee of the Faculty of Behavioral and Movement Sciences of the Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam (VCWE-2022-075) and all swimming instructors provided written informed consent before participation. Because the study concerned the observation of a regular educational activity, and involved no interventions nor the collection of data from individually identifiable children, it was sufficient to ask the instructors for permission and to inform the parents.
All verbalizations of the swimming instructors were recorded on audiotape using a waterproof microphone (Instamic Pro) when the swimming instructor taught from the water and a non-waterproof microphone (Debra DVO2) when the swimming instructor taught from the poolside. No video recordings were made in view of privacy considerations.
During the swimming lessons, practice aids such as kickboards, surfboards, fins, and trunks were provided to the children. Materials to climb over, jump over, swim through, and pick up from the pool floor were also used. No constraints were imposed on the use of swimming aids and materials.
To assess the extent of autonomy, competence, and relatedness support exhibited in the swimming lessons, the modified SDT teaching style scale of Tessier et al. (
A background information questionnaire was developed to obtain information about which education program instructors had followed to become a swimming instructor, how many years and how many hours per week instructors provided swimming lessons, and the number of children in the swimming lesson (
Students volunteered to observe the lessons provided by the swimming instructors as part of their education as human movement scientists. They were educated about the SDT and subsequently trained how to apply the modified SDT teaching style scale during a two-hour meeting led by the first author (CM). After the meeting, the students observed two swimming lessons and completed the modified SDT teaching style scale. Analysis of their scores revealed that sufficient reliability among raters had been achieved (see above).
Before observing the swimming lesson, the swimming instructor was informed about the observation procedure and given the opportunity to ask questions for clarification. We told the swimming instructor that the aim of the observation was to gain insight into the current teaching programs used during swimming lessons in the Netherlands. To this end, we asked the swimming instructor to provide a swimming lesson as usual. Specific aspects of the study, such as the exact aims of the study and the items observed, were not disclosed to the swimming instructor to avoid conscious adaptions of the swimming lesson or teaching style by the swimming instructor. Only after the observations were performed, the aims of the study were fully disclosed to the swimming instructor.
Every swimming lesson was observed independently by two trained raters, who were seated at separate locations but close to the pool to obtain an encompassing view of the swimming lesson. The modified SDT teaching style scale was completed at the end of the swimming lesson. In case of doubt, or if the observation was hindered because of poor acoustics, the raters listened to the audio recordings afterward and then completed the modified SDT teaching style scale. In cases where the raters' assessments did not align, the average score was computed and used in subsequent analyses.
When the swimming lesson was finished, the background information questionnaire was administered.
The data were prepared for statistical analysis in R studio (Rstudio-2022.07.2-576).
The total score on the modified SDT teaching style scale was calculated by averaging the ratings of all items, and then averaging it over the two raters. Additionally, the average scores for the items of the subscales autonomy, competence, and relatedness were calculated and also averaged over the two raters. Since the data were ordinal, the median instead of the mean was calculated for the total and subscale scores.
The instructor's experience was determined by multiplying the number of hours they taught per week by 44 (i.e., the number of weeks in a year minus holiday weeks), and then multiplying the resulting product by the number of years they had been teaching swimming lessons.
During each swimming lesson, one of the raters counted and recorded the number of children in the observed swimming lessons.
All statistical procedures were performed in Rstudio (Rstudio-2022.07.2-576). Statistical significance was set at
Confirmatory factor analysis was performed to check the structural validity of the modified SDT teaching style scale. The diagonally weighted least squares (DWLS) method was used for this analysis in view of the ordinal character of the data. It was tested whether the data fitted the hypothesized correlated three-factor model of the modified SDT teaching style scale (i.e., that the scale contains autonomy, competence, and relatedness as subscales). Items of the specific factors were constrained to load on the factor they belonged to (
Chi-square statistic (
Inter-rater reliability was assessed for the total SDT score, as well as for the scores on the subscales autonomy, competence, and relatedness. To this end, ICC based on a mean-rating (k = 2), absolute-agreement, One-Way Random-Effects Model was used. ICC values above 0.75 indicated good reliability (
Since the scale's midpoint (i.e., 4) was the threshold to distinguish between need-supportive and need-thwarting behaviors (
Kruskal-Wallis tests were performed to determine the effect of teaching program (group: Easyswim, ZwemABC, Optisport, SuperSpetters) on instructor's employment of SDT, instructor's autonomy support, instructor's provision of competence, and instructor's provision of relatedness in swimming lessons. Post hoc analyses were conducted using Wilcoxon's rank sum test with a Bonferroni correction of the alpha level to
Additionally, a hierarchical four-stepped linear regression analysis was performed to examine whether the teaching program (i.e., Easyswim, ZwemABC, Optisport, and SuperSpetters), the instructor's education and experience, and the group size predicted total and subscores on the modified SDT teaching style scale. Teaching program was entered as the first step in the regression, experience as the second step, education as the third step, and group size as the last step. Relevant assumptions were tested before conducting the hierarchical regression analysis. Q-Q plot analyses revealed that the data were normally distributed. Inspections of the residual plots showed that the assumptions of homogeneity of variance and linearity were met. Lastly, VIF values around 1 revealed that the assumption of multicollinearity was also met.
All 128 swimming instructors were included in the analysis.
Swimming Instructors’ characteristics for each teaching program (mean ± standard deviation).
Easyswim ( |
ZwemABC ( |
Optisport ( |
SuperSpetters ( |
|
---|---|---|---|---|
Age (years) | 36.24 ± 12.18 | 44.52 ± 13.31 | 43.55 ± 14.81 | 37.21 ± 16.14 |
Gender (percentage female) | 68.75% | 90.32% | 78.79% | 78.13% |
Experience (years) | 9.40 ± 10.90 | 17.15 ± 11.99 | 16.80 ± 11.78 | 12.26 ± 12.00 |
Education (percentage certified) | 93.75% | 87.10% | 93.94% | 71.88% |
Group size (number of children) | 7.16 ± 2.05 | 8.55 ± 3.38 | 9.79 ± 3.35 | 7.41 ± 2.30 |
Duration swimming lesson (minutes) | 48.75 ± 13.56 | 46.45 ± 11.85 | 50.91 ± 14.81 | 48.44 ± 19.90 |
Model fit indices showed acceptable to good fit for CFI, TLI, and SRMR (CFI = 0.988; TLI = 0.983; SRMR = 0.078), and poor fit for
Confirmatory factor analysis of the modified SDT teaching style scale.
Factor structure | df | CFI | TLI | RMSEA (90% CI) | SRMR | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
All items included | 64.920 | 32 | 0.001 | 0.988 | 0.983 | 0.090 (0.058–0.121) | 0.078 |
Exclusion of item C1 | 51.192 | 24 | 0.001 | 0.990 | 0.985 | 0.094 (0.058–0.130) | 0.077 |
Exclusion of item R3 | 34.560 | 24 | 0.075 | 0.996 | 0.994 | 0.059 (0.000–0.100) | 0.062 |
Exclusion of items C1 and R3 | 22.339 | 17 | 0.172 | 0.998 | 0.997 | 0.050 (0.000–0.100) | 0.056 |
Item C1, degree of differentiation; Item R3, interaction between children;
Reliability measures.
All items included | Exclusion of item R3 | |
---|---|---|
ICC (95% CI) | ||
Total scale | 0.94 (0.93–0.95) | 0.94 (0.93–0.94) |
Autonomy | 0.96 (0.95–0.96) | 0.96 (0.95–0.96) |
Competence | 0.85 (0.83–0.86) | 0.85 (0.83–0.86) |
Relatedness | 0.95 (0.95–0.96) | 0.86 (0.84–0.87) |
Internal consistency (alpha) (95% CI) | ||
Total scale | 0.81 (0.75–0.85) | 0.81 (0.76–0.86) |
Autonomy | 0.65 (0.55–0.76) | 0.65 (0.55–0.76) |
Competence | 0.53 (0.39–0.68) | 0.53 (0.39–0.68) |
Relatedness | 0.68 (0.58–0.78) | 0.91 (0.87–0.94) |
SEM | ||
Total scale | 0.40 | 0.40 |
Autonomy | 0.41 | 0.41 |
Competence | 0.53 | 0.53 |
Relatedness | 0.34 | 0.20 |
MDCgroup | ||
Total scale | 0.16 | 0.16 |
Autonomy | 0.20 | 0.20 |
Competence | 0.28 | 0.28 |
Relatedness | 0.18 | 0.11 |
MDCindividual | ||
Total scale | 1.11 | 1.11 |
Autonomy | 1.14 | 1.14 |
Competence | 1.47 | 1.47 |
Relatedness | 0.93 | 0.57 |
ICC, intraclass correlation coefficient; MDC, minimal detectable change; SEM, standard error of the measurement.
The median score for the total scale was 4.28 points, while the median scores on the subscales autonomy, competence, and relatedness were 3.50, 4.33, and 5.50 points, respectively (
Scores for the three basic psychological needs. The bold horizontal black lines indicate the median, the grey areas the interquartile range, and the vertical dotted lines the range of the scores. The circles represent outliers.
Total (top) and subscores (bottom) for the three basic psychological needs according to teaching program. The bold horizontal black lines indicate the median, the grey areas the interquartile range, and the vertical lines the range of the scores. The circles represent outliers.
The Kruskal-Wallis test also revealed a significant effect of teaching program on the subscore for autonomy, H(3) = 20.192,
The hierarchical multiple regression revealed that in step one, teaching program contributed significantly to the regression model, F(3, 124) = 4.87,
Hierarchical multiple regression for total SDT score, and autonomy, competence, and relatedness subscales.
Total SDT score | Autonomy | Competence | Relatedness | |||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Predictor | F | F | F | F | ||||||||||||
Model 1 | ||||||||||||||||
1. Teaching program | 0.11 | 0.11 | 4.87 | <0.01 | 0.17 | 0.17 | 8.27 | <0.01 | 0.05 | 0.05 | 2.29 | 0.08 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.35 | 0.79 |
Model 2 | ||||||||||||||||
2. Experience | 0.13 | 0.02 | 3.20 | 0.08 | 0.19 | 0.02 | 3.12 | 0.08 | 0.08 | 0.02 | 3.24 | 0.07 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.31 | 0.58 |
Model 3 | ||||||||||||||||
3. Education | 0.18 | 0.05 | 1.18 | 0.32 | 0.25 | 0.06 | 1.47 | 0.20 | 0.13 | 0.05 | 1.06 | 0.39 | 0.04 | 0.03 | 0.59 | 0.74 |
Model 4 | ||||||||||||||||
4. Group size | 0.18 | 0.00 | 0.08 | 0.78 | 0.25 | 0.00 | 0.20 | 0.65 | 0.13 | 0.00 | 0.11 | 0.74 | 0.04 | 0.00 | 0.45 | 0.50 |
The present study had three aims: (i) to examine to what extent Dutch swimming instructors cater to the three basic psychological needs during swimming lessons; (ii) to examine to what extent a teaching program based on SDT prompts swimming instructors to better meet the three basic psychological needs of SDT, and (iii) to examine to what extent the teaching program, the instructor's education and experience, and the group size predict the employment of SDT in swimming lessons by swimming instructors.
Before discussing the findings in light of our expectations and previous research findings, we address the SDT teaching style scale's validity and reliability. Previous studies already confirmed the SDT teaching style scale's internal validity and reliability (
First, regarding the scale's structural validity, when the original SDT teaching style scale was analyzed with the inclusion of item R3 (interaction between children), it yielded somewhat contradictory results. Some fit indices indicated acceptable to good levels of fit, while a few other fit indices had a poor fit. The model's fit indices were significantly enhanced by eliminating item R3 from the scale. We reasoned that promoting interactions between children would be an important manifestation of relatedness supporting behavior (
The scale's interrater reliability was good to excellent. The internal consistency was acceptable for the subscales autonomy and relatedness, but poor for the unmodified subscale competence. Especially item C1 (degree of differentiation) displayed a lack of coherence with the remaining items on the competence subscale. It was reasoned that more differentiation would support perceived competence (
The scale's minimal detectable change was 0.16 points at the group level and 1.11 points at the individual level, suggesting that the scale is suitable to compare the employment of SDT across different groups, such as teaching programs, but less suitable for monitoring changes in the employment of SDT by individual instructors.
Altogether, the scale's validity was confirmed for autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Moreover, the scale's internal consistency was corroborated for autonomy and relatedness. However, conflicting results were found for competence demonstrating poor internal consistency in our study, but good internal consistency in the study of Tessier et al. (
Now that the SDT teaching style scale's validity and reliability have been assessed, we can proceed to discuss our main findings. First and foremost, Dutch swimming instructors scored on average 3.50, 4.33, and 5.50 points for the employment of autonomy, competence, and relatedness in swimming lessons, respectively. We found that the median subscore for autonomy was significantly lower than 4, while the median subscores for competence and relatedness were significantly higher than 4. This indicates that Dutch swimming instructors mostly showed autonomy-thwarting behaviors, competence-supportive behaviors, and relatedness-supportive behaviors during their swimming lessons. This result only partially supports our hypothesis that instructors would typically exhibit need-thwarting behaviors since this does not apply to competence and relatedness. However, the median score of 4.33 for competence suggests only weakly supportive behaviors, considering that the score is in close proximity to the threshold of 4 and is still significantly lower than the maximum score of 7 points. Moreover, the median score of 5.50 for relatedness is also still considerably lower than 7. Therefore, although there may be some incoherence between the behaviors on the competence subscale, these findings suggest there is still room to better cater to all three fundamental needs of SDT in swimming lessons in the Netherlands.
The present results are consistent with those of Reeve, Jang et al. (
Our observations show that Dutch swimming instructors cater significantly less to autonomy than competence and relatedness, with the latter being catered to most effectively. This may have several reasons. First, considering that there is always a risk of drowning while learning to swim, providing autonomy support during swimming lessons may endanger children's safety, which might prompt instructors to adopt a more controlling teaching style. Second, children in swimming lessons may be too young to respond positively to autonomy, or instructors may assume this to be the case, resulting in more controlling behaviors. Third, it might be that instructors are not familiar with the potential benefits of an autonomy-oriented teaching style, either because it was no part of their education or what they have been taught was not sufficiently applied in practice. As a result, instructors may believe that a controlling teaching style is necessary to teach children how to swim. However, it might well be that teaching children more playfully with a great deal of autonomy may result in the same or perhaps even better learning outcomes (
In fact, and this speaks to the study's second aim, our results indicate that it is feasible to enhance swimming instructors’ ability to cater to the need for autonomy through training, given that instructors affiliated with Easyswim succeeded significantly better in this regard than instructors following other teaching programs, not based on SDT. Moreover, it also seems feasible to encourage swimming instructors to strengthen children's perceptions of competence and relatedness during swimming lessons, as Easyswim instructors received higher scores in these areas, although the differences were not statistically significant. Roughly speaking, our findings are consistent with our expectation that SDT-based teaching would prompt swimming instructors to better cater to all three basic psychological needs.
The lack of statistical significance for competence and relatedness can be explained as follows. It could well be more difficult to improve swimming instructors' competence- and relatedness-supportive behaviors, as these two needs are already better implemented in swimming lessons than autonomy. This assumption is supported by the findings of Reeve, Jang et al. (
As regards the study's third aim, only the teaching program was found to be a significant predictor of the employment of SDT by swimming instructors. Instructor's education and experience, and the size of the swimming class, had no significant influence on the employment of SDT in swimming lessons. This result stands in contrast with our hypothesis and with previous research in educational settings showing that instructors' experience and group size did influence the employment of SDT in class (
In conclusion it is useful to discuss the strengths and limitations of our study, beginning with the study's strengths. This research extends previous research findings obtained in educational settings demonstrating that is possible to expand a teacher's teaching style to become more autonomy-, competence-, and relatedness-supportive (
We established those results in a study with a considerably larger sample size than in previous research. With over 120 observations of swimming lessons provided by equally many instructors, the study involved a diverse group of instructors, providing an accurate and comprehensive assessment of the employment of the SDT in swimming lessons in the Netherlands. By including a large sample of instructors, working all over the Netherlands, our findings can be considered representative for the state of swimming education in the Netherlands, which may well be found to be representative for other Western countries as well. However, it should be explicitly noted that our study did not include the observation of swimming lessons in countries with different cultural, social, or economic backgrounds, which implies that the present findings should not be generalized to those countries in the absence of cross-cultural research on this matter.
As regard the study's limitations, one potential shortcoming is that we only observed relatedness supportive behaviors involving child-instructor interactions. Because of the exclusion of item R3 (interaction between children), no behaviors involving child-child interactions were observed. Consequently, no conclusions can be made about the relatedness among children during swimming lessons.
Another potential drawback could be that, although we asked the instructors to provide a swimming lesson as usual and did not inform them about the real aims of the observational study, the possibility remains that instructors unconsciously altered their teaching behaviors and made efforts to provide a more engaging and enjoyable lesson, given that their performance was being evaluated. Nonetheless, due to the large number of lessons observed, involving a diverse group of instructors, we believe these unconscious modifications in instructional behavior had little to no impact on our findings.
Another limitation is that we did not assess other factors that might have influenced the extent to which instructors employ the SDT in swimming lessons, such as personality traits (
Since the insights we obtained can be applied to help improve swimming lessons for children, it is also useful to address their practical implications before formulating the study's general conclusion. For starters, the design of swimming lessons should facilitate a considerable level of autonomy-, competence- and relatedness support among children. This requires a markedly different orientation and skill set from instructors than currently utilized in more traditional teaching approaches that are predominantly focused on the improvement of swimming technique(s). Although it remains to be demonstrated, we believe that if teaching programs, and thus swimming instructors, would prioritize the development of a learning environment that offers abundant affordances and challenges for children, while simultaneously guiding and monitoring children's progress in swimming performance, children will not only develop their swimming skills but also their intrinsic motivation for swimming. To achieve this, swimming instructor's education programs should focus more on the development of pedagogical and didactic skills of (future) instructors, with specific emphasis on employing SDT in swimming lessons. At present, such an approach is lacking in most current education programs.
The findings for Easyswim demonstrate that it is possible to incorporate SDT into swimming lessons by employing its principles into a teaching program. However, this does not imply their teaching program is the best pedagogic or didactic teaching program, nor that this teaching program will result in the highest intrinsic motivation. Our findings do also not demonstrate that the teaching program of Easyswim will lead to better learning results and better swimming performance, nor to more children that keep swimming or join a swimming club. Further research is needed to investigate these aspects.
In conclusion, Dutch swimming instructors mostly exhibit autonomy-thwarting, weakly competence-supportive, and relatedness-supportive behaviors to satisfy SDT's three basic psychological needs during swimming lessons. This suggests that the basic need for autonomy is implemented to a lesser extent in swimming lessons than competence and relatedness, with the latter being employed most effectively. The findings also show that it is possible for swimming instructors to improve their autonomy-supportive behaviors if the teaching program is grounded in the principles of SDT. Children's intrinsic motivation to continue swimming beyond the completion of their teaching program(s) will likely benefit from such programs, potentially resulting in more children joining a swimming club and the maintenance of swimming skills over years.
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
The studies involving humans were approved by Ethical Committee of the Faculty of Behavioral and Movement Sciences of the Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam (VCWE-2022-075). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The swimming instructors provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
CM, PB, and JK: conceptualized and designed the study. CM: performed the data processing. CM: was involved in data collection. CM, JK, and PB: drafted the current manuscript. All authors contributed substantially to the interpretation of the data, revised it critically, and approved the final version of the manuscript before submission. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
This research project was funded by the InnoSportLab De Tongelreep, National Swimming Safety Council, and the Royal Dutch Swimming Federation, all of which are non-profit organizations without a commercial interest in the outcome of the project.
The authors express their gratitude to Brigitte Muller, Roald van der Vliet, Demian Kortekaas, and Carlo van der Heijden for their valuable contributions to this study. Additionally, we thank Brigitte Muller, Claartje Driessen, and Martijn Groeneveld for their assistance in organizing the observations of the swimming lessons. We also extend our thanks to the team of InnoSportLab de Tongelreep and the students of the study of human movement sciences for their help in observing the swimming lessons. Finally, we would like to thank all swimming instructors who graciously allowed us to observe their swimming lessons.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: