Edited by: Sarah Morsbach Honaker, Indiana University Bloomington, United States
Reviewed by: David Ingram, Children's Mercy Kansas City, United States; Maria Paola Mogavero, Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Italy
This article was submitted to Pediatric and Adolescent Sleep, a section of the journal Frontiers in Sleep
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
Craniopharyngioma is a brain tumor arising in the region of the hypothalamic-pituitary axis. Children and adolescents with craniopharyngioma have high survival rates, but often experience significant morbidity, including high rates of sleep disorders. Vulnerabilities to circadian disruption are present in this population, but little is known about circadian health.
We present exploratory circadian findings from a prospective trial at a single center. Data presented here are from the baseline timepoint. Fifty-three patients between the ages of 7 and 20 years provided salivary melatonin samples, following surgical resection and prior to completion of proton therapy, when indicated. We estimated dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) and collected additional sleep data from actigraphy, overnight polysomnography, and the multiple sleep latency test.
Almost half of participants did not have a valid DLMO estimate during the sampling window, with most being above the threshold at the first sample timepoint. Those with greater disease severity variables (greater hypothalamic involvement and the presence of diabetes insipidus) were significantly more likely to have missed DLMO. For those with valid estimates, DLMO timing correlated with BMI and other sleep variables, including mean sleep latency values on the MSLT.
These findings suggest that a subset of those with pediatric craniopharyngioma may experience a phase advance and that this may relate to poorer prognostic indicators. Furthermore, circadian timing correlates with other sleep and health factors. Further research with earlier sampling is needed to better understand circadian rhythms in pediatric craniopharyngioma and associations with other health and disease variables.
香京julia种子在线播放
Craniopharyngioma is a rare intracranial tumor located in the hypothalamic and pituitary region (e.g., Halac and Zimmerman,
Related to sleep disturbance, rates of narcolepsy and hypersomnia due to medical condition in pediatric craniopharyngioma are as high as 80%, despite the low prevalence of these conditions in the general population (e.g., Plazzi et al.,
Damage to the hypothalamic region also suggests likely disruptions to circadian rhythms, which are regulated by the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) in the hypothalamus (Dibner et al.,
A theoretical overview of potential mechanisms of circadian rhythm disruption is outlined in
Theoretical mechanisms of circadian disruption in craniopharyngioma.
Dynamic vulnerabilities may interact with and be further exacerbated by resulting circadian disruption. Behavioral changes, such as increased napping, decreased physical activity, and increased caloric intake/changes to meal timing may lead to disruption and misalignment between circadian timing and behavior. This may further contribute to weight gain and poor sleep health (e.g., Carskadon and Acebo,
Thus far, a few studies have investigated the potential for circadian rhythm disruption in craniopharyngioma by measuring overall levels of melatonin, a hormone that helps establish circadian rhythmicity (Benloucif et al.,
While these findings are suggestive of potential circadian rhythm disruption, these studies have typically been small and/or included a wide age range from children to adults. Further study in a large sample of children and adolescents with craniopharyngioma would be beneficial for more precise characterization of circadian health in pediatric craniopharyngioma. In addition to measuring overall melatonin levels, the timing of melatonin secretion provides key information about circadian phase. Dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) reflects the timing of the rapid increase in melatonin secretion that occurs just prior to habitual bedtime (Crowley et al.,
The present study is derived from an exploratory aim of a treatment protocol for pediatric craniopharyngioma. Details of the larger treatment study are provided below. The purpose of the exploratory aim was to examine circadian factors in pediatric craniopharyngioma. Within this aim, we had two study goals:
To estimate DLMO values and phase angles of entrainment in our sample of children and adolescents with craniopharyngioma. Phase angles of entrainment demonstrate alignment between circadian phase and sleep timing and are defined as the time between DLMO and sleep onset and offset (Crowley et al.,
To examine correlations between DLMO timing and clinical variables, including hypothalamic involvement (HI), body mass index (BMI), Tanner stage, presence of a disorder of hypersomnolence, and additional sleep parameters, such as nighttime sleep duration, sleep onset latency, sleep efficiency, and wake after sleep onset.
Prior research on circadian factors and craniopharyngioma has been focused on melatonin levels, rather than timing of melatonin secretion. Therefore, our study hypotheses were largely exploratory. We hypothesized that DLMO timing would correlate with the presence of hypersomnia due to medical disorder or narcolepsy, sleep duration, and sleep efficiency, consistent with reported associations between low melatonin and other sleep parameters (self-reported daytime sleepiness, decreased nighttime sleep duration, reduced sleep efficiency; Müller et al.,
Participants (
All eligible patients agreed to participate. They were enrolled following surgical resection and prior to proton therapy (when indicated). Measures were collected at the baseline timepoint, defined as within 12 weeks of initiation of therapy or observation. At baseline, treatments recommended by St. Jude physicians for co-occurring conditions, such as endocrine or sleep conditions, had not yet been initiated. Participants provided melatonin samples 24.8 ± 15.3 (average ± SD) days prior to starting proton therapy. Recruitment of patients took place between 2011 and 2017.
All procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board of St. Jude Children's Research Hospital. Parents and adult participants provided consent, and pediatric participants provided assent for participation in the study. Fifty-nine participants met inclusion criteria and were asked to provide saliva samples for the study. Five participants did not provide sufficient samples for analysis (further information below), and one took a melatonin supplement when providing a sample; therefore, the final study sample included 53 participants.
Demographic and health characteristics were compiled as part of the larger treatment protocol. Hypothalamic involvement (HI), visual field deficit, and visual acuity were all graded categorically. HI was coded based on neuroimaging, with grade 0 (no HI), grade 1 (anterior HI), and grade 2 (anterior and posterior HI; Müller et al.,
We followed the procedure for obtaining in-home saliva collection previously outlined in Mandrell et al. (
Saliva samples were color-coded according to their collection time and stored overnight in a labeled box in the family's freezer. The following morning, participants and their caregivers delivered the samples to the study nurse. The samples were stored in a freezer at −80°C until they were shipped to Salimetrics® for analysis. Melatonin concentration levels were then extracted from saliva samples by Salimetrics® with an ELISA assay. In order to have adequate quantity for the assay, the recommended quantity for each saliva test sample was 100 μl; quantities less than this were considered of insufficient quantity.
DLMO was calculated based on a 4 pg/mL threshold (Carskadon et al.,
To assess sleep-wake patterns, patients were instructed to wear a Micromini Sleep Watch® on their wrist for 5–7 nights (Ambulatory Monitoring Inc., Ardsley, NY). Data were recorded in 1-min epochs and scored using the Sadeh algorithm (Sadeh et al.,
Participants completed an overnight polysomnography (PSG) and multiple sleep latency test (MSLT) the following day. These studies were typically performed beginning within 30 min of the subject's usual bedtime. We followed a standard MSLT protocol with 4 or 5 nap opportunities at 2-h intervals (Littner et al.,
We calculated DLMO timing for our sample as described above and were able to estimate DLMO for approximately half of our sample (
We examined Spearman's rank correlations between DLMO values and the following variables: age, BMI, Tanner stage, HI, sleep onset latency (SOL) during the MSLT. We also examined Spearman's rank correlations between DLMO values and the averages and standard deviations of the following actigraphy variables: wake after sleep onset (WASO), sleep efficiency (SE), SOL, sleep duration, and sleep onset time, midsleep time, and wake time. Finally, we examined Wilcoxon Mann-Whitney U tests to compare whether DLMO differed between those with or without disorders of hypersomnolence, with or without narcolepsy, with or without diabetes insipidus, and dichotomized puberty status (Tanner stage 1 vs. stages 2–5).
We were interested in understanding whether there were demographic or clinical differences between subgroups for whom we were and were not able to estimate DLMO. Therefore, patient characteristics were summarized by descriptive and clinical characteristics for both the DLMO and missed DLMO subgroups. Associations between measurement of DLMO and other variables were examined with the following analyses: Fisher's exact test—sleep disorder category (hypersomnia, narcolepsy, neither) and race (Black, White, Other); chi-square test—sex, presence of diabetes insipidus, and dichotomized puberty status (i.e., pre-pubertal, pubertal), Cochran Armitage trend test (HI, visual acuity, visual field deficit, Tanner stage). Due to non-normal distribution of the following continuous variables, and a small sample size, Wilcoxon Mann-Whitney U tests were performed to detect the median difference in age, BMI, MSLTsol and SOREMP between the DLMO and Missed DLMO subgroups.
In addition to demographic and clinical characteristics, we examined differences in sleep behavior and variability in sleep timing between the subgroups by comparing the means and standard deviations of the actigraphy variables with Wilcoxon Mann-Whitney U tests. All statistical analyses were completed with SPSS version 25 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY) and (SAS Institute, Cary, NC).
For the participants for whom we could not estimate DLMO (
Participant demographic, sleep, and health characteristics are presented in
Demographic, sleep, and health characteristics.
53 | 28 | 25 | ||
0.957 | ||||
Mean (SD) | 11.70 (3.74) | 11.79 (3.97) | 11.60 (3.56) | |
Median (IQR) | 11.00 (9.00–15.00) | 11.00 (8.50–15.00) | 10.00 (9.00–15.00) | |
0.487 | ||||
Female | 26 (49.1%) | 15 (53.6%) | 11 (44.0%) | |
Male | 27 (50.9%) | 13 (46.4%) | 14 (56.0 %) | |
0.810 | ||||
Asian | 2 (3.8%) | 2 (7.1%) | 0 (0%) | |
Black or African American | 11 (20.8%) | 6 (21.4%) | 5 (20.0%) | |
Multiracial | 5 (9.4%) | 2 (7.1%) | 3 (12.0%) | |
Other | 1 (1.9%) | 1 (3.6%) | 0 (0%) | |
White | 34 (64.2%) | 17 (60.7%) | 17 (68.0%) | |
0.838 | ||||
Hypersomnia | 25 (47.2%) | 15 (53.6%) | 10 (40.0%) | |
Narcolepsy | 19 (35.9%) | 9 (32.1%) | 10 (40.0%) | |
No Diagnosis | 7 (13.2%) | 3 (10.7%) | 4 (16.0%) | |
Missing | 2 (3.8%) | 1 (3.6%) | 1 (4.0%) | |
0.021 |
||||
0 | 7 (13.2%) | 7 (25.0%) | 0 (0%) | |
1 | 16 (30.2%) | 8 (28.6%) | 8 (32.0%) | |
2 | 30 (56.6%) | 13 (46.4%) | 17 (68.0%) | |
0.876 | ||||
0 | 33 (62.3%) | 19 (67.9%) | 14 (56.0%) | |
1 | 4 (7.5%) | 1 (3.6%) | 3 (12.0%) | |
2 | 14 (26.4%) | 8 (28.6%) | 6 (24.0%) | |
Missing | 2 (3.8%) | 0 (0%) | 2 (8.0%) | |
0.341 | ||||
0 | 37 (69.8%) | 22 (78.6%) | 15 (60.0%) | |
1 | 5 (9.4%) | 1 (3.6%) | 4 (16.0%) | |
2 | 4 (7.5%) | 2 (7.1%) | 2 (8.0%) | |
3 | 6 (11.3%) | 3 (10.7%) | 3 (12.0%) | |
4 | 1 (1.9%) | 0 (0%) | 1 (4.0%) | |
1.000 | ||||
1 (pre-pubertal) | 32 (60.4%) | 17 (60.7%) | 15 (60.0%) | |
2–5 (pubertal) | 18 (34.0%) | 9 (32.1%) | 9 (36.0%) | |
Missing | 3 (5.7%) | 2 (7.1%) | 1 (4.0%) | |
0.304 | ||||
Mean (SD) | 23.09 (5.06) | 22.04 (4.18) | 24.27 (5.76) | |
Median (IQR) | 22.00 (19.40–26.50) | 22.00 (19.40–24.80) | 22.80 (19.35–29.35) | |
0.040 |
||||
Present, |
26 (49.1%) | 10 (35.7%) | 16 (64.0%) | |
0.288 | ||||
Mean (SD) | 9.56 (9.76) | 9.33 (5.46) | 9.82 (13.03) | |
Median (IQR) | 7.60 (3.69–13.00) | 9.25 (4.70–13.10) | 6.00 (3.50-11.00) | |
0.300 | ||||
Mean (SD) | 1.16 (1.36) | 1.00 (1.36) | 1.35 (1.37) | |
Median (IQR) | 1.00 (0–2.00) | 0 (0–2.00) | 1.00 (0–3.00) |
Sample is separated into those with valid dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) estimates and those without. P-values indicate comparisons in demographic variables between DLMO and Missed-DLMO subgroups. Due to low frequencies in some racial categories, differences in race were examined as Black, White, Other, with Other including Asian, multiracial, and other. BMI, body mass index; DI, diabetes insipidus; MSLTsol, mean sleep latency measured on multiple sleep latency test; SOREMP, sleep onset rapid eye movement periods measured on multiple sleep latency test.
Polysomnography findings.
51 | |
Mean (SD) | 473.02 (57.63) |
Mean (SD) | 89.85 (6.87) |
Mean (SD) | 23.23 (41.72) |
Mean (SD) | 1.29 (1.87) |
Mean (SD) | 8.39 (11.66) |
There were two significant health characteristic differences identified between the subgroups of DLMO and missed DLMO. A higher grade of hypothalamic involvement was associated with higher likelihood of missed DLMO, Cochran Armitage trend test,
In terms of sleep behavior, the missed DLMO subgroup had a longer median SOL (averaged over all actigraphy days) of 32.10 min than the DLMO subgroup median SOL of 17.75 min (
Actigraphy comparisons between DLMO and missed DLMO.
Median (IQR) | Median (IQR) | ||
Average | 21:49:30 (21:29:40–22:06:30) | 21:44:35 (21:07:30–22:03:30) | 0.559 |
SD | 00:31:17 (00:19:08–00:48:52) | 00:37:42 (00:22:53–01:19:56) | 0.508 |
Average | 7:11:15 (6:48:30–7:27:30) | 7:21:05 (6:40:18–7:47:06) | 0.439 |
SD | 00:30:59 (00:28:29–00:41:08) | 00:47:14 (00:30:12–01:08:20) | 0.058 |
Average | 2:26:27 (2:03:45–2:26:27) | 2:27:39 (2:06:35–2:57:35) | 0.801 |
SD | 00:26:08 (00:22:02–00:35:04) | 00:39:34 (00:25:52–00:50:52) | 0.061 |
Average | 560.10 (528.50–591.67) | 575.90 (528.27–609.20) | 0.593 |
SD | 47.30 (31.32–64.59) | 64.79 (46.97–99.38) | 0.083 |
Average | 91.19 (83.60–95.49) | 87.36 (83.55–91.14) | 0.154 |
SD | 4.31 (3.25–7.39) | 5.33 (3.25–8.10) | 0.767 |
Average | 17.75 (10.33–29.50) | 32.10 (20.80–41.17) | 0.005 |
SD | 6.53 (4.11–19.42) | 19.38 (12.50–26.34) | 0.043 |
Average | 46.30 (26.00–88.50) | 66.83 (49.23–89.50) | 0.069 |
SD | 23.27 (19.91–52.64) | 30.19 (19.04–40.83) | 0.922 |
Sample is separated into those with valid dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) estimates and those without. Sample sizes for analyses with averages are
DLMO and phase angles of entrainment are presented in
DLMO and phase angles of entrainment.
DLMO (hours) | 28 | 21.11 (1.23) |
DLMO to bedtime phase angle (minutes) | 26 | 40.88 (72.53) |
DLMO to wake time phase angle (minutes) | 26 | 604.38 (76.99) |
DLMO, dim light melatonin onset. Ns for DLMO to bedtime and DLMO to wake time phase angles are 26 because two participants with valid DLMO estimates did not have valid actigraphy data.
In this study, we examined circadian rhythms among children and adolescents with craniopharyngioma by estimating DLMO. DLMO was missed for almost half our sample (
The likelihood of missed DLMO was not associated with any examined demographic variables. However, it was associated with greater disease severity, as defined by hypothalamic involvement and presence of diabetes insipidus. Diabetes insipidus, unless present prior to surgery, reflects extensive surgical resection (Edmonston et al.,
We also examined whether missed DLMO may have been related to sleep health variables. Surprisingly, missed DLMO did not relate to the presence of a central disorder of hypersomnolence. This was an unexpected finding, although it could have been due to the very high rates (83%) of these conditions in our sample, limiting variability. We found that patients in the missed DLMO subgroup took longer to fall asleep at night and had a more variable sleep onset latency, as measured by actigraphy, than those in the DLMO subgroup. It is not clear why patients in the missed DLMO subgroup had longer and more variable SOL. Many of these patients showed melatonin values that were above threshold at the beginning of sampling, which suggests that their DLMO may have occurred earlier (phase advanced). However, we do not know for certain if this is the case without more data to confirm a phase advance. It is also possible that those in the missed DLMO subgroup have circadian dysregulation overall, which could be directly or indirectly (e.g., napping behavior) contributing to lengthened sleep onset latency. Unfortunately, we did not gather data on napping behavior.
For those with a valid DLMO estimate, our hypotheses were partially supported. Although DLMO timing was not associated with the presence or absence of hypersomnia or narcolepsy, later DLMO timing was associated with increased sleepiness severity on the MSLT (reflected by a shorter average sleep onset latency). That we observed significant differences when data were looked at continuously, but not categorically, is not entirely surprising given the very high rates of disorders of hypersomnolence in our sample. In contrast to our hypotheses, we did not find associations between DLMO timing and sleep duration or efficiency. However, later DLMO timing was associated, as one might expect, with later midsleep and wake times. Later DLMO timing was also associated with decreased variability in actigraphy sleep onset time. Consistent with our hypothesis that DLMO timing and BMI would be associated, we found that patients with higher BMIs had later DLMO timing. Finally, we did not find an association between DLMO timing and age or Tanner stage, which is inconsistent with well-established findings of circadian shift in adolescence (Carskadon,
Overall, our findings suggest possible circadian phase advance in a subset of children and adolescents with pediatric craniopharyngioma and that circadian timing is related to additional health factors. It will be important to further study circadian rhythms in these patients in order to inform clinical practice. In clinical settings, DLMO is not routinely used, but actigraphy may be used as a proxy to identify patients with advanced phase or other forms of circadian disruption. Note, however, that validation of using actigraphy to estimate circadian phase has not been systematically tested in this patient group. If indicated, targeted interventions, such as bright light therapy and melatonin administration, may be beneficial (Abbott et al.,
This project had a few limitations. Our inability to capture DLMO for almost half our sample limits our ability to draw conclusions about circadian timing in this population. In addition, actigraphy collection was too brief to identify circadian rhythm abnormalities. We also did not recruit control participants. Although there are healthy norms published for DLMO and phase angles of entrainment among children and adolescents (Crowley et al.,
This project also had several strengths. Given the rarity of pediatric craniopharyngioma, we were able to recruit a relatively large cohort of patients. Our sample was well-characterized with all patients recruited prior to proton therapy. We gathered data with robust and well-validated sleep-wake and circadian rhythm measures (salivary melatonin, actigraphy, PSG/MSLT). Finally, we successfully collected saliva samples outside of the lab, which should have permitted a more normative sleep/wake schedule.
Given challenges with DLMO estimation, it will be important for future studies to begin sampling earlier to capture DLMO in these patients. Even more ideal would be to sample melatonin comprehensively for at least 24 h to determine the daily pattern of secretion. To assist with feasibility of 24-h collection, urine sampling may be used instead of saliva samples (Benloucif et al.,
Future studies should also evaluate how potential circadian disruption interacts with other sleep health characteristics, including daytime sleepiness and sleep variability. In this study, we found some significant associations between sleep variability and valid estimation of DLMO, as well as DLMO timing. We measured sleep variability over 3–5 days, and this may have limited our capacity to fully examine these associations. Variability in sleep timing is a key sleep health construct and relates to other health outcomes (Meltzer et al.,
Our study findings suggest that a subset of children and adolescents with craniopharyngioma may have phase advance and that this relates to poorer prognostic indicators (e.g., hypothalamic involvement of the tumor). For those with valid DLMO estimates, timing correlated with sleepiness and BMI. Further study of circadian health in pediatric craniopharyngioma is needed to refine treatments and improve care for patients.
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by St. Jude IRB. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.
TM is the principal investigator for the prospective treatment protocol from which circadian data was collected. TM, MW, BM, and DH completed sleep and health assessments and collected all data for the project. Specific project conceptualization was completed by BM, VC, DK, and SC. SC provided circadian expertise and consultation. Data management and analysis was completed by DK, YL, HD, and JS. All authors contributed to manuscript development and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
This work was supported by the Cancer Center Support Grant (CA21765) from the National Cancer Institute and ALSAC.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.