Edited by: Isabel Mercader Rubio, University of Almería, Spain
Reviewed by: Ivana Zubic, Union Nikola Tesla University, Serbia
Namra Shahzadi, University of Gujrat, Pakistan
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
This study examines the predictive roles of personality traits, rational/irrational beliefs, and self-efficacy in academic performance, while also investigating how these factors interact with gender, residence, and school type.
Data were collected from 453 students at George Emil Palade University of Medicine, Pharmacy, Science, and Technology in Târgu Mureş using the Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire (ZKPQ), the General Self-Efficacy Scale (SES), and the short-form Attitudes and Beliefs Scale (ABSs).
Results revealed that institutional factors, particularly high school type, emerged as the strongest predictors of academic performance. Among the psychological traits, aggressiveness/hostility, impulsive sensation seeking, and rationality significantly predicted academic performance. Notably, impulsive sensation seeking was positively linked to higher performance in female but not male students, while aggressiveness/hostility predicted better performance only among students with high self-efficacy.
These findings highlight the potential for tailored intervention programs that take into account gender and personality differences to improve academic outcomes.
香京julia种子在线播放
Academic performance is a cornerstone of future professional success and overall life achievement (
The relationship between personality traits and educational success has been widely examined, particularly within the framework of the Big Five model (
Agreeableness has been linked to academic success, though its influence appears weaker (
Findings regarding neuroticism and extraversion are less consistent. While some studies report no significant association between neuroticism and academic performance (
How can these findings be explained? Some researchers propose that neurotic students experience heightened anxiety and diminished self-confidence, hindering their full engagement in learning (
The link between extraversion and academic performance appears most pronounced in primary education, where teacher-student relationships play a more central role in shaping outcomes (
While most research on personality and academic performance has employed the Big Five model, few studies have utilized the alternative five-factor model (AFFM) (
Studies applying the AFFM reveal notable associations between aggressiveness and academic performance (
The link between impulsive sensation-seeking and academic performance is nuanced. Although traits like organization and routine tolerance typically facilitate learning, qualities seemingly at odds with impulsive sensation-seeking (
Caution is warranted in generalizing these findings, as personality profiles vary across academic disciplines (
Social cognitive theory views personality as a dynamic system of intrapersonal factors that motivate and regulate behavior (
Meta-analytic research reveals only minor gender differences in academic self-efficacy (
Rational Emotive Behavior Theory (
This review highlights persistent gaps and contradictions in understanding how personality traits and related factors influence academic performance. Future research should employ more nuanced analyses examining not only individual traits but also their interactions. Particularly valuable would be investigations using the alternative five-factor model (AFFM), which offers a distinct perspective from the dominant Big Five framework, while also accounting for contextual variables like gender and educational background. Additionally, given Romania’s pronounced rural-urban educational disparities (
Objectives and hypotheses
General objective
This study aims to investigate the predictive role of personality traits in academic performance on the high school graduation examination, and to examine the potential moderating effects of self-efficacy and rational/irrational beliefs on these relationships.
Specific objectives
I. Confirmatory objectives
To assess the direct effects of personality traits, self-efficacy, and rational/irrational beliefs on graduation exam performance.
To evaluate whether self-efficacy and rational/irrational beliefs moderate the relationship between personality traits and academic performance.
II. Exploratory objectives
To investigate gender differences (male vs. female students) in the associations between psychological variables (personality, self-efficacy, rational/irrational beliefs) and exam performance.
To analyze how residential environment (urban vs. rural) interacts with psychological variables to influence academic outcomes.
Research hypotheses
H1: Personality traits, self-efficacy, and rational/irrational beliefs significantly predict performance on the high school graduation examination.
H2: The relationship between personality traits and academic performance is moderated by self-efficacy and rational/irrational beliefs.
The exploratory objectives do not include
Throughout the duration of this study, ethical protocols were followed, and participation in the research was voluntary. All participants were required to provide written informed consent, and the confidentiality and anonymity of their data were guaranteed. We used a cross-sectional research design and collected data from 453 students; 157 were boys, 295 were girls, and one participant did not specify their gender. The participants are first-year students from the Department for Teacher Training at the “G.E. Palade” University of Medicine, Pharmacy, Science, and Technology of Târgu Mureş, who have just graduated from high school. In Romania, the Departments of Teacher Education prepare students who simultaneously pursue their primary academic majors. Consequently, the sample includes students studying diverse scientific disciplines, including medicine, engineering, economics, law, history, and sports.
The study utilized the Zuckerman–Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire (ZKPQ), which was adapted for the Romanian population by
Impulsive sensation seeking: Consists of two facets: impulsivity and sensation seeking. Impulsivity refers to a lack of planning and a tendency to act quickly on impulse without consideration. Sensation-seeking describes a general desire for thrills or the willingness to take risks for excitement, a preference for unpredictable friends and situations, and a need for change and novelty (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.760).
Neuroticism-anxiety: Describes emotional upset, fearfulness, tension, worry, lack of self-confidence, sensitivity to criticism, and obsessive indecision (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.869).
Aggression-hostility: Depicts readiness to express verbal aggression, rude antisocial behavior, vengefulness, spitefulness, a quick temper, and impatience with others (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.632).
Sociability: Differentiated into two aspects: enjoying large social events, interacting with many people, having many friends, and intolerance for social isolation (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.784).
Activity: Consists of two facets: the need for general activity, impatience, and restlessness when there is nothing to do, and a preference for challenging and hard work, and an active and busy life (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.718).
The General Self-Efficacy Scale (SES) by
The Attitudes and Beliefs Scale short form (ABS-SV;
Academic results were measured by the general average obtained in the baccalaureate exam. The exam takes place at the end of the 12th grade. The results obtained were categorized into four performance categories.
The descriptive statistics and correlation coefficients are presented in
Means, standard deviations, and correlations.
Variable | M | SD | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 |
1. Academic performance | 3.040 | 1.001 | ||||||||
2. Self-efficacy | 3.382 | 6.526 | 0.067 | |||||||
3. Impulsive sensation seeking | 9.987 | 3.802 | −0.125 |
−0.021 | ||||||
4. Neuroticism-anxiety | 8.640 | 4.961 | 0.047 | −0.257 |
0.143 |
|||||
5. Aggressivity | 5.499 | 2.958 | −0.165 |
−0.091 | 0.316 |
0.354 |
||||
6. Activity | 9.075 | 3.304 | 0.006 | 0.165 |
0.119 |
−0.222 |
−0.093 |
|||
7. Sociability | 6.932 | 3.746 | −0.069 | 0.047 | 0.234 |
−0.197 |
0.067 | 0.216 |
||
8. Irrationality ABS | 9.318 | 3.567 | −0.047 | −0.133 |
0.066 | 0.320 |
0.232 |
−0.01 | −0.055 | |
9. Rationality ABS | 14.695 | 3.814 | 0.190 |
0.110 |
−0.083 | −0.013 | −0.096 |
0.027 | −0.035 | 0.255 |
Mean ± SD are used to represent mean and standard deviation, respectively. * indicates
Given the important role of student gender and residence highlighted in previous studies, the analysis also controlled for these variables. When gender and residence were included in the analysis, the total variance explained by the model increased to 8.1%. The rural versus urban residence of the students had a significant effect on academic performance (β = 0.12,
The next step of the analysis was to test for the hypothesized interactions. The first tested interaction was between neuroticism and gender. As expected, the change in R2 was significant when the equation also included the interaction term [R2-change = 0.013,
The interaction between neuroticism and gender on academic performance was qualified by a three-way interaction between these variables and participants’ irrationality [R2-change = 0.013,
Three-way interaction effect of neuroticism (low, medium, high), irrational beliefs (
Gender also interacted marginally with impulsive sensation seeking and rationality [R2-change = 0.01,
Three-way interaction effect of impulsive sensation seeking (low, medium, high), rational beliefs (
Finally, the effects of aggressivity on academic performance depended on self-efficacy [R2-change = 0.02,
Two-way interaction between aggressivity (
This study examined two hypotheses concerning predictors of academic performance, operationalized through baccalaureate exam results in a sample of 453 Romanian students. Using three validated instruments, we assessed the Alternative Five Factor Model personality traits, self-efficacy, and rational/irrational beliefs. Our findings partially supported Hypothesis 1 (H1), revealing significant negative associations between academic performance and both impulsive sensation seeking and aggression/hostility. These results align with prior research (
However, when controlling for high school type, a proxy for prior academic achievement in Romania’s tracked education system (
The finding that personality traits accounted for only a modest proportion of variance in performance compared to high school type aligns with meta-analytic evidence underscoring the strong predictive role of prior academic achievement (
Contrary to H1, our analysis found no significant associations between academic performance and the remaining personality factors (activity, neuroticism/anxiety, and sociability). These null findings align with
This discrepancy may reflect unique aspects of the Romanian educational context, where several factors could attenuate the predictive power of self-efficacy. First, the baccalaureate represents a high-stakes testing situation with significant consequences for university admission. This may create external pressures that override students’ self-efficacy beliefs. Second, this high-stakes context could further magnify the negative effects of test anxiety on performance (
Our second hypothesis (H2) proposed that rational/irrational beliefs and self-efficacy would moderate the relationship between personality traits and academic performance. The results partially support this hypothesis, revealing complex interaction patterns. Consistent with previous research (
The analyses further showed that the aggression/hostility trait showed differential effects depending on self-efficacy levels. At high self-efficacy, aggression/hostility was positively associated with performance, suggesting these students may channel aggressive tendencies motivationally (
In conclusion, our results support a dynamic conceptualization where traits manifest differently depending on cognitive/affective moderators, gender influences how personality-behavior relationships unfold, and high self-efficacy may help “reframe” typically negative traits (like aggression) into performance-enhancing factors.
While this research offers valuable insights into personality-academic performance relationships, its findings are constrained by several limitations. The study’s reliance on self-report measures for assessing personality traits introduces potential response biases, including social desirability effects and subjective interpretations of questionnaire items, which may not fully capture the complexity of these psychological constructs. Furthermore, personality is a complex construct that cannot be fully captured through an evaluation tool such as the one used in the current study. Finally, the exclusive use of students from a single university limits the generalizability of our results to broader student populations. The non-representative nature of our sample suggests these findings should be complemented by future research employing more diverse, representative samples.
This study investigated the predictive relationships between personality traits (alternative five-factor model), rational beliefs, self-efficacy, and academic performance. Our findings revealed several key insights:
Initial analyses showed negative associations between academic performance and both impulsive sensation-seeking and aggression/hostility. However, after controlling for high school type (likely a proxy for prior academic achievement), gender, and residential environment, only rational beliefs maintained a significant direct relationship with performance. The strongest predictors emerged as institutional factors, particularly high school type, rather than individual psychological traits.
The study uncovered important interaction effects that qualify these relationships. The beneficial effect of rational beliefs on academic performance varied significantly by both gender and levels of impulsive sensation-seeking. Similarly, the impact of aggression/hostility depended crucially on students’ self-efficacy levels, demonstrating how motivational factors can modulate trait expression.
These results suggest three lines of action. First, the development of targeted personalized intervention programs that account for gender differences in how rational beliefs and personality traits influence learning outcomes. Second, training programs to enhance students’ academic self-efficacy, particularly for those displaying aggression/hostility traits, to help channel these characteristics productively. And third, greater support for students from rural areas, who demonstrated systematically lower performance despite comparable trait profiles.
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
This is an observational study, Research Ethics Committee has confirmed that no ethical approval is required. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
LC: Formal Analysis, Writing – review and editing, Software, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Data curation, Project administration, Conceptualization, Validation, Investigation, Supervision. VC: Software, Writing – review and editing, Writing – original draft, Data curation, Validation, Formal Analysis, Methodology.
The authors declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The authors declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.