Front. Psychol. Frontiers in Psychology Front. Psychol. 1664-1078 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1182749 Psychology Original Research Effects of smartphone addiction on cognitive function and physical activity in middle-school children: a cross-sectional study Al-Amri Ahlam 1 2 Abdulaziz Sahar 1 Bashir Shahid 3 Ahsan Mohammad 1 Abualait Turki 1 * 1Department of Physical Therapy, College of Applied Medical Sciences, Imam Abdulrahman Bin Faisal University, Dammam, Saudi Arabia 2Rehabilitation Health Services, Armed Forces Hospital Southern Region, Abha, Saudi Arabia 3Neuroscience Center, King Fahad Specialist Hospital, Dammam, Saudi Arabia

Edited by: Zhaojun Teng, Southwest University, China

Reviewed by: Seyed Sepehr Hashemian, University of Sherbrooke, Canada; Zhiyi Chen, Army Medical University, China

*Correspondence: Turki Abualait tsabualait@iau.edu.sa
14 08 2023 2023 14 1182749 09 03 2023 24 07 2023 Copyright © 2023 Al-Amri, Abdulaziz, Bashir, Ahsan and Abualait. 2023 Al-Amri, Abdulaziz, Bashir, Ahsan and Abualait

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Introduction

This study aimed to investigate the effects of smartphone addiction on cognitive function and physical activity in middle-school children.

Methods

A population of 196 children (boys and girls) from middle schools were recruited for this study with an average age of 12.99 ± 0.81 years, a height of 153.86 ± 6.50 meters, a weight of 48.07 ± 7.31 kilograms, and a body mass index of 20.22 ± 2.08 kg/m2. Smartphone addiction was determined using Arabic versions of the Smartphone Addiction Scale-Short Version, and physical activity levels were assessed by a physical activity questionnaire for older children. The working memory and selective attention domains of cognitive function were evaluated using a laptop screen's digital version of the memory automaticity and Flanker tasks, respectively. A one-way MANOVA was conducted to determine the differences in working memory between the smartphone-addicted and non-addicted groups. The relationship between smartphone addiction and physical activity was analyzed using Pearson's chi-squared test.

Results

The cognitive function-attention domain accuracy component showed a statistically significant difference between the groups, with a p-value of 0.05). The reaction time between smartphone-addicted and non-addicted children showed no statistically significant difference (p = 0.817). The relationship between smartphone addiction and physical activity was statistically significant (p < 0.001).

Discussion

The interaction effects between physical activity and smartphone addiction on reaction times showed statistically insignificant (p = 0.25) differences, showing that physical activity's effect on reaction times did not depend on smartphone addiction levels. The non-addicted children had significantly higher physical activity levels than the addicted children, indicating that smartphone addiction reduced physical activity.

smartphone addiction children cognitive function physical activity memory attention section-at-acceptance Developmental Psychology

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      Due to the rapid expansion of the Internet and other technological breakthroughs, it is anticipated that the number of mobile phone users will continue to increase annually. Smartphones are considered the most prevalent electronic device among children. A study discovered that, compared to tablets and laptops, smartphones were the most commonly used gadgets among children, with a mean weekly usage of 28.5 h (Alobaid et al., 2018). This could be considered a strong indicator of children's rapid exposure to smartphone usage. Children currently rely on smartphones for many things, including attending online classes, communicating, playing games, shopping, and watching entertaining videos. Children also use smartphones instead of books for reading and studying (Nasution, 2021). Smartphones can be used to access educational resources, stay connected with friends and family, and learn new skills. This way of life significantly impacts children's day-to-day activities, and they become too attached to their smartphones, which might lead to addiction (Nasution, 2021).

      Smartphones are characterized by rapid technological development and increasing prevalence due to their flexibility in function, portability, and purpose of use. There have been numerous unresolved queries concerning the effects of smartphone addiction on cognitive functions. Moreover, the conclusive evidence is still limited and conflicted, especially among children (Wilmer et al., 2017). Studies found that students exposed to smartphones or receiving more mobile phone calls or text messages showed shorter response times and were less accurate on working memory tasks (Abramson et al., 2009; Thomas et al., 2010). In contrast, Wasmuth et al. (2022). did not find a relation between general smartphone use (time/frequency) and inattention (Wasmuth et al., 2022). Neurophysiological studies report that heavy smartphone use is associated with attention, number processing, and right prefrontal cortex excitability impairments. However, there were no significant differences in working memory or inhibitory control (Hadar et al., 2017). While there is no conclusive evidence that smartphones harm a child's cognitive function, some studies have produced alarming results. Paulus et al. investigated the association between screen media activity behavior, brain structure, and cognitive function changes. They found a significant association between changes in the structural characteristics of the brain and time spent on screens, including smartphones. They also found that some activities related to screening media and brain structures are associated with worse cognitive performance, while others are associated with better cognitive performance. It suggests that screen media activity is not “good or bad for the brain” (Paulus et al., 2019). More research is required to determine whether or not the use of electronic devices impacts cognitive function, particularly by children.

      Physical activity and smartphone addiction are regarded as two health-related independent variables, yet they are interconnected (Wu et al., 2017; Li et al., 2020). The prolonged use of smartphones can affect physical health by reducing participation in physical activities, which leads to a decrease in muscle mass and an increase in fat mass, both of which are associated with bad health consequences (Kim et al., 2015). A recent review of the association between smartphone addiction and participation in sports and physical activity among children and adolescents revealed that an increase in smartphone addiction decreases physical activity and sports performance (Azam et al., 2020). Although a few studies indicate no significant association between smartphone addiction and physical activities, this is not the case in general (Buctot et al., 2020). Numerous studies have shown that the prolonged use of smartphones is highly correlated with sedentary lifestyles and physical inactivity (Fennell et al., 2019; Xiang et al., 2020).

      Physical activity can raise dopamine levels and receptor binding rates in the human body, which helps reduce addictive behaviors (Roberts et al., 2012). Fewer smartphone users would be extremely likely to experience better cognitive functions in their daily lives (Hadlington, 2015). According to a cross-sectional study, physical activity is directly connected with enhanced cognitive function (Hamer and Chida, 2008). A systematic review described that regular physical activity has the most protective effect against cognitive decline (Blondell et al., 2014). Therefore, this raises questions regarding the potential effects of smartphone addiction on cognitive function and physical activity in children. However, no systematic research has examined the effects of smartphone addiction on cognitive function and physical activity in Saudi Arabian children aged 12–14 years. This study aimed to assess the effect of smartphone addiction on cognitive function and physical activity among middle-school children. Another aim of the study was to compare the cognitive function and physical activity of addicted and non-addicted middle-school children.

      Materials and methods Study design and setting

      A cross-sectional study with an analytic and descriptive structure was adopted to conduct this study. The research was conducted between December 2021 and February 2022 at eight public and private middle schools for boys and girls in the Eastern Province of Saudi Arabia (Dammam, Al Khobar, and Dhahran).

      Sample size

      The sample size was calculated based on the Raosoft online calculator at (http://www.raosoft.com/samplesize.html). The confidence level was 95%, and the significance level was 5%, with an expected prevalence of 87% of mobile phone usage among children and adolescents in Saudi Arabia (DOCOMO GN, 2011). The recommended sample size is 173. With a 10% dropout rate, the estimated total sample required for the current study was 200 children.

      Ethics approval

      Ethics approval was obtained from the Institute Review Board of the University of Imam Abdulrahman Bin Faisal (IRB-PCS-2021-03-369). Each participant signs a written informed consent form prior to participating in the study.

      Selection of schools and participants

      The study samples were drawn from eight schools chosen randomly from a list of schools using a lottery method. Following contact with the selected schools to conduct the study, a random selection of children from these schools was also made using random number generator software (https://www.random.org/). All selected children were healthy school-going children of Saudi nationality and both sexes, with an age range of 12–14 years in the Gregorian calendar. Children with anemia, diabetes mellitus, hypertension, obesity, asthma, or seizures, a history of vision or hearing problems, anxiety, attention deficits, sleep disorders, a history of smoking, neuromuscular disorders, and physical disabilities and those who are unable to pay attention to the researcher's instructions or read and understand the questionnaires and testing procedures were excluded from the study. Based on their SAS-SV scores, the children were categorized into the smartphone-addicted and non-addicted groups.

      Procedure

      The researcher went to the selected schools and explained the study process to the principals. The principals gave their written informed consent once they were informed about the research process and agreed to the data being collected at their schools. Afterward, children were randomly selected. All participants were recruited from intermediate-school grades 7, 8, and 9, with equal numbers of students from each grade to control confounding factors. Then, the research process was explained to them in detail. Once they agreed, they signed the consent form and participated in the study. Finally, the parents gave their consent through the WhatsApp application. With the help of the school principal, the researcher was provided with a copy of the student's medical history report, and all participants were assessed for eligibility. Out of 200 children, 196 met the criteria for inclusion. The researcher then started collecting their demographic information, such as their gender, age, level of education, height, weight, and body mass index (BMI). Smartphone-related information was collected, such as the duration of smartphone use per day and the number of years the children owned the smartphones. These details were recorded for each child on a separate data entry sheet. The height was measured (in meters) using a measuring tape, and the digital weighing scale was used to measure the weight (in kilograms). The body mass index (BMI) was calculated by dividing weight in kilograms by height in meters squared. After obtaining the demographic data, each child participated in three evaluations: the first evaluation was for assessing smartphone addiction levels, the second for evaluating cognitive function, and the third for measuring physical activity levels. The smartphone addiction and physical activity levels were assessed by self-assessment paper-based questionnaires using the Arabic versions of the Smartphone Addiction Scale-Short Version and the Physical Activity Questionnaire for older children, respectively. The working memory and selective attention domains of cognitive function were evaluated using a laptop screen's digital version of the memory automaticity and Flanker tasks, respectively. During the test procedure, the children were individually seated in a quiet room with the investigator in front of a laptop screen. The investigator explained the tasks to the children, and once they were ready and understood the task's procedure, they started the actual tasks.

      The memory automaticity task was used to assess working memory. This task requires remembering whether or not a letter is in a memory set and classifying it accordingly. A memory set is a set of learned alphabets designed to be recognized on a given trial. Letters that match any of the memorized items are called “targets,” while letters that do not match any of the memorized items are called “distractors.” Response times and accuracy are the dependent metrics. This task has two main components: consistent and varied mapping. Consistent mapping is used when the target and distractor items do not overlap across trials; they are mapped consistently; thus, this task is performed automatically and requires less focus and attention (Schneider and Shiffrin, 1977; Servant et al., 2018; Zhao et al., 2022). Varied mapping is used if the target and distractor items overlap each other. Hence, completing the test requires much more control, focus, and attention than consistent mapping (Schneider and Shiffrin, 1977; Zhao et al., 2022). First, the children were given a memory set of one to four alphabets at two different levels. They were asked to remember one to two alphabets in the first level, while the second level has three to four alphabets. This list of levels came randomly and could be consistent or varied (Figure 1). Once the participants memorized the alphabet in a memory set, they had to press the spacebar to begin; thus, the previous memory set would be deleted, and then, they were shown a series of alphabet inputs. They must decide whether each letter matches one of the alphabets in the memory set previously presented or not. When the letters do not match, they are considered distractors, and the participant responds using the left shift key. Alternatively, consider it a target when it matches and responds using the right shift key.

      Illustration of the memory automaticity task for the assessment of working memory.

      The Flanker task assesses selective attention control by focusing on a stimulus while simultaneously inhibiting the detection of other stimuli (Servant et al., 2018). This task presented five randomly directed arrows in the screen's center. The participants were asked to determine the direction of the center arrow while ignoring the arrows in the periphery. Both hands' index and middle fingers were placed on the keyboard's right and left shift keys. The children were instructed to respond depending on the direction of the central arrow; if it points left, they should press the left shift key; if it points right, they should press the right shift key as fast as possible. The heads of the arrows surrounding the center arrow would either be in the same or the opposite direction, be absent, or only appear as lines (Figure 2). Before starting the main trials of the task, which had 96 trials, the children were allowed to practice for 10 trials. The overall mean reaction times in milliseconds and the mean accuracy were recorded for analysis. Data from the practice trials were excluded. The participants had approximately 3 min to complete the test. After obtaining all the required data, the children were categorized into the smartphone-addicted and non-addicted groups based on their SAS-SV scores. The scores of cognitive function and physical activity level were collected for further analysis.

      Illustration of the Flanker task for the assessment of selective attention.

      Outcome measurements Smartphone addiction

      Smartphone addiction levels were measured using the Arabic version of the Smartphone Addiction Scale-Short Version (SAS-SV) (Kwon et al., 2013a). It is a self-administered scale developed by Kwon et al. (2013a) and intended to assess smartphone addiction. The SAS-SV had 10 items assessed on a 6-point Likert scale, ranging from “one” for strongly disagree to “six” for strongly agree. The maximum score for the scale is 60, and the minimum score possible is 10 (Kwon et al., 2013a). The SAS-SV addresses five content areas: daily disturbances, tolerance, cyberspace-oriented relationships, overuse, and withdrawal (Haug et al., 2015). The original English scale version had excellent internal consistency, content, and concurrent validity (Kwon et al., 2013b). However, Sfendla et al. (2018) assessed the psychometric properties of the Arabic SAS-SV on the Moroccan sample and found excellent reliability (Sfendla et al., 2018). The Smartphone Addiction Scale-Short Version classified the users as addicts with scores of ≥31 for boys and ≥33 for girls or non-addicts with scores of < 31 for boys and < 33 for girls. This cutoff point is based on the original article that examined the validity and reliability of the SAS-SV questionnaire (Kwon et al., 2013a).

      Cognitive function

      The working memory and selective attention domains of cognitive function were assessed using the psychology experiment building language (PEBL) test battery. The PEBL is an open-source software system that is freely available for designing and conducting psychological experiments and is a versatile research tool for studying individual differences in neurocognitive performance (Piper et al., 2015). It has good reliability and validity (Piper et al., 2015). The battery can be freely downloaded from the website (http://pebl.sourceforge.net). However, under the PEBL battery, memory automaticity and Flanker tasks were employed to assess the working memory and selective attention domains, respectively.

      Physical activity

      The Arabic version of the physical activity questionnaire for older children was used to assess the children's physical activity. The PAQ-C is a self-administered scale designed to assess the children's physical activity in the last 7 days (Kowalski et al., 1997). The original scale has good reliability and validity and was invented to assess physical activity in children aged 8–14 (Benítez-Porres et al., 2016; Gobbi et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2016). In a recent study, the scale was translated into Arabic and proved excellent validity and reliability (Alharbi, 2019). The total number of items in the PAQ-C was nine. Each item on the scale was rated from 1 to 5, and the total score was the average of all the items (Kowalski et al., 1997). The total score ranges from 1 to 5, divided into a low physical activity score of ≤ 2.3, a moderate physical activity score of 2.4–3.7, and a high physical activity score of ≥ 3.8 (Alharbi, 2019).

      Statistics analysis

      Data were transferred to a single Excel sheet, and all variables were analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences software for Mac (IBM SPSS version: 28.0.1.0, New York, USA). The univariate analysis for the demographic characteristics and outcome measures was done using descriptive statistics. Descriptive statistics were reported as mean ± standard deviation for quantitative variables. Categorical variables were reported as frequencies and percentages. The normality of the variables' distribution was examined using the Shapiro–Wilk test. A one-way MANOVA was conducted to determine the differences in working memory between the smartphone-addicted and non-addicted groups. To illustrate smartphone addiction's effect on selective attention, the reaction times and accuracy means were compared between the two groups using an independent sample t-test. The relationship between smartphone addiction and physical activity was analyzed using the Pearson chi-squared test. The effect size was determined using Cohen's formula, which represents the average effect size as also d = 0.4, with 0.2, 0.4, and 0.6 considered small, medium, and large effects, respectively. The interaction effects between physical activity and smartphone addiction on reaction times were analyzed using a 2 × 3 factorial design. The criterion for statistical significance was set at a p-value of ≤ 0.05.

      Results

      Based on their smartphone addiction scale-short version scores, the children were categorized into two groups: smartphone-addicted and non-addicted. Moreover, approximately half (49.5%) of the children used their phones for more than 5 h a day, and approximately two-thirds used mobile phones for 2–4 years (66.3%). The mean ± (SD) of age, height, weight, and body mass index of the included participants was 12.99 ± 0.81, 153.86 ± 6.50, 48.07 ± 7.31, and 20.22 ± 2.08, respectively (Table 1).

      Demographic characteristics of total participants and differences between the smartphone-addicted and non-addicted children.

      Mean, standard deviation (SD) and frequency distribution of children' characteristics Addicted (n = 100) Non-addicted (n = 96) Total (n = 196)
      Mean (SD)
      Age in years 13.11 (0.82) 12.88 (0.78) 12.99 ± 0.81
      Height in meters 154.56 (6.40) 153.13 (6.55) 153.86 ± 6.50
      Weight in kilograms 48.80 (6.41) 47.31 (8.10) 48.07 ± 7.31
      Body mass index kg/(m)2 20.38 (1.93) 20.05 (2.22) 20.22 ± 2.08
      Frequency (Percent)
      Gender
      Male 44 (44 %) 54 (56.3 %) 98 (50 %)
      Female 56 (56 %) 42 (43.8 %) 98 (50 %)
      Education level
      Middle-school grade 1 29 (29 %) 37 (38.5 %) 66 (33.7 %)
      Middle-school grade 2 30 (30 %) 34 (35.4 %) 64 (32.7 %)
      Middle-school grade 3 41 (41 %) 25 (26 %) 66 (33.7 %)
      Daily smartphone usage time
      < 1 h per day 0 (0.0 %) 22 (22.9 %) 22 (11.2 %)
      < 4 h per day 9 (9 %) 68 (70.8 %) 77 (39.3 %)
      More than 5 h per day 91 (91 %) 6 (6.3 %) 97 (49.5 %)
      Years of smartphone ownership
      1 year and less 14 (14 %) 28 (29.2 %) 42 (21.4 %)
      2–4 years 69 (69 %) 61 (63.5 %) 130 (66.3 %)
      More than 5 years 17 (17 %) 7 (7.3 %) 24 (12.2 %)

      The difference between the addicted and non-addicted children in response times for both varied, and consistent components was not statistically significant; F(4, 191) = 1.154, p = 0.333; Wilks' Λ = 0.976; partial η2 = 0.024. Furthermore, the difference between the addicted and non-addicted children on accuracy for both varied and consistent components was not statistically significant; F(4, 191) = 0.968, p = 0.426; Wilks' Λ = 0.980; partial η2 = 0.020. The mean response times and accuracy differences between smartphone-addicted and non-addicted children showed that the addicted children had shorter response times and were more accurate than the non-addicted children (Figures 3, 4).

      Differences between smartphone-addicted and non-addicted children in response time for varied and consistent components at levels 1 and 2.

      Differences in accuracy between smartphone-addicted and non-addicted children for varied and consistent components at levels 1 and 2.

      Table 2 shows that the independent sample t-test has shown no statistically significant difference in reaction times between smartphone-addicted and non-addicted children (t = 0.464, p = 0.817). However, the accuracy component showed a statistically significant difference between the groups (t = 2.617, p = 0.005). The mean accuracy shows that smartphone-addicted children have a higher accuracy rate than non-addicted children. The small effect (d = 0.066) was shown between smartphone-addicted and non-addicted children for reaction times, whereas a medium effect (d = 0.374) size was shown for accuracy.

      Reaction times and accuracy differences between smartphone-addicted and non-addicted children.

      Children's groups Mean Standard deviation 95% CI t p-value Effect size (d)
      Lower Upper
      Reaction times (RT) Addicted 465.73 55.45 −12.138 19.611 0.464 0.817 0.066
      Non-Addicted 461.99 57.23 −12.149 19.622
      Accuracy % Addicted 91.88 6.72 0.744 5.300 2.617 0.005* 0.374
      Non-Addicted 88.86 9.29 0.727 5.316

      *Significant at 0.05 level.

      Two-way cross-tabulation shows that smartphone-addicted children had lower levels of physical activity. In contrast, non-addicted children had moderate-to-high levels of physical activity. In addition, the Pearson chi-square test showed that this relationship was statistically significant (X2 = 84.60, p < 0.001). A large effect size (phi = 0.657) can be observed between addicted and non-addicted children for physical activity levels (Table 3).

      Relationship between smartphone addiction and physical activity.

      Physical activity levels (number) Pearson chi-square (X2) p-value Effect size (phi)
      Low Moderate High
      Children's groups Addicted (n = 100) 61 39 0 84.60 < 0.001* 0.657
      Non-addicted (n = 96) 10 38 48

      *Significant at 0.05 level.

      Table 4 shows no statistically significant (p = 0.250) differences among smartphone-addicted and non-addicted children for low and moderate-to-high physical activity subgroups. Partial eta squared (η2 = 0.007) showed very little effect between addicted and non-addicted children for the level of physical activity.

      Interaction effects between physical activity and smartphone addiction on reaction times.

      Children's groups Physical activity levels Mean SD 95% CI F p-value Partial eta squared
      Lower Upper
      Addicted Low 457.41 55.025 443.211 471.607 1.330 0.250 0.007
      Moderate 478.74 54.281 460.988 496.501
      Non-addicted Low 469.23 52.791 434.158 504.292
      Moderate 463.96 51.576 445.973 481.951
      High 458.93 63.001 442.923 474.934
      Discussion

      The current study aimed to investigate the effects of smartphone addiction on cognitive function and physical activity in middle-school children. According to the present study, there are no significant differences in working memory or reaction times for selective attention tasks between smartphone-addicted and non-addicted children. A significant difference was observed only in the accuracy component of the selective attention task, indicating that the smartphone-addicted children were more accurate than the non-addicted children. Concerning the differences in physical activity between smartphone-addicted and non-addicted children, the present study's results indicate that non-addicted children were significantly more active than smartphone-addicted children. Concerning the interaction effects between smartphone addiction and physical activity, the results have shown no significant interaction effects between physical activity and smartphone addiction on reaction times.

      In the present study, mean response time and accuracy values indicated that the smartphone-addicted children performed the working memory task slightly better than the non-addicted children at each level. The smartphone-addicted children had shorter mean response times and were more accurate than the non-addicted children for varied and consistent mapping components. These findings suggest that smartphone-addicted children might remember the selected target faster and more accurately than non-addicted children might, which contradicts the current study's hypothesis. Most of the children in this study had been repetitively using their smartphones for more than 5 h per day for 2–4 years. Therefore, considering that smartphone overuse could enhance the user's sensory-motor coordination, decrease their response time, and increase their accuracy (Grewal and Sahni, 2019; Jordan and Dhamala, 2022). Moreover, the current study's working memory assessment depends on a computer-based task. As a result, smartphone-addicted children could complete tasks better and more efficiently than non-addicted children as they are more familiar with using such devices than non-addicted children.

      Furthermore, working memory performance may improve with practice because of the brain's plasticity (Jak, 2012; Choudhury and McKinney, 2013). Imren and Tekman found that media multitasking improves working memory but inhibits the ability to sustain attention. They hypothesized that these results might have occurred because multitasking requires working memory practice, and media multitasking involves switching between devices or their functions (Imren and Tekman, 2019). Therefore, working memory performance can increase with practice, improving cognitive function (Jak, 2012; Choudhury and McKinney, 2013; Loh and Kanai, 2016). In addition, Tanaka et al. (2013) indicated that smartphone addicts had greater gray matter volume in the posterior parietal cortex, which was associated with better visual working memory performance (Tanaka et al., 2013). However, statistically, there are no significant differences in working memory between smartphone-addicted and non-addicted children. The lack of statistically significant differences in the memory (accuracy) task may be attributed to the limited differences between the addicted and non-addicted children with regard to cognitive functions, making it difficult to draw statistically significant differences. Studies have demonstrated that childhood and adolescence are characterized by the continuous development and maturation of various prefrontal cortex-mediated behaviors, including planning, attentional control, working memory, inhibitory control, and decision-making (Hooper et al., 2004; Conklin et al., 2007; Luciana et al., 2009).

      The attention domain of cognitive function assessed by the Flanker task is based on reaction times in milliseconds and accuracy. Regarding reaction times, the current study showed no statistically significant difference between smartphone-addicted and non-addicted children. In a recent neurophysiological study, researchers aimed to determine whether excessive smartphone use is accompanied by measurable neural, cognitive, and behavioral changes. They conducted a longitudinal experiment to identify smartphone use's effects on the participants' cognitive functions and to observe the differences between heavy smartphone users and non-users. They found that heavy smartphone users were experiencing hyperactivity and increased impulsivity. Moreover, heavy smartphone users had reduced early transcranial magnetic stimulation-evoked potentials induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation on the right side of the prefrontal cortex compared to non-smartphone users, which were associated with self-reported inattention problems. However, the researchers did not observe significant differences between the groups' memory domains (Hadar et al., 2017). Consistent with the study mentioned above, the current study found no significant differences in the memory domain of cognitive function between smartphone-addicted and non-addicted children. However, regarding the attention function, the current study's findings contradict the previous study's results. The current study's results showed no statistically significant difference between smartphone-addicted and non-addicted children in the reaction time of the attention domain. In contrast, the prior study found that heavy smartphone use was significantly associated with inattention problems. The differences in results could be attributed to the methodological differences in how the studies were conducted.

      The study mentioned above used the Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scales (CAARS) questionnaire to assess inattention, whereas the present study used a computer-based test. Paper-based questionnaire assessments are different from computer-based tests. In paper-based tests, all the questions are in front of the participant at once, allowing them to move between questions as they wish, which may increase the chance of bias and error. In computer-based tests, by contrast, questions are presented one after the other with limited time to complete the task; therefore, participants have no opportunity to return to previously posed questions, which may help provide more accurate results of the attention function. However, we currently lack evidence to support this intuitive interpretation, so we cannot completely exclude it.

      The above-described study's sample included only adults, whereas the current study's participants were children. The developmental stage of childhood is characterized by ongoing neurological growth, which distinguishes children from adults (Larsen and Luna, 2018). There is substantial evidence to indicate that children influence attentional performance. For instance, the capacity to sustain attention, inhibit inappropriate responses, and shift attentional focus improves throughout childhood (Halperin et al., 1991; Greenberg and Waldman, 1993). Therefore, it is difficult to definitively judge smartphone addiction's effect on children's attention functions. However, the difference in the effects of smartphone addiction on attention function between adults and children indicates the possibility of adverse long-term effects. These results sparked further curiosity of the current study's team to move research forward, conduct a longitudinal study, and include a wide range of age groups to uncover more results.

      The current study's findings have shown no statistically significant difference in reaction times between smartphone-addicted and non-addicted children. However, smartphone-addicted children had a significantly higher accuracy rate than non-addicted children in the attention task. One possible explanation for the smartphone-addicted children's high accuracy rate could be the experience and skills acquired by these children from using smartphones. Smartphone-addicted children have been using the smartphone, repetitively, for an extended period, which could have enhanced their neuronal circuits, thus increasing their ability to filter irrelevant information. In a series of studies by Dye et al. (2009), and Bavelier et al. (2012) increased media multitasking was associated with better attention control; they found that smartphone-addicted participants were better at inhibiting irrelevant information than other groups (Dye et al., 2009; Bavelier et al., 2012). In addition, a recent study found that smartphone-addicted participants were more attentive than their counterparts (Alsaad et al., 2022).

      Regarding smartphone addiction's effect on physical activity, the current study confirmed the link between smartphone addiction and low physical activity. The findings showed that smartphone-addicted children were less physically active. In contrast, non-addicted children are likelier to have moderate-to-high physical activity levels. Furthermore, this association was statistically significant. In agreement with the present study's results, Azam et al. (2020) conducted a systematic review to emphasize the links between smartphone addiction, sports participation, and physical activity. Their review included eight global studies, all of which had been conducted on children and adolescents. All the studies included in their review showed similar results, demonstrating that an increase in smartphone use leads to decreases in physical activity and sports performance among children and adolescents (Azam et al., 2020). Similarly, Wang et al. (2016) revealed that smartphone addiction decreases physical fitness among university students (Li et al., 2022).

      The present study's secondary objective was to identify the interaction effects between physical activity and smartphone addiction on reaction times. It could be considered that the effects of physical activity on reaction times depend on the levels of smartphone addiction. Therefore, smartphone-addicted and non-addicted children were divided into low and moderate-to-high physical activity subgroups based on the physical activity questionnaire. However, there were no significant interaction effects between physical activity and smartphone addiction on reaction times. These findings indicate that the effect of physical activity on reaction times did not depend on whether the children were addicted or non-addicted to smartphones. These results could be attributed to the fact that reaction times depend on many other factors, including gender (Naglieri and Rojahn, 2001), sleep quality (Paavonen et al., 2010), children's birth order, residence, breakfast intake, and the mother's smoking history (Almomani et al., 2014). However, the current study did not investigate such factors' effects on reaction times. Notably, studies have yet to examine the interaction effects between physical activity and smartphone addiction on reaction times.

      This study also has several limitations, which indicate possibilities for further investigation. First, the cross-sectional study design prevents any cause–effect relationship. Further longitudinal investigation is required to determine the directionality of the investigated correlations. Second, the sample was a specific age group, and all participants were recruited only from schools in the eastern region, which may affect the likelihood of generalizability. Third, there was no intervention used to determine the cause-and-effect assumptions. Fourth, self-reported questionnaires were used to determine the level of physical activity and smartphone addiction behavior that may lead to biases. Fifth, the study investigated smartphone addiction in only two cognitive domains: working memory and selective attention. Future researchers can investigate the impact of smartphone addiction on other neurocognitive domains, such as problem-solving and planning, helping to highlight the other cognitive domains that could be affected by smartphone addiction. Finally, smartphone addiction is complicated and multidimensional. Thus, examining the varied activities, contents, and patterns of smartphone use in future research would be beneficial.

      Conclusion

      The present study demonstrated that smartphone-addicted children were significantly more accurate than non-addicted children. Non-addicted children had significantly higher physical activity levels than addicted children. Smartphone-addicted children have shorter response times and are more accurate than non-addicted children in working memory tasks for varied and consistent mapping. In addition, the current study showed no significant interaction effects between physical activity and smartphone addiction on reaction times, indicating that the effect of physical activity on reaction times did not depend on smartphone addiction levels. Further studies are required to corroborate findings and aid in developing preventative and intervention measures.

      Data availability statement

      The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

      Ethics statement

      The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Imam Abdulrahman Bin Faisal University, Dammam. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

      Author contributions

      AA-A, SA, and TA: conceptualization. AA-A: data curation, investigation, and writing—original draft. SB: formal analysis and software. AA-A and TA: methodology. SB and MA: resources and writing—reviewing and editing. SA, MA, and TA: supervision. SA and TA: validation. SA and MA: visualization. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

      The authors appreciate the permission to perform this study from the Head of the Department of Physical Therapy, Dean of the College of Applied Medical Sciences, Imam Abdulrahman Bin Faisal University. Aside from that, the authors would like to thank every participant and PT laboratory staff member who participated directly or indirectly in this study.

      Conflict of interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Publisher's note

      All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

      References Abramson M. J. Benke G. P. Dimitriadis C. Inyang I. O. Sim M. R. Wolfe R. S. . (2009). Mobile telephone use is associated with changes in cognitive function in young adolescents. Bioelectromagnetics. 30, 678686. 10.1002/bem.2053419644978 Alharbi M. (2019). Influence of individual and family factors on physical activity among Saudi girls: a cross-sectional study. Ann. Saudi Med. 39, 1321. 10.5144/0256-4947.2019.1330712046 Almomani F. Josman N. Al-Momani M. O. Malkawi S. H. Nazzal M. Almahdawi K. A. . (2014). Factors related to cognitive function among elementary school children. Scand. J. Occup. Ther. 21, 191198. 10.3109/11038128.2013.85309824215566 Alobaid L. BinJadeed H. Alkhamis A. Alotaibi R. Tharkar S. Gosadi I. Ashry G. A. D. (2018). Burgeoning rise in smartphone usage among school children in Saudi Arabia: Baseline assessment of recognition and attention skills among users and non-users using CANTAB tests. Ulutas Med. J. 4, 411. 10.5455/umj.20180203121836 Alsaad F. Binkhamis L. Alsalman A. Alabdulqader N. Alamer M. Abualait T. . (2022). Impact of action video gaming behavior on attention, anxiety, and sleep among university students. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 15, 151160. 10.2147/PRBM.S34769435058721 Azam M. Ali A. Mattiullah J. Perveen N. (2020). Physical activity, sports participation, and smartphone addiction in adolescent students: a systematic review. J. Evi. Based Psychotherapies 20, 2541. 10.24193/jebp.2020.1.2 Bavelier D. Achtman R. L. Mani M. Föcker J. (2012). Neural bases of selective attention in action video game players. Vision Res. 61, 132143. 10.1016/j.visres.2011.08.00721864560 Benítez-Porres J. López-Fernández I. Raya J. F. Álvarez Carnero S. Alvero-Cruz J. R. Álvarez Carnero E. . (2016). Reliability and validity of the PAQ-C questionnaire to assess physical activity in children. J Sch Health. 86, 677685. 10.1111/josh.1241827492937 Blondell S. J. Hammersley-Mather R. Veerman J. L. (2014). Does physical activity prevent cognitive decline and dementia? A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. BMC Pub. Health. 27, 510. 10.1186/1471-2458-14-51024885250 Buctot D. B. Kim N. Kim S. H. (2020). The role of nomophobia and smartphone addiction in the lifestyle profiles of junior and senior high school students in the Philippines. Soc. Sci. Humanit. Open. 2, 100035. 10.1016/j.ssaho.2020.100035 Choudhury S. McKinney K. A. (2013). Digital media, the developing brain and the interpretive plasticity of neuroplasticity. Transcult. Psychiatry. 50, 192215. 10.1177/136346151247462323599391 Conklin H. M. Luciana M. Hooper C. J. Yarger R. S. (2007). Working memory performance in typically developing children and adolescents: behavioral evidence of protracted frontal lobe development. Dev. Neuropsychol. 31, 103128. 10.1207/s15326942dn3101_617305440 DOCOMO GN (2011). Children's use of Mobile Phones—An International Comparison 2011. GSM Association and Mobile Society Research Institute, Within NTT DOCOMO Inc, Japan. Available online at: https://www.gsma.com/publicpolicy/wp-content/uploads/2012/03/Childrensuseofmobilephones2011.pdf (accessed December 15, 2022). Dye M. W. Green C. S. Bavelier D. (2009). Increasing speed of processing with action video games. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 18, 321326. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01660.x20485453 Fennell C. Barkley J. E. Lepp A. (2019). The relationship between cell phone use, physical activity, and sedentary behavior in adults aged 18–80. Comput. Hum. Behav. 90, 539. 10.1016/j.chb.2018.08.044 Gobbi E. Elliot C. Varnier M. Carraro A. (2016). Psychometric properties of the physical activity questionnaire for older children in italy: testing the validity among a general and clinical pediatric population. PLoS ONE. 11, e0156354. 10.1371/journal.pone.015635427228050 Greenberg L. M. Waldman I. D. (1993). Developmental normative data on the test of variables of attention (T.O.V.A.). J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 34, 10191030. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1993.tb01105.x8408366 Grewal S. Sahni R. K. (2019). Effect of smartphone addiction on reaction time in geriatric population. J. Nov. Physio. Phyl. Rehab. 6, 005009. 10.17352/2455-5487.000062 Hadar A. Hadas I. Lazarovits A. Alyagon U. Eliraz D. Zangen A. . (2017). Answering the missed call: Initial exploration of cognitive and electrophysiological changes associated with smartphone use and abuse. PLoS ONE. 12, e0180094. 10.1371/journal.pone.018009428678870 Hadlington L. J. (2015). Cognitive failures in daily life: exploring the link with internet addiction and problematic mobile phone use. Comput. Hum. Behav. 51, 7581. 10.1016/j.chb.2015.04.036 Halperin J. M. Sharma V. Greenblatt E. Schwartz S. T. (1991). Assessment of the continuous performance test: reliability and validity in a nonreferred sample. Psycho. Asses. J. Cons0 Clin. Psych. 3, 603608. 10.1037/1040-3590.3.4.603 Hamer M. Chida Y. (2008). Physical activity and risk of neurodegenerative disease: a systematic review of prospective evidence. Psychol. Med. 39, 311. 10.1017/S003329170800368118570697 Haug S. Castro R. P. Kwon M. Filler A. Kowatsch T. Schaub M. P. . (2015). Smartphone use and smartphone addiction among young people in Switzerland. J. Behav. Addict. 4, 299307. 10.1556/2006.4.2015.03726690625 Hooper C. J. Luciana M. Conklin H. M. Yarger R. S. (2004). Adolescents' performance on the Iowa Gambling Task: implications for the development of decision making and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Dev. Psychol. 40, 11481158. 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.114815535763 Imren M. Tekman H. G. (2019). The relationship between media multitasking, working memory and sustained attention. Uludag Çoklu Medya Görevi Ile Çalişma Bellegi ve Sürekli Dikkati Ilişkisi. 20, 10751100. 10.21550/sosbilder.487649 Jak A. J. (2012). The impact of physical and mental activity on cognitive aging. Curr. Top Behav. Neurosci. 10, 27391. 10.1007/7854_2011_14121818703 Jordan T. Dhamala M. (2022). Video game players have improved decision-making abilities and enhanced brain activities. Neuroimage Rep. 2, 100112. 10.1016/j.ynirp.2022.100112 Kim S. E. Kim J. W. Jee Y. S. (2015). Relationship between smartphone addiction and physical activity in Chinese international students in Korea. J. Behav. Addict. 4, 200205. 10.1556/2006.4.2015.02826551911 Kowalski K. C. Crocker P. R. E. Faulkner R. A. (1997). Validation of the physical activity questionnaire for older children. Pedia Exer Sci. 9, 17486. 10.1123/pes.9.2.174 Kwon M. Kim D. J. Cho H. Yang S. (2013a). The smartphone addiction scale: development and validation of a short version for adolescents. PLoS ONE. 8, e83558. 10.1371/journal.pone.008355832918427 Kwon M. Lee J. Y. Won W. Y. Park J. W. Min J. A. Hahn C. . (2013b). Development and validation of a smartphone addiction scale (SAS). PLoS ONE. 8, e56936. 10.1371/journal.pone.005693623468893 Larsen B. Luna B. (2018). Adolescence as a neurobiological critical period for the development of higher-order cognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 94, 179195. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.09.00530201220 Li L. Zhang J. Cao M. Hu W. Zhou T. Huang T. . (2020). The effects of chronic physical activity interventions on executive functions in children aged 3-7 years: a meta-analysis. J. Sci. Med. Sport. 23, 949954. 10.1016/j.jsams.2020.03.00732360243 Li W. Cui Y. Gong Q. Zhu Z. (2022). Association of smartphone use duration with physical fitness among university students: focus on strength and flexibility. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 19, 7386. 10.3390/ijerph1912738635742637 Loh K. K. Kanai R. (2016). How has the internet reshaped human cognition? Neuroscientist. 22, 506520. 10.1177/107385841559500526170005 Luciana M. Collins P. F. Olson E. A. Schissel A. M. (2009). Tower of London performance in healthy adolescents: the development of planning skills and associations with self-reported inattention and impulsivity. Dev. Neuropsychol. 34, 461475. 10.1080/8756564090296454020183711 Naglieri J. A. Rojahn J. (2001). Gender differences in planning, attention, simultaneous, and successive (PASS) cognitive processes and achievement. J. Edu. Psycho. 93, 430437. 10.1037/0022-0663.93.2.430 Nasution M. (2021). Factors affecting smartphone addiction in children. Proc. Int. Sem. Isl. Stu. 2, 108115. Paavonen E. J. Räikkönen K. Pesonen A. K. Lahti J. Komsi N. Heinonen K. . (2010). Sleep quality and cognitive performance in 8-year-old children. Sleep Med. 11, 386392. 10.1016/j.sleep.2009.09.00920202902 Paulus M. P. Squeglia L. M. Bagot K. Jacobus J. Kuplicki R. Breslin F. J. . (2019). Screen media activity and brain structure in youth: Evidence for diverse structural correlation networks from the ABCD study. Neuroimage. 185, 140153. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.10.04030339913 Piper B. J. Mueller S. T. Geerken A. R. Dixon K. L. Kroliczak G. Olsen R. H. . (2015). Reliability and validity of neurobehavioral function on the psychology experimental building language test battery in young adults. PeerJ. 3, e1460. 10.7717/peerj.146026713233 Roberts V. Maddison R. Simpson C. Bullen C. Prapavessis H. (2012). The acute effects of exercise on cigarette cravings, withdrawal symptoms, affect, and smoking behaviour: systematic review update and meta-analysis. Psychopharmacology. 222, 115. 10.1007/s00213-012-2731-z22585034 Schneider W. Shiffrin R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: i. detection, search, and attention. Psychol. Revi. 84, 166. 10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.1 Servant M. Cassey P. Woodman G. F. Logan G. D. (2018). Neural bases of automaticity. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn Mem. Cogn. 44, 440464. 10.1037/xlm000045428933906 Sfendla A. Laita M. Nejjar B. Souirti Z. Touhami A. A. O. Senhaji M. . (2018). Reliability of the arabic smartphone addiction scale and smartphone addiction scale-short version in two different moroccan samples. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 21, 325332. 10.1089/cyber.2017.041129762065 Tanaka S. Ikeda H. Kasahara K. Kato R. Tsubomi H. Sugawara S. K. . (2013). Larger right posterior parietal volume in action video game experts: a behavioral and voxel-based morphometry (VBM) study. PLoS ONE. 8, e66998. 10.1371/journal.pone.006699823776706 Thomas S. Benke G. Dimitriadis C. Inyang I. Sim M. R. Wolfe R. . (2010). Use of mobile phones and changes in cognitive function in adolescents. Occup. Environ. Med. 67, 861866. 10.1136/oem.2009.05408020798018 Wang J. J. Baranowski T. Lau W. P. Chen T. A. Pitkethly A. J. (2016). Validation of the physical activity questionnaire for older children (PAQ-C) among chinese children. Biomed Environ Sci. 29, 177186. 10.3967/bes2016.02227109128 Wasmuth J. M. Reinhard I. Hill H. Alpers G. W. Shevchenko Y. Kiefer F. . (2022). Smarter way to use your smartphone: an intervention to limit smartphone-related distractions reduces hyperactivity but not inattention symptoms. Eur. Addict. Res. 28, 255266. 10.1159/00052169335124666 Wilmer H. H. Sherman L. E. Chein J. M. (2017). Smartphones and cognition: a review of research exploring the links between mobile technology habits and cognitive functioning. Front. Psychol. 25, 605. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.0060528487665 Wu X. Y. Han L. H. Zhang J. H. Luo S. Hu J. W. Sun K. . (2017). The influence of physical activity, sedentary behavior on health-related quality of life among the general population of children and adolescents: a systematic review. PLoS ONE. 12, e0187668. 10.1371/journal.pone.018766829121640 Xiang M. Q. Lin L. Wang Z. R. Li J. Xu Z. B. Hu M. . (2020). Sedentary behavior and problematic smartphone use in Chinese adolescents: the moderating role of self-control. Front. Psychol. 10, 3032. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.0303232038398 Zhao Z. Zhao S. Wang Q. Zhang Y. Chen C. (2022). Effects of physical exercise on mobile phone addiction in college students: the chain mediation effect of psychological resilience and perceived stress. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 19, 15679. 10.3390/ijerph19231567936497752
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016www.hbcxwm.org.cn
      giiild.com.cn
      www.ipopay.com.cn
      www.nrchain.com.cn
      qg717.net.cn
      omfp.com.cn
      sousfb.com.cn
      www.udtw.com.cn
      perfdog.com.cn
      manytour.net.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p