Edited by: Rosario Cabello, University of Malaga, Spain
Reviewed by: Karolina Lutkiewicz, University of Gdansk, Poland; Darpan Kaur, Mahatma Gandhi Missions Medical College and Hospital, India
This article was submitted to Developmental Psychology, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
The transition to parenthood is a high-risk period for many parents and is an important period for child development. Research has identified that parental mental health, reflective functioning (capacity to consider mental states of oneself and others) and coparenting (capacity to work together well as a parenting team) may be particularly significant predictors of later child outcomes, however these factors have seldom been considered together. The present study therefore aimed to investigate the relationship between these factors and the extent to which they predict child social emotional development.
Three hundred and fifty parents of infants aged 0 to 3 years 11 months were recruited to complete an online Qualtrics questionnaire.
Results indicate that both positive coparenting and parental reflective functioning (Pre-mentalizing and Certainty subscales) were found to significantly predict child development. General reflective functioning (Uncertainty subscale) predicted parental depression and anxiety, however unexpectedly, parental mental health was not a significant predictor of child development, but did predict coparenting. General reflective functioning (Certainty subscale) was also found to predict coparenting, which in turn was found to predict parental reflective functioning. We found an indirect effect of general reflective functioning (Certainty) on child SE development via parental reflective functioning (Pre-mentalizing). We also found an indirect effect of negative coparenting on child development via parental reflective functioning (Pre-mentalizing).
The current results support a growing body of research highlighting the important role reflective functioning plays in child development and wellbeing as well as parental mental health and the interparental relationship.
香京julia种子在线播放
It is widely understood that the first 1,000 days of life—the period of development from conception to age two—is one of the most crucial periods of development for a child (
The transition to parenthood is accompanied by a series of novel and pre-existing stressors, and an increased demand on psychosocial resources that brings with it a greater risk of developing mental health difficulties for parents (
The importance of the parent infant relationship has been particularly emphasized within the field of attachment, with research consistently finding links between secure parent-infant attachment relationships and child outcomes such as positive mental health, social and emotional intelligence, physical health and enhanced cognitive capacity later in life (
Parental Reflective functioning (PRF) is a proposed mechanism through which these attachment relationships are transmitted from parent to child (
High levels of PRF is thought to be essential to children’s ability to regulate their emotions, and develop secure attachment relationships (
More recent research seeks to move beyond maternal–infant relationships to consider how the broader family system impacts a developing child. Family systems theory suggests that family-level processes influence child wellbeing over and above dyadic relationships within the family (i.e., the couple relationship, parent–child relationship and sibling relationships;
Positive coparenting is associated with a variety of child outcomes including cognitive development (
Coparenting has also been linked with parental mental health, with findings indicating that parental depression negatively impacts the coparenting relationship (
Recent research has hypothesized that coparenting may act as a mechanism through which anxiety is transmitted from parents to children, with study findings demonstrating correlations between parental anxiety and undermining coparenting, as well as between undermining coparenting and fearful temperaments in children (
While the examination of reflective functioning and coparenting together is growing, very few studies have gone a step further and examined how child outcomes fit within this picture. In their study,
It is of note that neither
To our knowledge,
The overall aim of the present study was to cross-sectionally investigate the variables involved in predicting child outcomes in early childhood, in particular, parental mental health, parental reflective functioning and coparenting and to examine how these variables are related to one another among parents. This is important to consider given the scarcity of research examining these variables together, particularly within a large sample of parents who have children in the period of early childhood. Given that the coparenting relationship emerges in early infancy, it is particularly worth examining how these variables interact in the first 4 years of the child’s life.
Informed by prior studies, we hypothesized that:
Poorer infant social emotional development will be predicted by higher levels of parental depression and anxiety, less positive and more negative coparenting and poorer general reflective functioning and parental reflective functioning. Poorer parental reflective functioning will be predicted by poorer general reflective functioning, less positive and more negative coparenting and increased symptoms of depression and anxiety. More negative and less positive coparenting will be predicted by poorer general reflective functioning and increased symptoms of depression and anxiety. Increased symptoms of depression and anxiety will be predicted by poorer general reflective functioning.
The present study implemented a cross-sectional, correlational research design to examine associations between parental mental health, parental reflective functioning, coparenting, and child social emotional development.
Participants were 350 parents (175 women, 175 men) with children aged 0 to 3 years 11 months who were recruited via Prolific, an online recruiting platform. Inclusion criteria were met if the participant had a child in the correct age range and was in a relationship with and living with the other parent of their child. Participants were paid £3.75 GBP (roughly $7.15 AUD) through the Prolific website after completion of the questionnaire.
Participants’ ages ranged from 19 to 61 years (
In order to detect a medium size effect using a mediation analysis, research suggests a sample size of at least 300 participants is needed (
The Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS;
The 4-item Couples Satisfaction Index (CSI;
The Coparenting Relationship Scale (CRS;
The 8-item Reflective Functioning Questionnaire (RFQ;
The Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire (PRFQ-18;
The Ages and Stages Questionnaire: Social–Emotional (ASQ: SE 6;
Ethics approval for the present study was granted by the Curtin University Human Research Ethics Committee (CUHREC). Following recruitment through Prolific, participants were redirected to a Qualtrics survey containing the study’s explanatory statement and all study measures. Participants then provided consent within Qualtrics before completing the online survey which took on average 30 min to complete.
Measures were preceded by several demographic questions (i.e., age, education level, and ethnicity and the final page of the survey provided a study debrief including links to support services). Participants were credited for their time upon valid completion of the survey.
Analyses were run using both SPSS (v.28) and R statistical software (
To address issues of non-linearity, square root transformations were conducted for the DASS Anxiety subscale, the CSI, the RFQ Uncertainty subscale, the PRFQ Pre-mentalizing subscale, and the ASQ prior to model testing. The bivariate correlations and descriptive statistics are provided in
The variables included in this sequential mediation model accounted for a statistically significant 18.7% of the variance in child social emotional development, equating to a small-sized effect. The total effect of reflective functioning (Uncertainty subscale only) on child social emotional development was statistically significant (
Despite statistically significant bivariate associations with children’s social–emotional development (
Descriptive statistics and correlations between measurement variables (
Correlations | Descriptives | |||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variables | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 |
|
|
|
|
1. Reflective functioning uncertainty | - |
|
|
|
|
- | - | - | - | - | - | 0.637 | 0.447 | 0.806 |
2. Reflective functioning certainty |
|
- |
|
|
|
- | - | - | - | - | - | 0.905 | 0.826 | 0.850 |
3. DASS depression | 0. |
−0. |
- |
|
|
- | - | - | - | - | - | 4.418 | 3.989 | 0.891 |
4. DASS Anxiety |
|
|
|
- |
|
- | - | - | - | - | - | 1.363 | 1.026 | 0.839 |
5. DASS stress |
|
|
|
|
- | - | - | - | - | - | - | 6.650 | 4.212 | 0.877 |
6. Parental reflective functioning pre-mentalizing |
|
|
|
|
|
- | - | - | - | - | - | 1.385 | 0.302 | 0.753 |
7. Parental reflective functioning certainty |
|
|
|
|
|
|
- | - | - | - | - | 4.002 | 1.057 | 0.783 |
8. Parental reflective functioning interest and curiosity | −0.057 | 0.087 | −0.010 | 0.052 | 0.036 |
|
|
- | - | - | - | 5.645 | 0.738 | 0.674 |
9. Positive coparenting |
|
|
|
|
|
|
0.085 |
|
- | - | - | 108.611 | 24.425 | 0.939 |
10. Negative coparenting |
|
|
|
|
|
|
−0.043 |
|
|
- | - | 11.304 | 10.331 | 0.890 |
11. Relationship satisfaction |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
- | 2.609 | 0.879 | 0.950 |
12. Child social emotional development |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
−0.097 |
|
0.026 | 1.273 | 0.493 | 0.336–0.912 |
Bivariate correlations are presented on the lower quadrant. **
Negative coparenting was also not a significant predictor. However, positive coparenting remained a significant predictor in the final model, albeit with a small effect size (
The Pre-mentalizing (
Predictors of child social emotional development, with 95% Bias corrected confidence intervals reported in parenthesis.
Variables |
|
Std. All |
|
|
---|---|---|---|---|
C1—Reflective functioning uncertainty | 0.131 (−0.053, 0.317) | 0.094 | 0.119 | 0.163 |
C2—Reflective functioning certainty | 0.000 (−0.089, 0.095) | 0.047 | 0.000 | 0.997 |
B1—Positive coparenting | 0.003 (0.000, 0.005) | 0.001 | 0.131 |
|
B2—Negative coparenting | 0.006 (−0.001, 0.012) | 0.003 | 0.115 | 0.086 |
B3—DASS anxiety | 0.058 (−0.015, 0.134) | 0.038 | 0.119 | 0.130 |
B4—DASS depression | 0.007 (−0.011, 0.025) | 0.009 | 0.055 | 0.464 |
B5—DASS stress | −0.009 (−0.029, 0.010) | 0.010 | −0.079 | 0.338 |
B6—Parental reflective functioning pre-mentalizing | 0.373 (0.136, 0.601) | 0.119 | 0.228 |
|
B7—Parental reflective functioning certainty | −0.057 (−0.109, −0.003) | 0.027 | −0.121 |
|
B8—Parental reflective functioning interest and curiosity | −0.044 (−0.124, 0.035) | 0.040 | −0.065 | 0.276 |
Total effect of RFQ uncertainty | 0.171 (0.002, 0.338) | 0.085 | 0.154 |
|
Total effect of RFQ certainty | −0.06 (−0.143, 0.029) | 0.044 | −0.101 | 0.172 |
R2 = 0.187. Confidence intervals and standard errors based on 10,000 Bootstrap samples (
The RFQ Uncertainty subscale was found to significantly predict all three PRFQ subscales: Pre-mentalizing (
Predictors of parental reflective functioning, with 95% bias corrected confidence intervals reported in parenthesis.
Variables |
|
|
Std. All |
|
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
A12—Reflective functioning uncertainty | −0.103 (−0.206, 0.000) | 0.052 | −0.151 |
|
A13—Reflective functioning certainty | −0.137 (−0.183, −0.090) | 0.024 | −0.372 |
|
|
A14—Positive coparenting | −0.001 (−0.002, 0.001) | 0.001 | −0.046 | 0.461 | |
A35—Negative coparenting | 0.009 (0.005, 0.013) | 0.002 | 0.292 |
|
|
A15—DASS anxiety | 0.030 (−0.009, 0.068) | 0.020 | 0.102 | 0.122 | |
A16—DASS depression | 0.005 (−0.005, 0.016) | 0.005 | 0.070 | 0.317 | |
A17—DASS stress | −0.009 (−0.021, 0.002) | 0.006 | −0.130 | 0.108 | |
|
A18—Reflective functioning uncertainty | −0.454 (−0.890, −0.001) | 0.228 | −0.191 |
|
A19—Reflective functioning certainty | 0.039 (−0.188, 0.272) | 0.116 | 0.030 | 0.737 | |
A20—Positive coparenting | 0.003 (−0.003, 0.009) | 0.003 | 0.064 | 0.338 | |
A36—Negative coparenting | 0.008 (−0.007, 0.023) | 0.008 | 0.080 | 0.273 | |
A21—DASS anxiety | 0.098 (−0.051, 0.243) | 0.075 | 0.095 | 0.189 | |
A22—DASS depression | −0.018 (−0.063, 0.029) | 0.024 | −0.068 | 0.444 | |
A23—DASS stress | −0.027 (−0.073, 0.015) | 0.022 | −0.109 | 0.216 | |
|
A24—Reflective functioning uncertainty | 0.452 (0.183, 0.720) | 0.137 | 0.274 |
|
A25—Reflective functioning certainty | 0.225 (0.077, 0.368) | 0.074 | 0.252 |
|
|
A26—Positive coparenting | 0.005 (0.000, 0.009) | 0.002 | 0.159 |
|
|
A37—Negative coparenting | −0.008 (−0.020, 0.003) | 0.006 | −0.113 | 0.157 | |
A27—DASS anxiety | 0.074 (−0.030, 0.177) | 0.053 | 0.103 | 0.159 | |
A28—DASS depression | −0.013 (−0.044, 0.019) | 0.016 | −0.072 | 0.402 | |
A29—DASS stress | 0.008 (−0.023, 0.038) | 0.016 | 0.048 | 0.591 |
Pre-mentalizing R2 = 0.268, Certainty R2 = 0.095, Interest and curiosity R2 = 0.097. Confidence intervals and standard errors based on 10,000 bootstrap samples (
The certainty subscale (but not the uncertainty subscale) of the RFQ was found to predict both positive (
Predictors of coparenting, with 95% bias corrected confidence intervals reported in parenthesis.
Variables |
|
Std. All |
|
||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
A1—Reflective functioning uncertainty | 5.207 (−4.389, 14.274) | 4.732 | 0.094 | 0.271 |
A2—Reflective functioning certainty | 7.647 (2.820, 12.232) | 2.402 | 0.256 |
|
|
A5—DASS anxiety | −2.501 (−6.058, 0.901) | 1.787 | −0.104 | 0.162 | |
A8—DASS depression | −1.698 (−2.672, −0.686) | 0.503 | −0.274 |
|
|
A11—DASS stress | 0.633 (−0.436, 1.699) | 0.544 | 0.108 | 0.245 | |
|
A30—Reflective functioning uncertainty | −1.476 (−4.940, 2.186) | 1.807 | −0.065 | 0.414 |
A31—Reflective functioning certainty | −2.667 (−4.422, −0.803) | 0.917 | −0.216 |
|
|
A32—DASS anxiety | 1.900 (0.468, 3.364) | 0.737 | 0.192 |
|
|
A33—DASS depression | 0.664 (0.243, 1.080) | 0.211 | 0.260 |
|
|
A34—DASS stress | −0.166 (−0.562, 0.242) | 0.207 | −0.069 | 0.422 |
Positive coparenting R2 = 0.151, Negative coparenting R2 = 0.188. Confidence intervals and standard errors based on 10,000 bootstrap samples (
The uncertainty subscale of the reflective functioning questionnaire was found to predict DASS symptoms of anxiety (
Predictors of depression, anxiety and stress, with 95% bias corrected confidence intervals reported in parenthesis.
Variables |
|
|
Std. All |
|
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
A3—Reflective functioning uncertainty | 0.946 (0.615, 1.297) | 0.174 | 0.412 |
|
A4—Reflective functioning certainty | −0.151 (−0.327, 0.034) | 0.093 | −0.122 | 0.103 | |
|
A6—Reflective functioning uncertainty | 5.051 (3.658, 6.402) | 0.695 | 0.566 |
|
A7—Reflective functioning certainty | 0.033 (−0.608, 0.670) | 0.323 | 0.007 | 0.919 | |
|
A9—Reflective functioning uncertainty | 4.815 (3.537, 6.043) | 0.632 | 0.511 |
|
A10—Reflective functioning certainty | −0.674 (−1.291, −0.011) | 0.327 | −0.132 |
|
Anxiety R2 = 0.258, Depression R2 = 0.315, Stress R2 = 0.378. Confidence Intervals and standard errors based on 10,000 bootstrap samples (
We performed a number of analyses to determine whether any indirect effects were present. In particular we explored whether there was an indirect effect of general reflective functioning on child social emotional development via parental reflective functioning. In the present study, there was an indirect effect of the certainty subscale of the RFQ on child social emotional development via the Pre-mentalizing subscale of the PRFQ (
Indirect effects of general reflective functioning on child social emotional development via parental reflective functioning, with 95% bias corrected confidence intervals reported in parenthesis.
Variables |
|
|
Std. All |
|
---|---|---|---|---|
Indirect pathway from RFQ (uncertainty) to ASQ via PRFQ (pre-mentalizing). | −0.038 (−0.098, 0.000) | 0.025 | −0.035 | 0.130 |
Indirect pathway from RFQ (certainty) to ASQ via PRFQ (pre-mentalizing). | −0.051 (−0.093, −0.017) | 0.019 | −0.085 |
|
Indirect pathway from RFQ (uncertainty) to ASQ via PRFQ (certainty). | 0.026 (−0.003, 0.068) | 0.019 | 0.023 | 0.170 |
Indirect pathway from RFQ (certainty) to ASQ via PRFQ (certainty). | −0.002 (−0.019, 0.012) | 0.007 | −0.004 | 0.767 |
Indirect pathway from RFQ (uncertainty) to ASQ via PRFQ (interest and curiosity). | −0.02 (−0.064, 0.016) | 0.020 | −0.018 | 0.325 |
Indirect pathway from RFQ (certainty) to ASQ via PRFQ (interest and curiosity). | −0.01 (−0.029, 0.009) | 0.009 | −0.016 | 0.299 |
Confidence intervals and standard errors based on 10,000 bootstrap samples (
We also explored whether there was an indirect effect of general reflective functioning on child social emotional development via coparenting. This was not found to be the case, however there was an indirect effect of negative coparenting on child social emotional development via the PRFQ Pre-mentalizing subscale (
Indirect effects of general reflective functioning on child social emotional development via coparenting, with 95% bias corrected confidence intervals reported in parenthesis.
Variables |
|
|
Std. All |
|
---|---|---|---|---|
Indirect pathway from RFQ (uncertainty) to ASQ via positive coparenting. | 0.014 (−0.011, 0.050) | 0.016 | 0.012 | 0.382 |
Indirect pathway from RFQ (certainty) to ASQ via positive coparenting. | 0.02 (0.000, 0.048) | 0.012 | 0.033 | 0.107 |
Indirect pathway from RFQ (uncertainty) to ASQ via negative coparenting. | −0.008 (−0.041, 0.013) | 0.013 | −0.007 | 0.536 |
Indirect pathway from RFQ (certainty) to ASQ via negative coparenting. | −0.015 (−0.039, 0.001) | 0.011 | −0.025 | 0.160 |
Indirect pathway from negative coparenting to ASQ via PRFQ (pre-mentalizing). | 0.003 (0.001, 0.006) | 0.001 | 0.067 |
|
Confidence intervals and standard errors based on 10,000 bootstrap samples (
Finally, we explored whether there would be an indirect effect of symptoms of depression and anxiety on child social emotional development via coparenting. As seen in
Indirect effects of DASS subscales on child social emotional development via coparenting, with 95% bias corrected confidence intervals reported in parenthesis.
Variables |
|
|
Std. All |
|
---|---|---|---|---|
Indirect pathway from DASS depression to ASQ via positive coparenting | −0.004 (−0.011, 0.000) | 0.003 | −0.036 | 0.109 |
Indirect pathway from DASS depression to ASQ via negative coparenting | 0.004 (0.000, 0.010) | 0.003 | 0.030 | 0.153 |
Indirect pathway from DASS anxiety to ASQ via positive coparenting | −0.007 (−0.021, 0.002) | 0.006 | −0.014 | 0.288 |
Indirect pathway from DASS anxiety to ASQ via negative coparenting | 0.011 (−0.001, 0.028) | 0.008 | 0.022 | 0.162 |
Indirect pathway from DASS stress to ASQ via positive coparenting | 0.002 (−0.001, 0.006) | 0.002 | 0.014 | 0.382 |
Indirect pathway from DASS stress to ASQ via negative coparenting | −0.001 (−0.004, 0.002) | 0.001 | −0.008 | 0.523 |
Confidence intervals and standard errors based on 10,000 bootstrap samples (
The overall aim of the present study was to cross-sectionally investigate the variables involved in predicting child outcomes in early childhood. The specific aims of the present study were to investigate relationships between parental mental health, parental reflective functioning, coparenting and child social emotional development in both mothers and fathers during early childhood. Surprisingly, the present study found that both general reflective functioning and parental symptoms of depression, anxiety and stress were not significant predictors of child social emotional (SE) development. However, in line with our expectations both coparenting (positive) and parental reflective functioning (in particular Pre-mentalizing and Certainty) were found to significantly predict child SE development.
As anticipated, general reflective functioning (uncertainty subscale only) predicted symptoms of depression and anxiety, while parental depression and anxiety were both predictors of coparenting (anxiety predicted negative coparenting only). General reflective functioning (certainty only) was also found to predict coparenting. Coparenting in turn was found to predict the parental reflective functioning (positive coparenting predicted PRFQ Pre-mentalizing, while negative coparenting predicted PRFQ Interest and Curiosity). Interestingly, parental reflective functioning was not predicted by parental depression and anxiety in the present study, but was predicted by general reflective functioning.
Given the pattern of findings that were identified, in conjunction with some preliminary suggestions in further research, some exploratory tests of indirect associations were carried out. Of note, we found an indirect effect of general reflective functioning (certainty) on child SE development via parental reflective functioning (Pre-mentalizing). We also found an indirect effect of negative coparenting on child SE development via parental reflective functioning (Pre-mentalizing). We did not however find any indirect effects between depression and anxiety, coparenting and child SE development. The current results support a growing body of research highlighting the important role reflective functioning plays in child development and wellbeing as well as parental mental health and the interparental relationship.
The significant relationship found in our study between parental reflective functioning and child SE development was anticipated given prior research demonstrating links between higher maternal and paternal reflective functioning and better social emotional adjustment in children (
Interestingly, in the present study greater certainty about mental states (as shown by the Certainty subscale of the PRFQ) was linked with fewer social emotional symptoms. It is important to note that very high levels of certainty about mental states may suggest intrusive mentalizing (also known as hypermentalizing), whereby the parent does not recognize that it is not possible to fully comprehend the mental states of others (e.g., mental states are opaque) while very low levels of certainty may indicate hypomentalizing (a very poor understanding of one’s child’s mental states;
The significant relationship found between higher levels of positive coparenting and better child SE development (
Based on prior research, we also hypothesized that reflective functioning would be a key variable involved in predicting coparenting, and this was found to be the case. In particular, higher levels of certainty about mental states were linked with more positive coparenting and less negative coparenting. This is unsurprising given prior research which has found associations between higher reflective functioning and better coparenting quality (
We also reasoned that having a strong coparenting relationship may support the development of parental reflective functioning, and this was again supported in our results. We found that more positive coparenting predicted fewer mentalizing difficulties as shown through lower levels of pre-mentalizing modes, while more negative coparenting predicted less interest and curiosity about their infant’s internal world. It makes sense that this reciprocal relationship would exist between coparenting and reflective functioning, whereby strong reflective capacity enhances one’s ability to work well in a parenting team and that in turn supports more ability to be reflective about a child’s internal world.
Surprisingly, in the present study, parental symptoms of depression, anxiety and stress were not significant predictors of child SE development. This was unexpected given the large body of research that has previously shown associations between parental depression and anxiety and child outcomes (
Relationship between parental depression and anxiety, reflective functioning, parental reflective functioning, coparenting and child social emotional symptoms. RFQ, reflective functioning questionnaire; DASS, depression, anxiety and stress scale; PRF-Q, parental reflective functioning questionnaire; ASQ-SE, ages and stages questionnaire—social emotional.
In line with our expectations, we did find that poorer general reflective functioning (as demonstrated by higher levels of uncertainty about mental states) predicted greater symptoms of depression and anxiety. This is consistent with a body of research demonstrating general associations between poorer reflective functioning and higher levels of depression and anxiety (
Parental depression and anxiety were also found to predict coparenting such that higher levels of parental depression were associated with less positive and more negative coparenting, while higher levels of parental anxiety were associated with more negative coparenting. This is in line with a body of research suggesting that parental depression and anxiety negatively impact the coparenting relationship (
We also found that parental reflective functioning was predicted by general reflective functioning such that higher levels of RFQ uncertainty and lower levels of RFQ certainty predicted increased scores on the PRFQ pre-mentalizing modes. This makes sense given that high levels of pre-mentalizing modes are indicative of a lack of reflective capacity, in the same way that very high uncertainty and low certainty may indicate difficulties with mentalizing (
In the present study we also carried out some exploratory mediation analyses, and found an indirect effect of general reflective functioning (certainty) on child SE development via parental reflective functioning (Pre-mentalizing). We found that greater certainty about mental states was associated with lower pre-mentalizing modes, which in turn was associated with better child SE development. General reflective functioning was not found to be a significant predictor of child SE development, however this is likely because the relationships between general reflective functioning and child SE development is fully explained by parental reflective functioning.
Given prior research suggesting that coparenting may act a mediator for the relationships between anxiety and depression and child outcomes (
Unlike
Our study is strengthened by our inclusion of both mothers and fathers, and an adequately-sized sample that allowed us to examine a range of key variables (parental mental health, coparenting, both general and parental reflective functioning) that are thought to predict child SE development. Nevertheless, our findings do need to be considered in light of several limitations. Firstly, the cross-sectional nature of this data prevents us from drawing causal inferences between study variables. The order in which we tested our variables was informed by prior literature and theoretical considerations, however these analyses alone are unable to make an inference of causality. For example, we argue that poor general reflective functioning may lead to increased risk of experiencing depression and anxiety, however there is also evidence suggesting that while experiencing depression and anxiety, an individual’s reflective processes are impeded (
Another limitation within our study is our sole reliance on self-report measures for all study variables. In particular, coparenting, reflective functioning and child SE development are likely to be more accurately measured via observational tasks. This is because parents may lack the insight to answer accurately, or may attempt to portray a more favorable image of themselves and their coparental and parent–child relationships. Future research examining the relationship between these variables would benefit from including additional methods of data collection such as behavioral observation or interviews. Our data is also limited by the fact that while we included both fathers and mothers, we did not recruit couples and therefore we are limited in the inferences we can draw about how one parent’s reflective functioning may influence the other parent and in turn did not have an additional source of data on either the coparenting relationship or child SE development (i.e., the other parent may view the coparenting relationship or child’s level of development differently).
Our study also recruited participants from Western countries with a majority of participants identifying as Caucasian, thus some caution should be applied when attempting to generalize these findings into other cultural settings. Future research may wish to consider investigating how coparenting and reflective functioning relate to child SE development in different cultural contexts, given prior research establishing cultural differences in child care practices (
Finally, the predictors examined in the present study explained only 18.7% of the variance in child SE development, which is a relatively small proportion of variance. This leaves 81.3% of the variance unexplained by the predictors considered in this study. This would suggest that numerous other variables are involved in predicting child outcomes, and future research may wish to consider additional factors that may be important to social emotional development in young children. In particular it may be important to consider variables such as the social support available, maternal and paternal attachment style, level of parental self-efficacy and stress as well as parental self-compassion.
This study adds to a small but growing body of research investigating how both coparenting and reflective functioning interact to predict child outcomes. We are one of the first studies to demonstrate that reflective functioning is a key predictor of the coparenting relationship. We are also one of the first studies to consider how parental mental health fits into this picture. Parental mental health, and maternal depression in particular, has long been considered a key risk factor for the development of adverse child outcomes, and therefore targeting maternal depression has been a key focus of many public health initiatives during the perinatal period. Our results appear to suggest that parental reflective functioning is one of the most important predictors of child outcomes over and above parental mental health. Current interventions designed to improve parental reflective functioning, both group-based and dyadic, are still being refined and there is limited evidence for their effectiveness (
Our findings suggest that parental reflective functioning appears to play a large role in developing both a strong coparenting relationship and also supporting child social emotional development. Therefore, we hope these findings will inform future research and enable the continued development of early interventions for new parents that specifically target their reflective capacity. Targeting reflective functioning is likely to in turn reduce symptoms of poor mental health, improve coparenting and general family functioning and most importantly enable optimal social emotional development in infants and young children.
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Curtin Human Research Ethics Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
MD conceptualized and carried out the research project, including the selection of study variables, data collection, and data analysis and was the principal author of this publication. RR, EI, VM, and RK were the supervisors of the study, assisting in designing and conducting the research, and providing feedback on the publication. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.