Front. Psychol. Frontiers in Psychology Front. Psychol. 1664-1078 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1054723 Psychology Original Research The relationship between parental mental health, reflective functioning coparenting and social emotional development in 0-3 year old children De PalmaMia1* RooneyRosanna1* IzettElizabeth1 ManciniVincent123 KaneRobert1 1Discipline of Psychology, School of Population Health, Curtin University, Perth, WA, Australia 2Telethon Kids Institute, Perth, WA, Australia 3Division of Paediatrics, UWA Medical School, University of Western Australia, Perth, WA, Australia

Edited by: Rosario Cabello, University of Malaga, Spain

Reviewed by: Karolina Lutkiewicz, University of Gdansk, Poland; Darpan Kaur, Mahatma Gandhi Missions Medical College and Hospital, India

*Correspondence: Mia De Palma, mia.depalma@curtin.edu.au Rosanna Rooney, r.rooney@curtin.edu.au

This article was submitted to Developmental Psychology, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology

30 05 2023 2023 14 1054723 27 09 2022 03 03 2023 Copyright © 2023 De Palma, Rooney, Izett, Mancini and Kane. 2023 De Palma, Rooney, Izett, Mancini and Kane

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Introduction

The transition to parenthood is a high-risk period for many parents and is an important period for child development. Research has identified that parental mental health, reflective functioning (capacity to consider mental states of oneself and others) and coparenting (capacity to work together well as a parenting team) may be particularly significant predictors of later child outcomes, however these factors have seldom been considered together. The present study therefore aimed to investigate the relationship between these factors and the extent to which they predict child social emotional development.

Methods

Three hundred and fifty parents of infants aged 0 to 3 years 11 months were recruited to complete an online Qualtrics questionnaire.

Results

Results indicate that both positive coparenting and parental reflective functioning (Pre-mentalizing and Certainty subscales) were found to significantly predict child development. General reflective functioning (Uncertainty subscale) predicted parental depression and anxiety, however unexpectedly, parental mental health was not a significant predictor of child development, but did predict coparenting. General reflective functioning (Certainty subscale) was also found to predict coparenting, which in turn was found to predict parental reflective functioning. We found an indirect effect of general reflective functioning (Certainty) on child SE development via parental reflective functioning (Pre-mentalizing). We also found an indirect effect of negative coparenting on child development via parental reflective functioning (Pre-mentalizing).

Discussion

The current results support a growing body of research highlighting the important role reflective functioning plays in child development and wellbeing as well as parental mental health and the interparental relationship.

parental reflective functioning coparenting child social emotional development reflective functioning parental mental health child development and infant mental health

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      It is widely understood that the first 1,000 days of life—the period of development from conception to age two—is one of the most crucial periods of development for a child (Moore et al., 2017). Given the importance of this developmental period, it is thought that adverse experiences during this time may be particularly harmful for the child’s ongoing social emotional development, with consequences potentially spanning the child’s lifetime (Lyons-Ruth et al., 2017; Moore et al., 2017). Social emotional development in infancy entails the gradual increase in emotion recognition and expression, and participation in social interaction (Halle and Darling-Churchill, 2016). It is important to consider which factors in particular place a child at increased risk of adverse outcomes (Newland, 2015).

      The transition to parenthood is accompanied by a series of novel and pre-existing stressors, and an increased demand on psychosocial resources that brings with it a greater risk of developing mental health difficulties for parents (Nyström and Öhrling, 2004). Within Australia, 21% of adults meet the diagnostic criteria for a mental health disorder (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2020-21), and both maternal and paternal depression and anxiety are linked with a number of adverse child outcomes (McCall-Hosenfeld et al., 2016). These include delayed social (Ip et al., 2018), emotional (Kingston and Tough, 2014), behavioral and cognitive development (Kingston et al., 2012), lower ability to self-regulate (Hernández-Martínez et al., 2008), a more difficult temperament (Werner et al., 2007; Parfitt et al., 2013) and developmental delays (Davis and Sandman, 2010). Research has also found an increase in rates of internalizing and externalizing symptoms (Kane and Garber, 2004; Verbeek et al., 2012; Matijasevich et al., 2015) as well as depression (Murray et al., 2011; Pearson et al., 2013), among children of parents with perinatal depression or anxiety. Poor parental mental health has further been linked with difficulties in the parent-infant relationship (Murray et al., 2011; Lilja et al., 2012; Verbeek et al., 2012; Pearson et al., 2013; Matijasevich et al., 2015).

      The importance of the parent infant relationship has been particularly emphasized within the field of attachment, with research consistently finding links between secure parent-infant attachment relationships and child outcomes such as positive mental health, social and emotional intelligence, physical health and enhanced cognitive capacity later in life (Ranson and Urichuk, 2008; Boldt et al., 2014). Caregiver sensitivity/responsiveness to an infant’s needs has been associated with attachment security, as has the parent’s own attachment representations (van Ijzendoorn, 1995; O'Neill et al., 2021). Parental attachment has been also linked with parenting behaviors, whereby secure attachment correlated with more positive parenting behaviors (Huang, 2021). Furthermore, attachment relationships are known to pass from parent to child, across generations (Steele and Steele, 2008; Sette et al., 2015).

      Parental Reflective functioning (PRF) is a proposed mechanism through which these attachment relationships are transmitted from parent to child (Kelly et al., 2005; Steele and Steele, 2008). Reflective functioning, also referred to as mentalization, is defined as one’s ability to understand and link mental states with behavior both for oneself and for others (Slade, 2005; Stacks et al., 2014). Psychodynamic theorists assert that reflective functioning is involved in the development and maintenance of psychopathology including depression (Luyten et al., 2013; Luyten and Fonagy, 2018) and anxiety (Lavoie et al., 2014). They suggest that while in a depressed state, individuals may be significantly biased in their reflective processes and are typically not able to engage in reflective thinking (Luyten et al., 2013; Luyten and Fonagy, 2018). A lack of reflective capacity is also thought to prevent an individual from regulating their intense emotional experiences or modulating the behavioral expression of these emotions (Bouchard et al., 2008). These suggestions are supported by a body of research finding associations between poorer reflective functioning and higher depression levels across varying samples (Fischer-Kern et al., 2013; Belvederi Murri et al., 2017; Bigelow et al., 2018; Wendelboe et al., 2021). Interventions targeting reflective functioning have also been found to have a small effect in reducing both general and interpersonal distress symptoms (Hayden et al., 2018).

      High levels of PRF is thought to be essential to children’s ability to regulate their emotions, and develop secure attachment relationships (Ordway et al., 2015). In fact, reflective functioning has been found to mediate the relationship between parental attachment and child social emotional wellbeing (Nijssens et al., 2020). Research suggests that parental reflective functioning allows parents to more consistently and sensitively respond to cues from their infant (Stacks et al., 2014). Moreover, poor maternal reflective functioning has been linked with adverse child outcomes including anxiety, externalizing behaviors, poor social competence and difficulty regulating emotions (Camoirano, 2017; Colonnesi et al., 2019). Other recent studies have shown that both maternal and paternal reflective functioning are linked with better social emotional adjustment (Gordo et al., 2020; Salo et al., 2021), enhanced social competence and higher levels of reflective functioning among adolescents (aged 14–18 years; Benbassat and Priel, 2012). Lower levels of reflective functioning in both parents have also been linked with more dysfunctional parent–child interactions (Vismara et al., 2021).

      More recent research seeks to move beyond maternal–infant relationships to consider how the broader family system impacts a developing child. Family systems theory suggests that family-level processes influence child wellbeing over and above dyadic relationships within the family (i.e., the couple relationship, parent–child relationship and sibling relationships; Boričević Maršanić and Kušmić, 2013). Coparenting is a concept nested within family systems theory, and focuses on the intersection between parents’ romantic relationship and their new role as a parent (Salo et al., 2021). Correlational research has found that coparenting predicts unique variance in child social emotional development, and argues coparenting may have a larger impact on social emotional development than that of general parenting and the couple relationship alone (Feinberg and Kan, 2008; Boričević Maršanić and Kušmić, 2013). Coparenting is defined as a parents’ ability to work in harmony as a team for their child’s benefit (Le et al., 2016). When coparenting works, parents are able to come together and agree on how to parent their child, making coparenting a key predictor of overall family functioning (Dollberg et al., 2021). Feinberg (2003) describes a model of coparenting comprised of 7 dimensions, 5 encapsulating positive coparenting (coparenting agreement, coparenting closeness, coparenting support, endorsement of partner parenting, division of labor) and two which make up negative coparenting (exposure to conflict, and coparenting undermining).

      Positive coparenting is associated with a variety of child outcomes including cognitive development (Shai, 2019) psychological and social emotional wellbeing (Teubert and Pinquart, 2010), social skills (Cabrera et al., 2012) and prosocial behavior (Scrimgeour et al., 2013). Increased positive coparenting has also been moderately linked to increased academic achievement in school (Dopkins Stright and Neitzel, 2003; Cabrera et al., 2012), faster language development, and increased social functioning (Cheng et al., 2009). Negative coparenting has been linked with behavior problems (LeRoy et al., 2013), reduced communication and social skills (Nandy et al., 2021), poor child adjustment and later psychopathology (Umemura et al., 2015).

      Coparenting has also been linked with parental mental health, with findings indicating that parental depression negatively impacts the coparenting relationship (Price-Robertson et al., 2017; Tissot et al., 2017; Williams, 2018; Turney and Hardie, 2021). Other research suggests that coparenting conflict increases depressive symptoms among mothers (Cabrera et al., 2012), which is in line with studies demonstrating a link between relationship conflict and parental anxiety and depression (Yap et al., 2014). These findings are also consistent with Feinberg and colleagues, who found that interventions targeting the coparenting relationship can reduce symptoms of depression and anxiety in mothers (Feinberg and Kan, 2008; Feinberg et al., 2016).

      Recent research has hypothesized that coparenting may act as a mechanism through which anxiety is transmitted from parents to children, with study findings demonstrating correlations between parental anxiety and undermining coparenting, as well as between undermining coparenting and fearful temperaments in children (Metz et al., 2018). A similar finding has also been shown for maternal depression with one study finding that coparenting support mediated the relationship between maternal depression and child outcomes, with increased symptoms of depression linked to poorer coparental support, which then predicted an increase in behavior problems among children (Tissot et al., 2016).

      Jessee et al. (2018) theorized that reflective functioning may be a protective factor during the transition to parenthood. Given this transition is often characterized by conflict and distress for new parents, a greater capacity to understand the emotional experiences underlying the behavior of themselves and their partner protects the couple relationship and the emerging coparenting relationship. Since the relational patterns that emerge during this period often endure throughout the remainder of the coparenting relationship, it is crucial to understand the factors that may underpin both successful and at risk coparenting relationships (Jessee et al., 2018). Several studies have found links between reflective functioning and coparenting or couple interactions. One such study followed a high-risk sample of pregnant women, finding that reflective functioning was associated with greater couple cohesion (Borelli et al., 2021). Similarly, other studies have also found a relationship between better parental reflective functioning and more positive coparenting relationships (Jessee, 2012; Marcu et al., 2016; Shai et al., 2017; Holtzinger, 2021).

      While the examination of reflective functioning and coparenting together is growing, very few studies have gone a step further and examined how child outcomes fit within this picture. In their study, Jessee et al. (2018) recruited 103 couples who were followed longitudinally from pre-birth to 13 months post-birth. Findings suggested that maternal, but not paternal, reflective functioning predicted both supportive and undermining coparenting (Jessee et al., 2018). They also found that higher interparental conflict was associated with greater levels of anger and lower levels of enthusiasm and compliance in children. Reflective functioning was not found to be associated with any child outcome variable (Jessee et al., 2018). The authors hypothesized that this may have been due to the low stress nature of the 15-min family play task used, which may not have been sufficient for behaviors typically associated with poor reflective functioning to emerge (Jessee et al., 2018).

      León and Olhaberry (2020) went a step further in their study, carrying out an exploratory mediation analysis which found that the quality of triadic interactions (the interaction between both parents and their infant, which includes coparenting) mediated the relationship between maternal but not paternal reflective functioning and child social emotional outcomes (León and Olhaberry, 2020). Fifty Chilean families whose 12 to 38 month old children had been referred for social–emotional difficulties participated in this study (León and Olhaberry, 2020). In addition to the novel mediation analysis, they also found that more positive triadic interactions were associated with higher levels of both maternal and paternal reflective functioning as well as fewer social emotional difficulties in children. The relationships between maternal and paternal reflective functioning and social emotional difficulties were not significant because this relationship was fully explained by triadic interactions. This study also found that when both mothers’ and fathers’ reflective functioning were included as predictors of triadic interactions, only mothers’ reflective functioning remained a significant predictor (León and Olhaberry, 2020).

      It is of note that neither Jessee et al. (2018) nor León and Olhaberry (2020) included parental mental health as a variable within their studies. Given the established link between parental mental health difficulties and adverse child outcomes (McCall-Hosenfeld et al., 2016), it can be argued that parental mental health may be a significant piece of the puzzle linking parental reflective functioning, coparenting and child outcomes.

      To our knowledge, Dollberg et al. (2021) were the first to include parental mental health, proposing a mediation-moderation hypothesis whereby coparenting would mediate the relationship between parental anxiety and child outcomes with parental reflective functioning acting as a moderator variable (Dollberg et al., 2021). They recruited 78 couples with children aged between 3 and 5, and found that coparenting did mediate the relationship between parent anxiety and child outcomes, however no support was found for reflective functioning as a moderator within this relationship (Dollberg et al., 2021). Findings suggested that reflective functioning did moderate the relationship between parental anxiety and child outcomes when coparenting was not included in the model (Dollberg et al., 2021). Reflective functioning was not found to be significantly correlated with any study variables with the exception of father’s reflective functioning which was significantly associated with father’s anxiety levels (Dollberg et al., 2021). The authors suggest that the low sample size may have contributed to the insignificant mediation-moderation hypothesis (Dollberg et al., 2021), therefore it may be worth examining whether this relationship exists in a larger sample of parents.

      The current study: Aims and hypotheses

      The overall aim of the present study was to cross-sectionally investigate the variables involved in predicting child outcomes in early childhood, in particular, parental mental health, parental reflective functioning and coparenting and to examine how these variables are related to one another among parents. This is important to consider given the scarcity of research examining these variables together, particularly within a large sample of parents who have children in the period of early childhood. Given that the coparenting relationship emerges in early infancy, it is particularly worth examining how these variables interact in the first 4 years of the child’s life.

      Informed by prior studies, we hypothesized that:

      Poorer infant social emotional development will be predicted by higher levels of parental depression and anxiety, less positive and more negative coparenting and poorer general reflective functioning and parental reflective functioning.

      Poorer parental reflective functioning will be predicted by poorer general reflective functioning, less positive and more negative coparenting and increased symptoms of depression and anxiety.

      More negative and less positive coparenting will be predicted by poorer general reflective functioning and increased symptoms of depression and anxiety.

      Increased symptoms of depression and anxiety will be predicted by poorer general reflective functioning.

      Materials and methods Methods Design

      The present study implemented a cross-sectional, correlational research design to examine associations between parental mental health, parental reflective functioning, coparenting, and child social emotional development.

      Participants

      Participants were 350 parents (175 women, 175 men) with children aged 0 to 3 years 11 months who were recruited via Prolific, an online recruiting platform. Inclusion criteria were met if the participant had a child in the correct age range and was in a relationship with and living with the other parent of their child. Participants were paid £3.75 GBP (roughly $7.15 AUD) through the Prolific website after completion of the questionnaire.

      Participants’ ages ranged from 19 to 61 years (M = 33.63, SD = 5.31) and children were aged between 0 and 46 months (M = 21.29, SD = 12.77). 72.9% of the sample were married, 16.3% were engaged, 8.3% were in a defacto relationship and the remaining participants described their relationship status as other. This sample consisted predominantly of participants who identified as Caucasian (including British, European, American, Australian, or New Zealander; 82.5%). Other ethnicities included Asian or South East Asian (5.7%), Black (including African, African American, African British and Black Carribean) 4.3%, Hispanic 2.6%, South Asian (including Pakistani, Indian and Bangladeshi; 3.1%), Arabic or Islam 0.86%, mixed ethnicity 0.86%, while the final 0.29% of participants identified as Wichita or Native American.

      In order to detect a medium size effect using a mediation analysis, research suggests a sample size of at least 300 participants is needed (Fritz and MacKinnon, 2007). Therefore, the present sample of 350 participants was deemed sufficient to detect at least medium-sized effects.

      Materials

      The Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS; Lovibond and Lovibond, 1995) is a 21-item self-report questionnaire measuring symptoms of depression, anxiety and stress over the past 7 days, across three 7-item subscales. Items are measured on a four-point Likert scale from 0 (“Did not apply to me at all”) to 3 (“Applied to me very much, or most of the time”) and are summed, with higher scores indicative of more severe symptoms. The DASS 21 is a widely used, well-validated scale that has demonstrated good internal reliability across its three subscales (Cronbach’s α = 0.81–0.88) as well as good convergent validity (r = 0.5–0.8) as shown by correlations between the DASS and other validated measures of depression and anxiety (Osman et al., 2012).

      The 4-item Couples Satisfaction Index (CSI; Funk and Rogge, 2007) is a measure of relationship satisfaction, developed using item response theory. Responses are recorded on a 6- or 7-point Likert scale. Ratings are summed, with higher scores indicative of greater relationship satisfaction. This scale has shown good reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.98) and convergent validity (r = 0.85–0.99) as shown by correlations between the CSI and other validated measures of relationship satisfaction and has been found sensitive to changes in relationship satisfaction (Funk and Rogge, 2007).

      The Coparenting Relationship Scale (CRS; Feinberg et al., 2012) is 35-item self-report questionnaire that measures coparenting across 7 dimensions: agreement, endorsement, closeness, support and cooperation, division of labor, competition, undermining and the extent of child exposure to parental conflict. Items include “I believe my partner is a good parent” and “My partner undermines my parenting” and are rated on a 7-point scale from 0 (“Not true of us”) to 6 (“Very true of us”). Of the seven subscales, 5 focus on positive aspects of coparenting, while 2 (competition and undermining) focus on the more negative parts of the construct. Therefore, in our study, we created two subscales, positive coparenting and negative coparenting. Items were summed, and higher scores on the positive coparenting subscale indicate a more positive coparenting relationship, while higher scores on the negative coparenting subscale indicate greater levels of competition, undermining and parental conflict. This scale has shown good reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.91–0.94) and construct validity (r = 0.60–0.74) as shown by correlations between the CRS and other related constructs (Feinberg et al., 2012).

      The 8-item Reflective Functioning Questionnaire (RFQ; Fonagy et al., 2016) is a measure of mentalizing that is made up of two scales (certainty about mental states [RFQ_C] and uncertainty about mental states [RFQ_U]). This measure is scored on a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (“do not agree at all”) to 7 (“agree completely”). Items include “People’s thoughts are a mystery to me” and “I always know what I feel.” Adequate reliability has been demonstrated (Cronbach’s α ≥ 0.7), along with good construct validity shown through positive correlations between RFQ_U and the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (r = 0.66), and positive correlations between RFQ_C and the Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness (r = 0.39; Cucchi et al., 2018).

      The Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire (PRFQ-18; Luyten et al., 2017) is an 18-item self-report questionnaire that measures parental reflective functioning across three subscales. Subscales include: Pre-Mentalizing (e.g., “My child cries around strangers to embarrass me”), Certainty about Mental States (e.g., “I can always predict what my child will do”), Interest and Curiosity (e.g., I wonder a lot about what my child is thinking and feeling”). Items are measured on a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (“Strongly disagree”) to 7 (“Strongly agree”). This questionnaire has been related to attachment, sensitivity and parenting stress and has shown good reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.79–0.85). Construct validity (r = 0.49) has been demonstrated through correlations between the Pre-Mentalizing subscale on the PRFQ-18 and both attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance measured with the Experience of Close Relationships-Revised, as well as correlations with other related constructs (Luyten et al., 2017).

      The Ages and Stages Questionnaire: Social–Emotional (ASQ: SE 6; Squires et al., 2001) is a measure of social–emotional development in infants aged 3 to 65 months. There are specific forms for eight different age ranges. The number of items vary for each age range. This questionnaire includes 7 subscales: self-regulation, compliance, communication, autonomy affect, interaction with people, and adaptive functioning, with items measured on a 3-point Likert scale from (0 = “Most of the time,” 5 = “Sometime,” 10 = “Rarely or Never”). Mothers are also able to indicate whether the listed behavior is of concern. Five points are added to the total score if this option is ticked. Higher scores are indicative of more social–emotional problems on each respective dimension. Because of the varying number of items for each age group, total scores were averaged by dividing by total number of items on the form to enable comparison between age groups. These scales have been widely used in this area of research, and have demonstrated sufficient internal reliability and concurrent validity (Squires et al., 2001).

      Procedure

      Ethics approval for the present study was granted by the Curtin University Human Research Ethics Committee (CUHREC). Following recruitment through Prolific, participants were redirected to a Qualtrics survey containing the study’s explanatory statement and all study measures. Participants then provided consent within Qualtrics before completing the online survey which took on average 30 min to complete.

      Measures were preceded by several demographic questions (i.e., age, education level, and ethnicity and the final page of the survey provided a study debrief including links to support services). Participants were credited for their time upon valid completion of the survey.

      Data analysis plan

      Analyses were run using both SPSS (v.28) and R statistical software (R Core Team, 2020). Our mediation model was run using the Lavaan package (Rosseel, 2012). We specified a sequential mediation model to assess the association between reflective functioning and child social emotional development. Using a sequential model in this way allows the relationship between mediators to be measured as well as allowing mediators to be predicted both by reflective functioning and by preceding mediator variables. Negative emotional symptoms (DASS scores) were included as the first mediator, parental reflective functioning (PRFQ scores) included as the second mediator, and finally coparenting (CRS scores) was included as the third mediator. Bias-corrected bootstrapped confidence intervals [10,000 iterations; as recommended by Hayes, (2017)] were used to test the indirect effect of reflective functioning on child social emotional development via each of these mediators.

      Results Correlations and descriptive analyses

      To address issues of non-linearity, square root transformations were conducted for the DASS Anxiety subscale, the CSI, the RFQ Uncertainty subscale, the PRFQ Pre-mentalizing subscale, and the ASQ prior to model testing. The bivariate correlations and descriptive statistics are provided in Table 2. All of the variables with the exception of positive coparenting were significantly correlated with child social emotional development. Additionally, we observed a number of significant correlations between the predictor variables (see Table 2).

      Predicting child social emotional development

      The variables included in this sequential mediation model accounted for a statistically significant 18.7% of the variance in child social emotional development, equating to a small-sized effect. The total effect of reflective functioning (Uncertainty subscale only) on child social emotional development was statistically significant (b = 0.171, p = 0.046, 95% CI: 0.002, 0.338).

      Predictors of child development

      Despite statistically significant bivariate associations with children’s social–emotional development (Table 1), both subscales of the RFQ as well as parental symptoms of depression, anxiety and stress were not significant predictors of child social emotional development in the final model that included the complete set of predictor variables.

      Descriptive statistics and correlations between measurement variables (N = 350).

      Correlations Descriptives
      Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 M SD α
      1. Reflective functioning uncertainty - - - - - - - - - - - 0.637 0.447 0.806
      2. Reflective functioning certainty −0.737 ** - - - - - - - - - - 0.905 0.826 0.850
      3. DASS depression 0.557 ** −0.408 ** - - - - - - - - - 4.418 3.989 0.891
      4. DASS Anxiety 0.498 ** 0.424 ** 0.611 ** - - - - - - - - 1.363 1.026 0.839
      5. DASS stress 0.606 ** −0.504 ** 0.716 ** 0.700 ** - - - - - - - 6.650 4.212 0.877
      6. Parental reflective functioning pre-mentalizing 0.238** −0.370** 0.240** 0.249** 0.198** - - - - - - 1.385 0.302 0.753
      7. Parental reflective functioning certainty −0.257 ** −0.210* −0.194 ** −0.122* −0.208** −0.176 ** - - - - - 4.002 1.057 0.783
      8. Parental reflective functioning interest and curiosity −0.057 0.087 −0.010 0.052 0.036 −0.403** 0.189** - - - - 5.645 0.738 0.674
      9. Positive coparenting −0.243** 0.285** −0.317** −0.254** −0.241** −0.328** 0.085 0.225** - - - 108.611 24.425 0.939
      10. Negative coparenting 0.311** −0.321** 0.373** 0.353** 0.328** 0.426** −0.043 −0.192** −0.619** - - 11.304 10.331 0.890
      11. Relationship satisfaction 0.190* −0.208** 0.303** 0.188** 0.236** 0.210** −0.107* −0.118* −0.726** 0.476** - 2.609 0.879 0.950
      12. Child social emotional development 0.246** −0.237** 0.220** 0.236** 0.191** 0.340** −0.209** −0.165** −0.097 0.223** 0.026 1.273 0.493 0.336–0.912

      Bivariate correlations are presented on the lower quadrant. **p < 0.001, *p < 0.05. Bold values indicate statistical significance.

      Negative coparenting was also not a significant predictor. However, positive coparenting remained a significant predictor in the final model, albeit with a small effect size (b = 0.003, p = 0.043, 95% CI: 0.000, 0.005).

      The Pre-mentalizing (b = 0.373, p = 0.002, 95% CI: 0.136, 0.601) and Certainty (b = −0.057, p = 0.037, 95% CI: −0.109, −0.003) subscales of the PRFQ, but not Interest and Curiosity, were also found to be significant predictors of child social emotional development. Of the variables examined in the present study, the pre-mentalizing subscale of the PRFQ was the most significant predictor of child social emotional development (see Table 2).

      Predictors of child social emotional development, with 95% Bias corrected confidence intervals reported in parenthesis.

      Variables B (95% CI) SE B Std. All p
      C1—Reflective functioning uncertainty 0.131 (−0.053, 0.317) 0.094 0.119 0.163
      C2—Reflective functioning certainty 0.000 (−0.089, 0.095) 0.047 0.000 0.997
      B1—Positive coparenting 0.003 (0.000, 0.005) 0.001 0.131 0.043
      B2—Negative coparenting 0.006 (−0.001, 0.012) 0.003 0.115 0.086
      B3—DASS anxiety 0.058 (−0.015, 0.134) 0.038 0.119 0.130
      B4—DASS depression 0.007 (−0.011, 0.025) 0.009 0.055 0.464
      B5—DASS stress −0.009 (−0.029, 0.010) 0.010 −0.079 0.338
      B6—Parental reflective functioning pre-mentalizing 0.373 (0.136, 0.601) 0.119 0.228 0.002
      B7—Parental reflective functioning certainty −0.057 (−0.109, −0.003) 0.027 −0.121 0.037
      B8—Parental reflective functioning interest and curiosity −0.044 (−0.124, 0.035) 0.040 −0.065 0.276
      Total effect of RFQ uncertainty 0.171 (0.002, 0.338) 0.085 0.154 0.046
      Total effect of RFQ certainty −0.06 (−0.143, 0.029) 0.044 −0.101 0.172

      R2 = 0.187. Confidence intervals and standard errors based on 10,000 Bootstrap samples (N = 350). Bold values indicate statistical significance.

      Predictors of parental reflective functioning

      The RFQ Uncertainty subscale was found to significantly predict all three PRFQ subscales: Pre-mentalizing (b = −0.103, p = 0.050, 95% CI: −0.206, 0.000), Certainty (b = −0.454, p = 0.047, 95% CI: −0.890, −0.001) and Interest and Curiosity (b = 0.452, p = 0.001, 95% CI: 0.183, 0.720). Whereas the RFQ Certainty subscale was found to only significantly predict two PRFQ subscales: Pre-mentalizing (b = −0.137, p = 0.000, 95% CI: −0.183, −0.090), Interest and Curiosity (b = 0.225, p = 0.002, 95% CI: 0.077, 0.368). Negative coparenting was also found to predict the Pre-mentalizing subscale of the PRFQ (b = 0.009, p = 0.000, 95% CI: 0.005, 0.013) while Positive coparenting was found to predict the Interest and Curiosity subscale of the PRFQ (b = 0.005, p = 0.038, 95% CI: 0.000, 0.009). Parental symptoms of depression, anxiety and stress were not found to predict parental reflective functioning in the present study (see Table 3).

      Predictors of parental reflective functioning, with 95% bias corrected confidence intervals reported in parenthesis.

      Variables b SE B Std. All p
      Pre-mentalizing A12—Reflective functioning uncertainty −0.103 (−0.206, 0.000) 0.052 −0.151 0.050
      A13—Reflective functioning certainty −0.137 (−0.183, −0.090) 0.024 −0.372 0.000
      A14—Positive coparenting −0.001 (−0.002, 0.001) 0.001 −0.046 0.461
      A35—Negative coparenting 0.009 (0.005, 0.013) 0.002 0.292 0.000
      A15—DASS anxiety 0.030 (−0.009, 0.068) 0.020 0.102 0.122
      A16—DASS depression 0.005 (−0.005, 0.016) 0.005 0.070 0.317
      A17—DASS stress −0.009 (−0.021, 0.002) 0.006 −0.130 0.108
      Certainty A18—Reflective functioning uncertainty −0.454 (−0.890, −0.001) 0.228 −0.191 0.047
      A19—Reflective functioning certainty 0.039 (−0.188, 0.272) 0.116 0.030 0.737
      A20—Positive coparenting 0.003 (−0.003, 0.009) 0.003 0.064 0.338
      A36—Negative coparenting 0.008 (−0.007, 0.023) 0.008 0.080 0.273
      A21—DASS anxiety 0.098 (−0.051, 0.243) 0.075 0.095 0.189
      A22—DASS depression −0.018 (−0.063, 0.029) 0.024 −0.068 0.444
      A23—DASS stress −0.027 (−0.073, 0.015) 0.022 −0.109 0.216
      Interest and curiosity A24—Reflective functioning uncertainty 0.452 (0.183, 0.720) 0.137 0.274 0.001
      A25—Reflective functioning certainty 0.225 (0.077, 0.368) 0.074 0.252 0.002
      A26—Positive coparenting 0.005 (0.000, 0.009) 0.002 0.159 0.038
      A37—Negative coparenting −0.008 (−0.020, 0.003) 0.006 −0.113 0.157
      A27—DASS anxiety 0.074 (−0.030, 0.177) 0.053 0.103 0.159
      A28—DASS depression −0.013 (−0.044, 0.019) 0.016 −0.072 0.402
      A29—DASS stress 0.008 (−0.023, 0.038) 0.016 0.048 0.591

      Pre-mentalizing R2 = 0.268, Certainty R2 = 0.095, Interest and curiosity R2 = 0.097. Confidence intervals and standard errors based on 10,000 bootstrap samples (N = 350). Bold values indicate statistical significance.

      Predictors of coparenting

      The certainty subscale (but not the uncertainty subscale) of the RFQ was found to predict both positive (b = 7.647, p = 0.001, 95% CI: 2.820, 12.232) and negative (b = −2.667, p = 0.004, 95% CI: −4.422, −0.803) coparenting. Of the DASS subscales, only symptoms of depression were found to predict both positive (b = −1.698, p = 0.001, 95% CI: −2.672, −0.686) and negative (b = 0.664, p = 0.002, 95% CI: 0.243, 1.080) coparenting, while symptoms of anxiety were found to predict negative coparenting only (b = 1.900, p = 0.010, 95% CI: 0.468, 3.364; see Table 4).

      Predictors of coparenting, with 95% bias corrected confidence intervals reported in parenthesis.

      Variables b (95% CI) SE B Std. All p
      Positive coparenting A1—Reflective functioning uncertainty 5.207 (−4.389, 14.274) 4.732 0.094 0.271
      A2—Reflective functioning certainty 7.647 (2.820, 12.232) 2.402 0.256 0.001
      A5—DASS anxiety −2.501 (−6.058, 0.901) 1.787 −0.104 0.162
      A8—DASS depression −1.698 (−2.672, −0.686) 0.503 −0.274 0.001
      A11—DASS stress 0.633 (−0.436, 1.699) 0.544 0.108 0.245
      Negative coparenting A30—Reflective functioning uncertainty −1.476 (−4.940, 2.186) 1.807 −0.065 0.414
      A31—Reflective functioning certainty −2.667 (−4.422, −0.803) 0.917 −0.216 0.004
      A32—DASS anxiety 1.900 (0.468, 3.364) 0.737 0.192 0.010
      A33—DASS depression 0.664 (0.243, 1.080) 0.211 0.260 0.002
      A34—DASS stress −0.166 (−0.562, 0.242) 0.207 −0.069 0.422

      Positive coparenting R2 = 0.151, Negative coparenting R2 = 0.188. Confidence intervals and standard errors based on 10,000 bootstrap samples (N = 350). Bold values indicate statistical significance.

      Predictors of parental mental health

      The uncertainty subscale of the reflective functioning questionnaire was found to predict DASS symptoms of anxiety (b = 0.946, p = 0.000, 95% CI: 0.615, 1.297), depression (b = 5.051, p = 0.000, 95% CI: 3.658, 6.402) and stress (b = 4.815, p = 0.000, 95% CI: 3.537, 6.043), while the certainty subscale predicted symptoms of stress only (b = −0.674, p = 0.039, 95% CI: −1.291, −0.011; see Table 5).

      Predictors of depression, anxiety and stress, with 95% bias corrected confidence intervals reported in parenthesis.

      Variables b SE B Std. All p
      Anxiety A3—Reflective functioning uncertainty 0.946 (0.615, 1.297) 0.174 0.412 0.000
      A4—Reflective functioning certainty −0.151 (−0.327, 0.034) 0.093 −0.122 0.103
      Depression A6—Reflective functioning uncertainty 5.051 (3.658, 6.402) 0.695 0.566 0.000
      A7—Reflective functioning certainty 0.033 (−0.608, 0.670) 0.323 0.007 0.919
      Stress A9—Reflective functioning uncertainty 4.815 (3.537, 6.043) 0.632 0.511 0.000
      A10—Reflective functioning certainty −0.674 (−1.291, −0.011) 0.327 −0.132 0.039

      Anxiety R2 = 0.258, Depression R2 = 0.315, Stress R2 = 0.378. Confidence Intervals and standard errors based on 10,000 bootstrap samples (N = 350). Bold values indicate statistical significance.

      Exploratory indirect effect analyses

      We performed a number of analyses to determine whether any indirect effects were present. In particular we explored whether there was an indirect effect of general reflective functioning on child social emotional development via parental reflective functioning. In the present study, there was an indirect effect of the certainty subscale of the RFQ on child social emotional development via the Pre-mentalizing subscale of the PRFQ (b = −0.051, p = 0.009, 95% CI:-0.093, −0.017). The remaining mediation analyses explored were not significant (see Table 6).

      Indirect effects of general reflective functioning on child social emotional development via parental reflective functioning, with 95% bias corrected confidence intervals reported in parenthesis.

      Variables b SE B Std. All p
      Indirect pathway from RFQ (uncertainty) to ASQ via PRFQ (pre-mentalizing). −0.038 (−0.098, 0.000) 0.025 −0.035 0.130
      Indirect pathway from RFQ (certainty) to ASQ via PRFQ (pre-mentalizing). −0.051 (−0.093, −0.017) 0.019 −0.085 0.009
      Indirect pathway from RFQ (uncertainty) to ASQ via PRFQ (certainty). 0.026 (−0.003, 0.068) 0.019 0.023 0.170
      Indirect pathway from RFQ (certainty) to ASQ via PRFQ (certainty). −0.002 (−0.019, 0.012) 0.007 −0.004 0.767
      Indirect pathway from RFQ (uncertainty) to ASQ via PRFQ (interest and curiosity). −0.02 (−0.064, 0.016) 0.020 −0.018 0.325
      Indirect pathway from RFQ (certainty) to ASQ via PRFQ (interest and curiosity). −0.01 (−0.029, 0.009) 0.009 −0.016 0.299

      Confidence intervals and standard errors based on 10,000 bootstrap samples (N = 350). Bold values indicate statistical significance.

      We also explored whether there was an indirect effect of general reflective functioning on child social emotional development via coparenting. This was not found to be the case, however there was an indirect effect of negative coparenting on child social emotional development via the PRFQ Pre-mentalizing subscale (b = 0.003, p = 0.011, 95% CI: 0.001, 0.006). The remaining mediation analyses explored were not significant (see Table 7).

      Indirect effects of general reflective functioning on child social emotional development via coparenting, with 95% bias corrected confidence intervals reported in parenthesis.

      Variables b SE B Std. All p
      Indirect pathway from RFQ (uncertainty) to ASQ via positive coparenting. 0.014 (−0.011, 0.050) 0.016 0.012 0.382
      Indirect pathway from RFQ (certainty) to ASQ via positive coparenting. 0.02 (0.000, 0.048) 0.012 0.033 0.107
      Indirect pathway from RFQ (uncertainty) to ASQ via negative coparenting. −0.008 (−0.041, 0.013) 0.013 −0.007 0.536
      Indirect pathway from RFQ (certainty) to ASQ via negative coparenting. −0.015 (−0.039, 0.001) 0.011 −0.025 0.160
      Indirect pathway from negative coparenting to ASQ via PRFQ (pre-mentalizing). 0.003 (0.001, 0.006) 0.001 0.067 0.011

      Confidence intervals and standard errors based on 10,000 bootstrap samples (N = 350). Bold values indicate statistical significance.

      Finally, we explored whether there would be an indirect effect of symptoms of depression and anxiety on child social emotional development via coparenting. As seen in Table 8, we did not find any support for this hypothesis, with all p values found to be above the 0.05 cut-off for statistical significance.

      Indirect effects of DASS subscales on child social emotional development via coparenting, with 95% bias corrected confidence intervals reported in parenthesis.

      Variables b SE B Std. All p
      Indirect pathway from DASS depression to ASQ via positive coparenting −0.004 (−0.011, 0.000) 0.003 −0.036 0.109
      Indirect pathway from DASS depression to ASQ via negative coparenting 0.004 (0.000, 0.010) 0.003 0.030 0.153
      Indirect pathway from DASS anxiety to ASQ via positive coparenting −0.007 (−0.021, 0.002) 0.006 −0.014 0.288
      Indirect pathway from DASS anxiety to ASQ via negative coparenting 0.011 (−0.001, 0.028) 0.008 0.022 0.162
      Indirect pathway from DASS stress to ASQ via positive coparenting 0.002 (−0.001, 0.006) 0.002 0.014 0.382
      Indirect pathway from DASS stress to ASQ via negative coparenting −0.001 (−0.004, 0.002) 0.001 −0.008 0.523

      Confidence intervals and standard errors based on 10,000 bootstrap samples (N = 350).

      Discussion

      The overall aim of the present study was to cross-sectionally investigate the variables involved in predicting child outcomes in early childhood. The specific aims of the present study were to investigate relationships between parental mental health, parental reflective functioning, coparenting and child social emotional development in both mothers and fathers during early childhood. Surprisingly, the present study found that both general reflective functioning and parental symptoms of depression, anxiety and stress were not significant predictors of child social emotional (SE) development. However, in line with our expectations both coparenting (positive) and parental reflective functioning (in particular Pre-mentalizing and Certainty) were found to significantly predict child SE development.

      As anticipated, general reflective functioning (uncertainty subscale only) predicted symptoms of depression and anxiety, while parental depression and anxiety were both predictors of coparenting (anxiety predicted negative coparenting only). General reflective functioning (certainty only) was also found to predict coparenting. Coparenting in turn was found to predict the parental reflective functioning (positive coparenting predicted PRFQ Pre-mentalizing, while negative coparenting predicted PRFQ Interest and Curiosity). Interestingly, parental reflective functioning was not predicted by parental depression and anxiety in the present study, but was predicted by general reflective functioning.

      Given the pattern of findings that were identified, in conjunction with some preliminary suggestions in further research, some exploratory tests of indirect associations were carried out. Of note, we found an indirect effect of general reflective functioning (certainty) on child SE development via parental reflective functioning (Pre-mentalizing). We also found an indirect effect of negative coparenting on child SE development via parental reflective functioning (Pre-mentalizing). We did not however find any indirect effects between depression and anxiety, coparenting and child SE development. The current results support a growing body of research highlighting the important role reflective functioning plays in child development and wellbeing as well as parental mental health and the interparental relationship.

      The significant relationship found in our study between parental reflective functioning and child SE development was anticipated given prior research demonstrating links between higher maternal and paternal reflective functioning and better social emotional adjustment in children (Gordo et al., 2020; Salo et al., 2021). In particular, we found that higher scores on the pre-mentalizing subscale of the PRFQ were associated with poorer SE development. This makes sense given that increased levels of pre-mentalizing modes in caregivers are indicative of severe mentalizing difficulties (Luyten et al., 2017). This is often displayed as high levels of certainty about a child’s mental state which may cause parents to attribute false malevolent intentions to a child’s difficult behaviors (e.g., “my child cries around strangers to embarrass me”; Luyten et al., 2017). These parents may also have difficulty understanding their child’s internal world (Luyten et al., 2017).

      Interestingly, in the present study greater certainty about mental states (as shown by the Certainty subscale of the PRFQ) was linked with fewer social emotional symptoms. It is important to note that very high levels of certainty about mental states may suggest intrusive mentalizing (also known as hypermentalizing), whereby the parent does not recognize that it is not possible to fully comprehend the mental states of others (e.g., mental states are opaque) while very low levels of certainty may indicate hypomentalizing (a very poor understanding of one’s child’s mental states; Luyten et al., 2017). Therefore, better parental reflective functioning would be shown by scores in the mid-range on this subscale of the PRFQ. Given that parental reflective functioning is thought to be essential to children developing both emotion regulation skills and a secure parent-infant attachment relationship (Ordway et al., 2015) our results are overall in line with expectations based on what has been shown in the literature.

      The significant relationship found between higher levels of positive coparenting and better child SE development (b = 0.003, p = 0.043) was also anticipated given the large body of research linking coparenting with later child adjustment (Teubert and Pinquart, 2010; Umemura et al., 2015). This is thought to be because better coparenting is a key predictor of overall family functioning, and may lead to reduced interparental conflict and stress and more consistent and sensitive parenting (Feinberg et al., 2010; Dollberg et al., 2021).

      Based on prior research, we also hypothesized that reflective functioning would be a key variable involved in predicting coparenting, and this was found to be the case. In particular, higher levels of certainty about mental states were linked with more positive coparenting and less negative coparenting. This is unsurprising given prior research which has found associations between higher reflective functioning and better coparenting quality (Jessee, 2012; Marcu et al., 2016; Shai et al., 2017; Borelli et al., 2021; Holtzinger, 2021). It is thought that higher levels of reflective functioning should enable increased understanding of a spouse’s emotional experience and perspective, which in turn may assist couples to better manage conflict and repair ruptures in their relationship (Jessee et al., 2018).

      We also reasoned that having a strong coparenting relationship may support the development of parental reflective functioning, and this was again supported in our results. We found that more positive coparenting predicted fewer mentalizing difficulties as shown through lower levels of pre-mentalizing modes, while more negative coparenting predicted less interest and curiosity about their infant’s internal world. It makes sense that this reciprocal relationship would exist between coparenting and reflective functioning, whereby strong reflective capacity enhances one’s ability to work well in a parenting team and that in turn supports more ability to be reflective about a child’s internal world.

      Surprisingly, in the present study, parental symptoms of depression, anxiety and stress were not significant predictors of child SE development. This was unexpected given the large body of research that has previously shown associations between parental depression and anxiety and child outcomes (McCall-Hosenfeld et al., 2016). We hypothesize that this may be because previous studies examining parental mental health as a predictor of child outcomes have not also considered other significant predictors such as reflective functioning and coparenting which both explain a higher percentage of the variance in child SE development. This would make sense, given the statistically significant bivariate associations observed between parental depression and anxiety and children’s SE development. As anticipated, our community sample had generally low levels of parental depression and anxiety. In fact, 75.4% of our sample were considered to have normal to mild symptoms of depression (60.4% of these fell in the normal range), while 82.6% of participants had normal to mild symptoms of anxiety (69.7% fell in the normal range). Parents in our sample also predominantly self-reported that their children had few SE difficulties. It is therefore possible that the low-risk nature of our sample reduced our capacity to pick up on the relationship between parental mental health and child SE development. It is therefore likely that these variables remain relevant, but may be less important as predictors in a general community sample when exampled alongside other important predictor variables (Figure 1).

      Relationship between parental depression and anxiety, reflective functioning, parental reflective functioning, coparenting and child social emotional symptoms. RFQ, reflective functioning questionnaire; DASS, depression, anxiety and stress scale; PRF-Q, parental reflective functioning questionnaire; ASQ-SE, ages and stages questionnaire—social emotional.

      In line with our expectations, we did find that poorer general reflective functioning (as demonstrated by higher levels of uncertainty about mental states) predicted greater symptoms of depression and anxiety. This is consistent with a body of research demonstrating general associations between poorer reflective functioning and higher levels of depression and anxiety (Fischer-Kern et al., 2013; Belvederi Murri et al., 2017; Bigelow et al., 2018; Wendelboe et al., 2021). This is thought to be because biases in reflective processes are thought to prevent an individual from regulating their intense emotional experiences or modulating the behavioral expression of these emotions (Bouchard et al., 2008; Luyten et al., 2013; Luyten and Fonagy, 2018). In turn, we also hypothesized that parental mental health may act as a predictor for parental reflective functioning, whereby it is easier to reflect on your child’s inner world when your own mental health is stronger. However, once all variables were entered into our final model, this relationship was no longer significant. It may be that this relationship does not show up with the self-report measures used in the present study, or it could be the case that other variables such as emotion regulation (Schultheis et al., 2019), and attachment history (Suchman et al., 2011) play a larger role in predicting parental reflective functioning.

      Parental depression and anxiety were also found to predict coparenting such that higher levels of parental depression were associated with less positive and more negative coparenting, while higher levels of parental anxiety were associated with more negative coparenting. This is in line with a body of research suggesting that parental depression and anxiety negatively impact the coparenting relationship (Price-Robertson et al., 2017; Tissot et al., 2017; Metz et al., 2018; Williams, 2018; Turney and Hardie, 2021). This makes sense given that both executive functioning and reflective capacity are so impaired by poor mental health, and these factors make it harder to see a partner’s perspective and work well as a parenting team.

      We also found that parental reflective functioning was predicted by general reflective functioning such that higher levels of RFQ uncertainty and lower levels of RFQ certainty predicted increased scores on the PRFQ pre-mentalizing modes. This makes sense given that high levels of pre-mentalizing modes are indicative of a lack of reflective capacity, in the same way that very high uncertainty and low certainty may indicate difficulties with mentalizing (Luyten et al., 2017). We also found that higher levels of RFQ uncertainty predicted lower scores on the PRFQ certainty subscale, which once again makes conceptual sense. Finally, increased RFQ uncertainty predicted less PRFQ interest and curiosity, while more RFQ certainty predicted greater PRFQ interest and curiosity. High levels of interest and curiosity are suggestive of greater reflective capacity, and as such this finding is in line with what we would expect to see. Given that most prior studies that have examined reflective functioning or parental reflective functioning have done so using observational or interview measures, few studies have examined how the RFQ and PRFQ are related among parents of young children. However, these results are all in the expected direction and make sense from a conceptual perspective.

      In the present study we also carried out some exploratory mediation analyses, and found an indirect effect of general reflective functioning (certainty) on child SE development via parental reflective functioning (Pre-mentalizing). We found that greater certainty about mental states was associated with lower pre-mentalizing modes, which in turn was associated with better child SE development. General reflective functioning was not found to be a significant predictor of child SE development, however this is likely because the relationships between general reflective functioning and child SE development is fully explained by parental reflective functioning.

      Given prior research suggesting that coparenting may act a mediator for the relationships between anxiety and depression and child outcomes (Tissot et al., 2016; Metz et al., 2018), we explored whether this would be the case in the present study. However, we did not find any evidence of an indirect effect of parental mental health on child SE development via coparenting. This may be because neither parental depression, anxiety or coparenting were strong predictors of child SE development once entered into our complete model, and therefore these relationships may have been overshadowed by stronger predictor variables. Or it could be the nature of the self-report measures included in the current study and the fact that on the whole our community sample had generally low levels of parental depression and anxiety as well as child SE difficulties, which may have reduced our ability to detect this relationship.

      Unlike León and Olhaberry (2020) we also did not find an indirect effect of general reflective functioning on child SE development via coparenting, however given the exploratory nature of this part of our analysis we also considered some alternate pathways. In doing so, we found an indirect effect of negative coparenting on child SE development via parental reflective functioning (Pre-mentalizing). This effect is such that more negative coparenting predicted higher pre-mentalizing modes, which in turn was associated with worse child SE development. This makes sense given the likely reciprocal relationship between reflective functioning and coparenting, whereby the presence of a strong parenting team is likely to support stronger reflective capacity, especially in the context of parenting. We found that negative coparenting was not a significant predictor of child SE development, and once again, this is likely because the relationship between negative coparenting and child SE development is fully explained by parental reflective functioning, which overall has shown up in our study as the strongest predictor of child development.

      Strengths, limitations, and future directions

      Our study is strengthened by our inclusion of both mothers and fathers, and an adequately-sized sample that allowed us to examine a range of key variables (parental mental health, coparenting, both general and parental reflective functioning) that are thought to predict child SE development. Nevertheless, our findings do need to be considered in light of several limitations. Firstly, the cross-sectional nature of this data prevents us from drawing causal inferences between study variables. The order in which we tested our variables was informed by prior literature and theoretical considerations, however these analyses alone are unable to make an inference of causality. For example, we argue that poor general reflective functioning may lead to increased risk of experiencing depression and anxiety, however there is also evidence suggesting that while experiencing depression and anxiety, an individual’s reflective processes are impeded (Luyten et al., 2013; Luyten and Fonagy, 2018). The same is true for the relationship between depression and anxiety and coparenting. We argue that poor mental health is likely to lead to a worse coparenting relationship, however there is also a body of research suggesting that coparental conflict may predict declining mental health (Cabrera et al., 2012). Given these considerations, we recognize that causal inferences cannot be drawn solely from this cross-sectional data. However, we hope that the findings presented in this paper will inform future more resource intensive longitudinal studies.

      Another limitation within our study is our sole reliance on self-report measures for all study variables. In particular, coparenting, reflective functioning and child SE development are likely to be more accurately measured via observational tasks. This is because parents may lack the insight to answer accurately, or may attempt to portray a more favorable image of themselves and their coparental and parent–child relationships. Future research examining the relationship between these variables would benefit from including additional methods of data collection such as behavioral observation or interviews. Our data is also limited by the fact that while we included both fathers and mothers, we did not recruit couples and therefore we are limited in the inferences we can draw about how one parent’s reflective functioning may influence the other parent and in turn did not have an additional source of data on either the coparenting relationship or child SE development (i.e., the other parent may view the coparenting relationship or child’s level of development differently).

      Our study also recruited participants from Western countries with a majority of participants identifying as Caucasian, thus some caution should be applied when attempting to generalize these findings into other cultural settings. Future research may wish to consider investigating how coparenting and reflective functioning relate to child SE development in different cultural contexts, given prior research establishing cultural differences in child care practices (Chen et al., 1998; Rosenthal and Roer-Strier, 2001).

      Finally, the predictors examined in the present study explained only 18.7% of the variance in child SE development, which is a relatively small proportion of variance. This leaves 81.3% of the variance unexplained by the predictors considered in this study. This would suggest that numerous other variables are involved in predicting child outcomes, and future research may wish to consider additional factors that may be important to social emotional development in young children. In particular it may be important to consider variables such as the social support available, maternal and paternal attachment style, level of parental self-efficacy and stress as well as parental self-compassion.

      Implications

      This study adds to a small but growing body of research investigating how both coparenting and reflective functioning interact to predict child outcomes. We are one of the first studies to demonstrate that reflective functioning is a key predictor of the coparenting relationship. We are also one of the first studies to consider how parental mental health fits into this picture. Parental mental health, and maternal depression in particular, has long been considered a key risk factor for the development of adverse child outcomes, and therefore targeting maternal depression has been a key focus of many public health initiatives during the perinatal period. Our results appear to suggest that parental reflective functioning is one of the most important predictors of child outcomes over and above parental mental health. Current interventions designed to improve parental reflective functioning, both group-based and dyadic, are still being refined and there is limited evidence for their effectiveness (Barlow et al., 2021; Lo and Wong, 2022). The findings of the current study support the continued development of these interventions as they indicate changes in parental reflective functioning may contribute to changes in child outcomes.

      Our findings suggest that parental reflective functioning appears to play a large role in developing both a strong coparenting relationship and also supporting child social emotional development. Therefore, we hope these findings will inform future research and enable the continued development of early interventions for new parents that specifically target their reflective capacity. Targeting reflective functioning is likely to in turn reduce symptoms of poor mental health, improve coparenting and general family functioning and most importantly enable optimal social emotional development in infants and young children.

      Data availability statement

      The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

      Ethics statement

      The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Curtin Human Research Ethics Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

      Author contributions

      MD conceptualized and carried out the research project, including the selection of study variables, data collection, and data analysis and was the principal author of this publication. RR, EI, VM, and RK were the supervisors of the study, assisting in designing and conducting the research, and providing feedback on the publication. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

      Conflict of interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Publisher’s note

      All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

      References Australian Bureau of Statistics . (2020-21). National Study of Mental Health and Wellbeing. ABS. Available at: https://www.abs.gov.au/statistics/health/mental-health/national-study-mental-health-and-wellbeing/2020-21 Barlow J. Sleed M. Midgley N. (2021). Enhancing parental reflective functioning through early dyadic interventions: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Infant Ment. Health J. 42, 2134. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21896, PMID: 33210359 Belvederi Murri M. Ferrigno G. Penati S. Muzio C. Piccinini G. Innamorati M. . (2017). Mentalization and depressive symptoms in a clinical sample of adolescents and young adults. Child Adolesc. Mental Health 22, 6976. doi: 10.1111/camh.12195, PMID: 32680319 Benbassat N. Priel B. (2012). Parenting and adolescent adjustment: the role of parental reflective function. J. Adolesc. 35, 163174. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.03.004, PMID: 21497896 Bigelow A. E. Beebe B. Power M. Stafford A.-L. Ewing J. Egleson A. . (2018). Longitudinal relations among maternal depressive symptoms, maternal mind-mindedness, and infant attachment behavior. Infant Behav. Dev. 51, 3344. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2018.02.006, PMID: 29567547 Boldt L. J. Kochanska G. Yoon J. E. Koenig Nordling J. (2014). Children’s attachment to both parents from toddler age to middle childhood: links to adaptive and maladaptive outcomes. Attach. Hum. Dev. 16, 211229. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2014.889181, PMID: 24605850 Borelli J. L. Ensink K. Gillespie M. L. Falasiri E. Bernazzani O. Fonagy P. . (2021). Mothers’ self-focused reflective functioning interacts with childhood experiences of rejection to predict current romantic relationship quality and parenting behavior. Fam. Process 60, 920934. doi: 10.1111/famp.12603, PMID: 33026653 Boričević Maršanić V. Kušmić E. (2013). Coparenting within the family system: review of literature. Coll. Antropol. 37, 13791384. Bouchard M.-A. Target M. Lecours S. Fonagy P. Tremblay L.-M. Schachter A. . (2008). Mentalization in adult attachment narratives: reflective functioning, mental states, and affect elaboration compared. Psychoanal. Psychol. 25, 4766. doi: 10.1037/0736-9735.25.1.47 Cabrera N. J. Scott M. Fagan J. Steward-Streng N. Chien N. (2012). Coparenting and children's school readiness: a mediational model. Fam. Process 51, 307324. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2012.01408.x, PMID: 22984971 Camoirano A. (2017). Mentalizing makes parenting work: a review about parental reflective functioning and clinical interventions to improve it. Front. Psychol. 8:14. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00014, PMID: 28163690 Chen X. Hastings P. D. Rubin K. H. Chen H. Cen G. Stewart S. L. (1998). Child-rearing attitudes and behavioural inhibition in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: a cross-cultural study. Dev. Psychol. 34, 677686. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.4.677, PMID: 9681259 Cheng S. Maeda T. Tomiwa K. Yamakawa N. Koeda T. Kawai M. . (2009). Contribution of parenting factors to the developmental attainment of 9-month-old infants: results from the Japan Children’s study. J. Epidemiol. 19, 319327. doi: 10.2188/jea.JE20081014, PMID: 19776496 Colonnesi C. Zeegers M. A. Majdandžić M. van Steensel F. J. Bögels S. M. (2019). Fathers’ and mothers’ early mind-mindedness predicts social competence and behavior problems in childhood. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 47, 14211435. doi: 10.1007/s10802-019-00537-2, PMID: 30929182 Cucchi A. Hampton J. A. Moulton-Perkins A. (2018). Using the validated reflective functioning questionnaire to investigate mentalizing in individuals presenting with eating disorders with and without self-harm. PeerJ 6:e5756. doi: 10.7717/peerj.5756, PMID: 30397541 Davis E. P. Sandman C. A. (2010). The timing of prenatal exposure to maternal cortisol and psychosocial stress is associated with human infant cognitive development. Child Dev. 81, 131148. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01385.x, PMID: 20331658 Dollberg D. G. Hanetz Gamliel K. Levy S. (2021). Mediating and moderating links between coparenting, parental mentalization, parents’ anxiety, and children’s behavior problems. J. Fam. Psychol. 35, 324334. doi: 10.1037/fam0000728, PMID: 32525331 Dopkins Stright A. Neitzel C. (2003). Beyond parenting: Coparenting and children's classroom adjustment. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 27, 3140. doi: 10.1080/01650250143000580 Feinberg M. E. (2003). The internal structure and ecological context of Coparenting: a framework for research and intervention. Parenting 3, 95131. doi: 10.1207/S15327922PAR0302_01, PMID: 21980259 Feinberg M. E. Brown L. D. Kan M. L. (2012). A multi-domain self-report measure of coparenting. Parenting 12, 121. doi: 10.1080/15295192.2012.638870, PMID: 23166477 Feinberg M. E. Jones D. E. Hostetler M. L. Roettger M. E. Paul I. M. Ehrenthal D. B. (2016). Couple-focused prevention at the transition to parenthood, a randomized trial: effects on coparenting, parenting, family violence, and parent and child adjustment. Prev. Sci. 17, 751764. doi: 10.1007/s11121-016-0674-z, PMID: 27334116 Feinberg M. E. Jones D. E. Kan M. L. Goslin M. C. (2010). Effects of family foundations on parents and children: 3.5 years after baseline. J. Fam. Psychol. 24, 532542. doi: 10.1037/a0020837, PMID: 20954763 Feinberg M. E. Kan M. L. (2008). Establishing family foundations: intervention effects on coparenting, parent/infant well-being, and parent-child relations. J. Fam. Psychol. 22, 253263. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.22.2.253, PMID: 18410212 Fischer-Kern M. Fonagy P. Kapusta N. D. Luyten P. Boss S. Naderer A. . (2013). Mentalizing in female inpatients with major depressive disorder. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 201, 202207. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182845c0a Fonagy P. Luyten P. Moulton-Perkins A. Lee Y.-W. Warren F. Howard S. . (2016). Development and validation of a self-report measure of mentalizing: The reflective functioning questionnaire. PloS one 11:e0158678. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0158678 Fritz M. S. MacKinnon D. P. (2007). Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychol. Sci. 18, 233239. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01882.x, PMID: 17444920 Funk J. L. Rogge R. D. (2007). Testing the ruler with item response theory: increasing precision of measurement for relationship satisfaction with the couples satisfaction index. J. Fam. Psychol. 21, 572583. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.4.572, PMID: 18179329 Gordo L. Martínez-Pampliega A. Iriarte Elejalde L. Luyten P. (2020). Do parental reflective functioning and parental competence affect the socioemotional adjustment of children? J. Child Fam. Stud. 29, 36213631. doi: 10.1007/s10826-020-01840-z Halle T. G. Darling-Churchill K. E. (2016). Review of measures of social and emotional development. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 45, 818. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2016.02.003 Hayden M. C. Müllauer P. K. Gaugeler R. Senft B. Andreas S. (2018). Improvements in mentalization predict improvements in interpersonal distress in patients with mental disorders. J. Clin. Psychol. 74, 22762286. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22673, PMID: 29998458 Hayes A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. Guilford publications. Hernández-Martínez C. Arija V. Balaguer A. Cavallé P. Canals J. (2008). Do the emotional states of pregnant women affect neonatal behaviour? Early Hum. Dev. 84, 745750. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2008.05.002 Holtzinger C. M. (2021). The relationship between the reflective function and the Coparenting Alliance. Doctor of Philosophy. Palo Alto University, Available at: https://www.proquest.com/docview/2564508866/fulltextPDF/37EBDA3F808941D0PQ/1?accountid=10382 Huang Y. (2021). The association between parental attachment and the parenting: a review and preliminary meta-analysis. Psychol. Thought 14, 339362. doi: 10.37708/psyct.v14i2.596 Ip P. Li T. M. Chan K. L. Ting A. Y. Y. Chan C. Y. Koh Y. W. . (2018). Associations of paternal postpartum depressive symptoms and infant development in a Chinese longitudinal study. Infant Behav. Dev. 53, 8189. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2018.08.002, PMID: 30213511 Jessee A. (2012). The role of reflective functioning in predicting parenting and coparenting quality. Doctor of Philosophy. University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana-Champaign. Available at: http://hdl.handle.net/2142/34348 Jessee A. Mangelsdorf S. C. Wong M. S. Schoppe-Sullivan S. J. Shigeto A. Brown G. L. (2018). The role of reflective functioning in predicting marital and coparenting quality. J. Child Fam. Stud. 27, 187197. doi: 10.1007/s10826-017-0874-6 Kane P. Garber J. (2004). The relations among depression in fathers, children's psychopathology, and father–child conflict: a meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 24, 339360. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2004.03.004, PMID: 15245835 Kelly K. Slade A. Grienenberger J. F. (2005). Maternal reflective functioning, mother–infant affective communication, and infant attachment: exploring the link between mental states and observed caregiving behavior in the intergenerational transmission of attachment. Attach. Hum. Dev. 7, 299311. doi: 10.1080/14616730500245963, PMID: 16210241 Kingston D. Tough S. (2014). Prenatal and postnatal maternal mental health and school-age child development: a systematic review. Matern. Child Health J. 18, 17281741. doi: 10.1007/s10995-013-1418-3, PMID: 24352625 Kingston D. Tough S. Whitfield H. (2012). Prenatal and postpartum maternal psychological distress and infant development: a systematic review. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 43, 683714. doi: 10.1007/s10578-012-0291-4, PMID: 22407278 Lavoie M.-A. Battaglia M. Achim A. M. (2014). A meta-analysis and scoping review of social cognition performance in social phobia, posttraumatic stress disorder and other anxiety disorders. J. Anxiety Disord. 28, 169177. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.09.005, PMID: 24239443 Le Y. McDaniel B. T. Leavitt C. E. Feinberg M. E. (2016). Longitudinal associations between relationship quality and coparenting across the transition to parenthood: a dyadic perspective. J. Fam. Psychol. 30, 918926. doi: 10.1037/fam0000217, PMID: 27183188 León M. J. Olhaberry M. (2020). Triadic interactions, parental reflective functioning, and early social-emotional difficulties. Infant Ment. Health J. 41, 431444. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21844, PMID: 32057127 LeRoy M. Mahoney A. Pargament K. I. DeMaris A. (2013). Longitudinal links between early coparenting and infant behaviour problems. Early Child Dev. Care 183, 360377. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2012.711588 Lilja G. Edhborg M. Nissen E. (2012). Depressive mood in women at childbirth predicts their mood and relationship with infant and partner during the first year postpartum. Scand. J. Caring Sci. 26, 245253. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6712.2011.00925.x, PMID: 21950600 Lo C. K. M. Wong S. Y. (2022). The effectiveness of parenting programs in regard to improving parental reflective functioning: a meta-analysis. Attach. Hum. Dev. 24, 7692. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2020.1844247, PMID: 33143556 Lovibond P. F. Lovibond S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behav. Res. Ther. 33, 335343. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U Luyten P. Blatt S. J. Fonagy P. (2013). Impairments in self structures in depression and suicide in psychodynamic and cognitive behavioral approaches: implications for clinical practice and research. Int. J. Cogn. Ther. 6, 265279. doi: 10.1521/ijct.2013.6.3.265 Luyten P. Fonagy P. (2018). The stress–reward–mentalizing model of depression: an integrative developmental cascade approach to child and adolescent depressive disorder based on the research domain criteria (RDoC) approach. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 64, 8798. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.09.008, PMID: 29107398 Luyten P. Mayes L. C. Nijssens L. Fonagy P. (2017). The parental reflective functioning questionnaire: development and preliminary validation. PLoS One 12:e0176218. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0176218, PMID: 28472162 Lyons-Ruth K. Todd Manly J. Von Klitzing K. Tamminen T. Emde R. Fitzgerald H. . (2017). The worldwide burden of infant mental and emotional disorder: report of the task force of the world association for infant mental health. Infant Ment. Health J. 38, 695705. doi: 10.1002/imhj, PMID: 29088514 Marcu I. Oppenheim D. Koren-Karie N. (2016). Parental insightfulness is associated with cooperative interactions in families with toddlers. J. Fam. Psychol. 30, 927934. doi: 10.1037/fam0000240, PMID: 27559929 Matijasevich A. Murray J. Cooper P. J. Anselmi L. Barros A. J. Barros F. C. . (2015). Trajectories of maternal depression and offspring psychopathology at 6 years: 2004 Pelotas cohort study. J. Affect. Disord. 174, 424431. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.12.012, PMID: 25553403 McCall-Hosenfeld J. S. Phiri K. Schaefer E. Zhu J. Kjerulff K. (2016). Trajectories of depressive symptoms throughout the peri-and postpartum period: results from the first baby study. J. Womens Health 25, 11121121. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2015.5310, PMID: 27310295 Metz M. Majdandžić M. Bögels S. (2018). Concurrent and predictive associations between infants’ and toddlers’ fearful temperament, coparenting, and parental anxiety disorders. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 47, 569580. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2015.1121823, PMID: 26909851 Moore T. Arefadib N. Deery A. West S. (2017). The first thousand days: an evidence paper. Centre for Community Child Health, Murdoch Children's research institute: Parkeville. Victoria. Murray L. Arteche A. Fearon P. Halligan S. Goodyer I. Cooper P. (2011). Maternal postnatal depression and the development of depression in offspring up to 16 years of age. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 50, 460470. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.02.001, PMID: 21515195 Nandy A. Nixon E. Quigley J. (2021). Observed and reported coparenting and toddlers' adaptive functioning. Infant Child Dev. 30:e2226. doi: 10.1002/icd.2226 Newland L. A. (2015). Family well‐being, parenting, and child well‐being: Pathways to healthy adjustment. Clin. Psychol. 19, 314. doi: 10.1111/cp.12059 Nijssens L. Vliegen N. Luyten P. (2020). The mediating role of parental reflective functioning in child social–emotional development. J. Child Fam. Stud. 29, 23422354. doi: 10.1007/s10826-020-01767-5 Nyström K. Öhrling K. (2004). Parenthood experiences during the child's first year: literature review. J. Adv. Nurs. 46, 319330. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2004.02991.x O'Neill M. C. Badovinac S. Pillai Riddell R. Bureau J. -F. Rumeo C. Costa S. (2021). The longitudinal and concurrent relationship between caregiver sensitivity and preschool attachment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One 16:e0245061. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0245061, PMID: 33481826 Ordway M. R. Webb D. Sadler L. S. Slade A. (2015). Parental reflective functioning: An approach to enhancing parent-child relationships in pediatric primary care. J. Pediatr. Health Care 29, 325334. doi: 10.1016/j.pedhc.2014.12.002 Osman A. Wong J. L. Bagge C. L. Freedenthal S. Gutierrez P. M. Lozano G. (2012). The depression anxiety stress scales—21 (DASS-21): further examination of dimensions, scale reliability, and correlates. J. Clin. Psychol. 68, 13221338. doi: 10.1002/jclp.21908, PMID: 22930477 Parfitt Y. Pike A. Ayers S. (2013). The impact of parents’ mental health on parent–baby interaction: a prospective study. Infant Behav. Dev. 36, 599608. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.06.003, PMID: 23850989 Pearson R. M. Evans J. Kounali D. Lewis G. Heron J. Ramchandani P. G. . (2013). Maternal depression during pregnancy and the postnatal period: risks and possible mechanisms for offspring depression at age 18 years. JAMA Psychiat. 70, 13121319. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.2163, PMID: 24108418 Price-Robertson R. Baxter J. Mathews S. (2017). Longitudinal associations between fathers' mental health and the quality of their coparenting relationships. Clin. Psychol. 21, 215226. doi: 10.1111/cp.12072 Ranson K. E. Urichuk L. J. (2008). The effect of parent–child attachment relationships on child biopsychosocial outcomes: a review. Early Child Dev. Care 178, 129152. doi: 10.1080/03004430600685282 R Core Team . (2020). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R foundation for statistical computing. Rosenthal M. K. Roer-Strier D. (2001). Cultural differences in mothers' developmental goals and ethnotheories. Int. J. Pscyhol. 36, 2031. doi: 10.1080/00207590042000029 Rosseel Y. (2012). lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. J. Stat. Softw. 48,136. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02 Salo S. J. Pajulo M. Vinzce L. Raittila S. Sourander J. Kalland M. (2021). Parent relationship satisfaction and reflective functioning as predictors of emotional availability and infant behavior. J. Child Fam. Stud. 30, 12141228. doi: 10.1007/s10826-021-01934-2 Schultheis A. M. Mayes L. C. Rutherford H. J. V. (2019). Associations between emotion regulation and parental reflective functioning. J. Child Fam. Stud. 28, 10941104. doi: 10.1007/s10826-018-01326-z, PMID: 31156323 Scrimgeour M. B. Blandon A. Y. Stifter C. A. Buss K. A. (2013). Cooperative coparenting moderates the association between parenting practices and children’s prosocial behavior. J. Fam. Psychol. 27, 506511. doi: 10.1037/a0032893, PMID: 23750531 Sette G. Coppola G. Cassibba R. (2015). The transmission of attachment across generations: the state of art and new theoretical perspectives. Scand. J. Psychol. 56, 315326. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12212, PMID: 25810347 Shai D. (2019). The inconsolable doll task: prenatal coparenting behavioral dynamics under stress predicting child cognitive development at 18 months. Infant Behav. Dev. 56:101254. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2018.04.003, PMID: 29853155 Shai D. Dollberg D. Szepsenwol O. (2017). The importance of parental verbal and embodied mentalizing in shaping parental experiences of stress and coparenting. Infant Behav. Dev. 49, 8796. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.08.003, PMID: 28818676 Slade A. (2005). Parental reflective functioning: An introduction. Attach. Hum. Dev. 7, 269281. doi: 10.1080/14616730500245906 Squires J. Bricker D. Heo K. Twombly E. (2001). Identification of social-emotional problems in young children using a parent-completed screening measure. Early Child Res. Q. 16, 405419. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2006(01)00115-6 Stacks A. M. Muzik M. Wong K. Beeghly M. Huth-Bocks A. Irwin J. L. . (2014). Maternal reflective functioning among mothers with childhood maltreatment histories: Links to sensitive parenting and infant attachment security. Attach. Hum. Dev. 16, 515533. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2014.935452 Steele H. Steele M. (2008). “On the origins of reflective functioning” in Mentalization: Theoretical considerations, research findings, and clinical implications. ed. Busch F. N. , vol. 29 (New York, NY: Taylor & Francis Group), 133158. Suchman N. E. Decoste C. Mcmahon T. J. Rounsaville B. Mayes L. (2011). The mothers and toddlers program, an attachment-based parenting intervention for substance-using women: results at 6-week follow-up in a randomized clinical pilot. Infant Ment. Health J. 32, 427449. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20303, PMID: 22685361 Teubert D. Pinquart M. (2010). The association between coparenting and child adjustment: a meta-analysis. Parent. Sci. Pract. 10, 286307. doi: 10.1080/15295192.2010.492040 Tissot H. Favez N. Frascarolo F. Despland J.-N. (2016). Coparenting behaviors as mediators between postpartum parental depressive symptoms and toddler’s symptoms. Front. Psychol. 7:1912. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01912, PMID: 28018259 Tissot H. Favez N. Ghisletta P. Frascarolo F. Despland J. N. (2017). A longitudinal study of parental depressive symptoms and coparenting in the first 18 months. Fam. Process 56, 445458. doi: 10.1111/famp.12213, PMID: 27062426 Turney K. Hardie J. H. (2021). The repercussions of parental depression for perceptions of coparental cooperation. J. Marriage Fam. 83, 466481. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12708 Umemura T. Christopher C. Mann T. Jacobvitz D. Hazen N. (2015). Coparenting problems with toddlers predict children’s symptoms of psychological problems at age 7. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 46, 981996. doi: 10.1007/s10578-015-0536-0, PMID: 25663037 van Ijzendoorn M. H. (1995). Adult attachment representations, parental responsiveness, and infant attachment: a meta-analysis on the predictive validity of the adult attachment interview. Psychol. Bull. 117, 387403. PMID: 7777645 Verbeek T. Bockting C. L. van Pampus M. G. Ormel J. Meijer J. L. Hartman C. A. . (2012). Postpartum depression predicts offspring mental health problems in adolescence independently of parental lifetime psychopathology. J. Affect. Disord. 136, 948954. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.08.035, PMID: 21930302 Vismara L. Sechi C. Lucarelli L. (2021). Reflective function in first-time mothers and fathers: association with infant temperament and parenting stress. Eur. J. Trauma Dissoc 5:100147. doi: 10.1016/j.ejtd.2020.100147 Wendelboe K. I. Smith-Nielsen J. Stuart A. C. Luyten P. Skovgaard Væver M. (2021). Factor structure of the parental reflective functioning questionnaire and association with maternal postpartum depression and comorbid symptoms of psychopathology. PLoS One 16:e0254792. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0254792, PMID: 34339422 Werner E. A. Myers M. M. Fifer W. P. Cheng B. Fang Y. Allen R. . (2007). Prenatal predictors of infant temperament. Dev. Psychobiol. 49, 474484. doi: 10.1002/dev.20232 Williams D. T. (2018). Parental depression and cooperative coparenting: a longitudinal and dyadic approach. Fam. Relat. 67, 253269. doi: 10.1111/fare.12308, PMID: 29887656 Yap M. B. H. Pilkington P. D. Ryan S. M. Jorm A. F. (2014). Parental factors associated with depression and anxiety in young people: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 156, 823. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.11.007, PMID: 24308895
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016www.eryao.net.cn
      mbservice.com.cn
      www.jdzhdwy.org.cn
      www.himokids.com.cn
      mhchain.com.cn
      www.e-ting.net.cn
      www.szicif.com.cn
      qhchain.com.cn
      weida888.net.cn
      wzfc0577.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p