Front. Psychol. Frontiers in Psychology Front. Psychol. 1664-1078 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.606249 Psychology Brief Research Report The Moderating Role of Education on the Relationship Between Perceived Stereotype Threat and False Memory in Aging Gilet Anne-Laure 1 * Evrard Christelle 1 Galharret Jean-Michel 2 Colombel Fabienne 1 1 Laboratoire de Psychologie des Pays de la Loire (LPPL – EA 4638), Université de Nantes, Nantes, France 2 Laboratoire de Mathématiques Jean Leray (LMJL-UMR 6629), Université de Nantes, Nantes, France

Edited by: Hannah J. Swift, University of Kent, United Kingdom

Reviewed by: Sara N. Gallant, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, United States; Tara Patterson, University of California Los Angeles Life Sciences, United States

*Correspondence: Anne-Laure Gilet, anne-laure.gilet@univ-nantes.fr

This article was submitted to Organizational Psychology, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology

12 01 2021 2020 11 606249 14 09 2020 11 12 2020 Copyright © 2021 Gilet, Evrard, Galharret and Colombel. 2021 Gilet, Evrard, Galharret and Colombel

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Studies regularly show that an age-based stereotype threat impairs older adults’ performance on memory tasks. Results regarding stereotype threat effects on false memories are less clear. Some studies suggest that education may moderate the relationship between an age-related stereotype threat and episodic memory performance in older adults. The present study aimed at examining the moderating role of education on the relationship between perceived stereotype threat (PST) and false memories in older adults. With this aim, 82 adults between 60 and 70 years of age performed a Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) task followed by a free recall test and completed questionnaires assessing both their perception of an age-based stereotype threat and their education level. Regression analyses showed no effect of PST on the production of critical lures. However, as was expected, our results showed that in higher educated older adults, as the perception of stereotype increases, the production of critical lures increases. These results confirm the moderating role of education and highlight its key role in the relationship between the age-based stereotype threat and older adults’ susceptibility to false memories.

memory aging DRM false memories stereotype threat

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      Age-related stereotypes are very present in our environment and our daily lives. Research shows that both young and older adults share many positive and negative perceptions and stereotypes about aging (e.g., Hummert et al., 1994; Grühn et al., 2011). However, people’s representations of older adults are more imbued with negative perceptions than those of younger adults (e.g., Kite et al., 2005; Hummert, 2011). The effects of negative age-related stereotypes on older adults’ episodic memory performance have been regularly highlighted in the literature. Several studies have shown that older participants previously subliminally exposed to negative age-related stereotypes performed worse in memory tasks than older adults exposed to positive age-related stereotypes or non-exposed to age-related stereotypes (e.g., Hess et al., 2004; Levy, 2009). In other studies, researchers observed that memory performance of participants aged 60 and over was worse (and the age-related difference increased) when the memory component of the task was emphasized than when it was not (e.g., Rahhal et al., 2001; Chasteen et al., 2005; Desrichard and Köpetz, 2005). In such laboratory conditions, older adults may perceive and experience what Steele and Aronson (1995, p. 797) called the stereotype threat, i.e., “being at risk of confirming, as self-characteristic, a negative stereotype about one’s group.”

      A growing body of research has shown that an induced age-based stereotype threat impairs older adults’ performance in a large variety of cognitive tasks (e.g., Barber and Mather, 2014; Lamont et al., 2015; Barber, 2017) such as episodic memory (see Armstrong et al., 2017 for a recent meta-analysis on this topic). Several hypotheses have been proposed to explain these effects relying either on cognitive or on motivational mechanisms. According to the executive control interference hypothesis (Schmader and Johns, 2003), the stereotype threat takes up cognitive resources, thereby increasing the cognitive load placed upon the cognitive system (particularly upon working memory), which induces a performance decrease. Recent results showing a deleterious effect of an age-based stereotype threat on older adults’ associative memory (Brubaker and Naveh-Benjamin, 2018) or the use of controlled processes while strengthening the use of automatic processes (Mazerolle et al., 2012) offer support to this cognitive hypothesis. The motivational explanation is based on the regulatory focus theory (Higgins, 1997). Research suggests that when participants perform a memory task under a stereotype threat, they adopt a prevention focus, become more vigilant to errors and try to avoid committing errors as much as possible (see Barber, 2017 for an extensive presentation; Seibt and Förster, 2004; as opposed to a promotion focus leading participants to remember as many words as possible whatever the number of errors they would make). This motivational hypothesis finds support in studies showing a decrease in correct recalls and/or a decrease in memory errors (e.g., Hess et al., 2003, 2009; Barber and Mather, 2013a,b; Popham and Hess, 2015; Barber, 2017).

      One type of memory errors that is likely to be affected by an age-based stereotype threat is false memory. False memories are usually defined as distorted memories of events that occurred or memories of events that did not happen (Roediger and McDermott, 1995) and are associated with high levels of certitude. One of the most widely used paradigm to study individuals’ susceptibility to false memories is the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM; Deese, 1959; Roediger and McDermott, 1995) paradigm, in which participants study lists of semantically related words (e.g., bed, awake, tired, dream, etc.) strongly associated with a critical lure that is never presented (e.g., sleep), before being asked to recognize and/or recall as many words as possible without guessing. Participants often wrongly recall or recognize critical lures, thus making false memories. A large amount of studies has focused on factors influencing the occurrence of false memories. Research has shown that factors, such as age (e.g., Tun et al., 1998; Dehon and Brédart, 2004), positive and high arousing moods (e.g., Storbeck and Clore, 2005; Corson and Verrier, 2007; Storbeck and Clore, 2011), cognitive style (e.g., Corson et al., 2009), stress (Payne et al., 2002), the use of long rather than short DRM lists (e.g., Robinson and Roediger, 1997), or a relational rather than an item-specific processing during encoding (e.g., Hunt and Einstein, 1981), increase the occurrence of false memories.

      To our knowledge, only three studies investigated the effects of an induced age-based related stereotype threat on older adults’ propensity to false memories (Thomas and Dubois, 2011; Wong and Gallo, 2016; Smith et al., 2017), but these studies did not yield any consensual results. In these studies, participants were first presented with DRM lists, then some of them were exposed to a blatant age-based stereotype threat induction (by reading short paragraphs on age-related cognitive decline), and finally performed a recognition task. Thomas and Dubois (2011) observed that adults aged between 60 and 74 years were more inclined to falsely recognize critical lures in the age-based stereotype condition than in the control condition (reading a short text on language processing after encoding DRM lists). No significant effects of the age-based stereotype threat were found on hits. On the contrary, Wong and Gallo (2016) found that older adults (65–87 years) exposed to an age-based stereotype threat induction made fewer false alarms on the critical lures than their counterparts in the control condition who read an age-neutral text on language research. However, it is worth noting that Wong and Gallo (2016) had included a DRM warning before the participants performed the recognition task. Participants were informed that some of the words in the recognition task were strongly associated with words they had studied but that these words were not included in the initial list. Participants were then told to avoid confusion between those words and the words they had studied. As this warning may partly explain the contradictory results, and in an attempt to explain the discrepancies between Thomas and Dubois (2011) and Wong and Gallo (2016) studies, Smith et al. (2017) chose to include four experimental conditions combining the presence or the absence of an age-based stereotype threat and the presence or the absence of a DRM warning. This warning was presented before the recognition task and consisted of a short text explaining the principles of the DRM paradigm, i.e., that studied words were all related to another word that is often falsely remembered by participants. Participants aged 56–90 years thus performed a DRM associated with a recognition test in one of the four experimental conditions. Consistent with the results of Thomas and Dubois (2011), they showed that older adults exposed to negative age-related stereotypes made more false recognition of critical lures than older adults non-exposed to negative age-related stereotypes and did not replicate the results obtained in Wong and Gallo (2016). Interestingly, Smith et al. (2017) also highlighted the moderating role of several factors such as years of education and retirement status. They showed that older adults’ susceptibility to false recognitions (false memories) increases under the stereotype threat even more in retired or highly educated participants.

      Armstrong et al. (2017) also investigated the potential moderator effects of education, age, and type of memory tests. In their meta-analysis comprising studies using blatant or subtle (e.g., non-direct communication of memory decline) stereotype threat manipulation, they concluded that neither the years of education nor the age moderated the association between the age-based stereotype threat and the episodic memory performance in older adults. They also showed that the age-based stereotype threat effect was significant in free recall tasks but not in cued-recall or recognition tasks. This is consistent with the idea that, in addition to aging, a stereotype threat reduces the ability to use strategic processes leading to larger memory impairments in older adults (e.g., Craik and McDowd, 1987; Mazerolle et al., 2012).

      The objective of the present study was to investigate the moderator role of years of education on the relationship between the perceived age-based stereotype threat and the production of false memories in older adults. Unlike previous studies on false memories, we chose not to induce a stereotype threat but to assess instead the older adults’ perceived stereotype threat (PST) when performing such a DRM task. This allowed us to be closer to natural situations of memory examination that older adults encounter (e.g., Rahhal et al., 2001; Brubaker and Naveh-Benjamin, 2018). Besides, since the age-based stereotype threat is known to be higher under difficult tasks (e.g., Steele and Aronson, 1995), we decided to present adults aged 60–70 years with a DRM task associated with a free recall task. First, we expected older adults to correctly recall fewer words as the perception of stereotype threat increases. We also expected the relationship between participants’ perception of stereotype threat and correct recalls to increase with the participants’ level of education. Second, regarding false memories, we expected older adults to produce more critical lures as their perception of a stereotype threat increases. In line with Smith et al. (2017), we expected this relationship to be moderated by education. The relationship between participants’ perception of a stereotype threat and the production of critical lures should be stronger as education increases.

      Materials and Methods Participants

      Eighty-two older adults (60–70 years, M = 64.65 years, SD = 3.15, 54% female) were recruited in senior centers and community dwelling homes in the area of Nantes. Participants were all native French speakers; they received no compensation for their participation. All participants gave their informed consent before their inclusion in the study. Global cognitive efficiency was assessed using the French version of the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE; Derouesné et al., 1999). All older adults scored equal or higher than 26 on the MMSE; results indicated a preserved global cognitive functioning (M = 28.74, SD = 1.18) in our participants. The French version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger et al., 1993) revealed no signs of trait-anxiety in our sample (MDState = 29.84, SDState = 8.62; MTrait = 37.09, SDTrait = 8.56). Participants also completed a demographic questionnaire comprising questions regarding the highest diploma they got, the age until which they have been to school, and their current or former occupation. These items were used to compute the Education variable, which is defined as the number of years of instruction since the age of 6 (as instruction was mandatory from the age of 6 in France at the time). Overall, our participants reported a mean of 12.72 years of education (SD = 3.12). Participant characteristics are presented in Table 1.

      Participant characteristics: means, SD, and Pearson correlations with confidence intervals.

      Variable n M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
      1. Age (in years) 82 64.65 3.15
      2. Education (in years) 81 12.72 3.12 −0.34 **
      [−0.52, −0.13]
      3. MMSE 82 28.74 1.18 −0.17 0.35 **
      [−0.38, 0.05] [0.14, 0.53]
      4. PST 82 3.54 1.61 0.18 −0.10 −0.16
      [−0.03, 0.39] [−0.31, 0.12] [−0.36, 0.06]
      5. State-anxiety 82 29.84 8.62 0.08 −0.14 −0.09 0.05
      [−0.14, 0.29] [−0.35, 0.08] [−0.30, 0.13] [−0.17, 0.27]
      6. Trait-anxiety 82 37.09 8.56 0.02 −0.11 0.04 0.14 0.42 **
      [−0.20, 0.23] [−0.32, 0.12] [−0.18, 0.25] [−0.08, 0.34] [0.22, 0.58]
      7. Correct recalls a 82 0.53 0.12 −0.32 ** 0.37 ** 0.56 ** −0.21 −0.10 0.07
      [−0.50, −0.11] [0.16, 0.54] [0.39, 0.69] [−0.41, 0.01] [−0.31, 0.12] [−0.15, 0.28]
      8. Critical lures a 82 0.56 0.25 0.13 −0.08 0.00 0.06 −0.14 −0.01 −0.03
      [−0.09, 0.33] [−0.29, 0.15] [−0.22, 0.22] [−0.16, 0.27] [−0.34, 0.08] [−0.23, 0.20] [−0.24, 0.19]

      MMSE, Mini Mental State Exam; PST, perceived stereotype threat. Values in square brackets indicate the 95% confidence interval for each correlation.

      Correct recalls and critical lures are reported as proportions.

      p < 0.01.

      An ethics approval was not required for this study as per institutional and national guidelines and regulations. However, the study was carried out in accordance with University of Nantes ethics guidelines and the French law no. 2004-801 of August 6, 2004 relating to the protection of the natural persons with regard to the processing of personal data and amending law no. 78-17 of January 6, 1978 relating to data, files, and freedoms. Ethics was checked at the laboratory level. Participation in the research was voluntary, and the data were collected anonymously in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (World Medical Association, 2013). Participants were informed that they could receive a written document explaining the main results of the study upon request.

      Materials Perception of Stereotype Threat

      Participants’ stereotype threat perception was assessed with two items drawn from Gaillard et al. (2011). Participants rated their level of agreement with each of the two statements (“I worry that my ability to perform well on this test is affected by my age” and “I worry that if I perform poorly on this test, the experimenter will attribute my poor performance to my age”) using a 7-point scale ranging from “1-strongly disagree” to “7-strongly agree.” As responses to these two statements were highly correlated (r = 0.66, p < 0.001), we computed one averaged score of PST.

      DRM Task

      The material used in the present study consisted of six DRM lists, each comprising 15 words, drawn from Corson and Verrier (2007). We created two orders of presentation of the six DRM lists and counterbalanced them across participants.

      Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1.

      Procedure

      Participants were tested individually in a single session that lasted approximately 40 min. After giving their informed consent, participants completed the MMSE, the demographic questionnaire, the state-anxiety questionnaire, and then the DRM task. The DRM lists were presented orally by the experimenter, one at a time, at a rate of one word every 1.5 s. Immediately after each list presentation, participants were given 90 s maximum to perform an oral free recall task. Participants were told that the experimenter was more interested in the type of words (quality of the recall) than in the number of words they recall (quantity of the recall). Immediately after the DRM task, participants assessed their perception of stereotype threat and then their level of trait-anxiety. At the end of the session, participants were fully debriefed and thanked for their participation.

      Results

      The results are presented in the next three sections. We first examined the correlations between all the variables taken into account in our study. Second, we examined the moderator role of education on the relationship between PST and the correct recalls. Finally, we investigated the moderating role of education on the relationship between PST and false memories production (production of critical lures). Statistical analyses were performed using R software (Version 3.6.0; R Core Team, 2019) and JASP (Version 0.13.1; JASP Team, 2020).

      Correlation Analyses

      The analysis revealed that Education was negatively and significantly correlated with Age but positively and significantly correlated with global cognitive efficiency assessed with the MMSE. Higher levels of education were associated with higher global cognitive efficiency and younger age. As expected, the two measures of state‐ and trait-anxiety were positively and significantly correlated. In addition, proportions of correct recalls in the DRM task were significantly and positively correlated with Education and global cognitive efficiency but negatively with Age. No other significant correlation was found (see Table 1).

      Correct Recalls

      We conducted a regression analysis to investigate both the role of PST as a predictor of the proportion of correct recalls and the moderating role of Education on the proportions of correct recalls in the DRM task (see Table 2).

      Hierarchical regression analyses for variables predicting the proportion of correct recalls.

      Predictor b p b 95% CI sr 2 sr 2 95% CI Fit Difference
      Model 1
      (Intercept) 0.53 <0.001 [0.50, 0.61]
      Education 0.01 0.001 [−0.03, 0.04] 0.00 [−0.02, 0.02]
      PST −0.01 0.155 [−0.02, 0.01] 0.00 [−0.03, 0.04]
      R 2 = 0.156 [0.03, 0.06]
      p = 0.738
      Model 2
      (Intercept) 0.54 < 0.001 [0.51, 0.56]
      Education 0.01 0.002 [0.00, 0.02] 0.11 [−0.01, 0.23]
      PST −0.01 0.235 [−0.03, 0.01] 0.02 [−0.03, 0.06]
      PST × Education 0.01 0.068 [−0.00, 0.01] 0.04 [−0.04, 0.11]
      R 2 = 0.192 [0.04, 0.32] ΔR 2 = 0.036 [0.00, 0.13]
      p < 0.001 p = 0.068
      Model 3
      (Intercept) −0.30 0.447 [−1.09, 0.49]
      Education 0.01 0.176 [−0.00, 0.01] 0.02 [−0.03, 0.06]
      PST −0.01 0.442 [−0.02, 0.01] 0.00 [−0.02, 0.03]
      PST × Education 0.00 0.306 [−0.00, 0.01] 0.01 [−0.02, 0.04]
      Age −0.01 0.059 [−0.01, 0.00] 0.03 [−0.03, 0.09]
      STAI.State −0.00 0.655 [−0.00, 0.00] 0.00 [−0.01, 0.02]
      STAI.Trait 0.00 0.278 [−0.00, 0.00] 0.01 [−0.02, 0.04]
      MMSE 0.04 < 0.001 [0.02, 0.06] 0.15 [0.02, 0.27]
      R 2 = 0.401 [0.17, 0.49] ΔR 2 = 0.209 [0.07, 0.35]
      p < 0.001 p < 0.001

      PST, perceived stereotype threat. A significant b-weight indicates that the semi-partial correlation is also significant. b represents unstandardized regression weights. sr 2 represents the semi-partial correlation squared.

      First, PST and Education were introduced as predictors of the proportion of correct recalls (Model 1). The analysis yielded a significant main effect of Education (b = 0.01, p = 0.001, sr 2 = 0.00). When the interaction between Education and PST was added to the model, the results indicated that 19% of the variance of the correct recalls is related to the model (p < 0.001). More precisely, Education was the only significant predictor of the proportion of correct recalls (b = 0.01, p = 0.002, sr 2 = 0.11). Neither the PST (b = −0.01, p = 0.235) nor the Education × PST interaction (b = 0.01, p = 0.068) predicted the proportion of correct recalls (Model 2).

      Finally, we added Age, MMSE, State-, and Trait-Anxiety to the previous model as controlled variables (Model 3). The overall fit increases significatively (R 2 = 0.401, p < 0.001; ΔR 2 = 0.209, p < 0.001) and MMSE became the only significant predictor (b = 0.04, p < 0.001, sr 2 = 0.15) offsetting the effect of Education (b = 0.01, p = 0.176, sr 2 = 0.02).

      False Memories

      We conducted a regression analysis to investigate both the role of PST as a predictor of the proportion of critical lures produced and the moderating role of Education on the proportions of critical lures produced in the DRM task (see Table 3).

      Hierarchical regression analyses for variables predicting the proportion of critical lures.

      Predictor b p b 95% CI sr 2 sr 2 95% CI Fit Difference
      Model 1
      (Intercept) 0.56 <0.001 [0.50, 0.61]
      PST 0.01 0.691 [−0.03, 0.04] 0.00 [−0.02, 0.02]
      Education −0.01 0.531 [−0.02, 0.01] 0.00 [−0.03, 0.04]
      R 2 = 0.008 [0.00, 0.06]
      p = 0.738
      Model 2
      (Intercept) 0.56 <0.001 [0.51, 0.62]
      PST 0.01 0.455 [−0.02, 0.05] 0.01 [−0.03, 0.04]
      Education −0.01 0.364 [−0.03, 0.01] 0.01 [−0.03, 0.05]
      PST × Education 0.01 0.013 [0.00, 0.03] 0.08 [−0.03, 0.19]
      R 2 = 0.084 [0.00, 0.19] ΔR 2 = 0.076 [0.00, 0.22]
      p = 0.078 p = 0.013
      Model 3
      (Intercept) 0.17 0.858 [−1.76, 2.11]
      PST 0.01 0.593 [−0.03, 0.04] 0.00 [−0.02, 0.03]
      Education −0.01 0.559 [−0.03, 0.01] 0.00 [−0.02, 0.03]
      PST × Education 0.01 0.020 [0.00, 0.03] 0.07 [−0.04, 0.17]
      Age 0.01 0.315 [−0.01, 0.03] 0.01 [−0.03, 0.06]
      STAI.State −0.00 0.234 [−0.01, 0.00] 0.02 [−0.04, 0.07]
      STAI.Trait 0.00 0.906 [−0.01, 0.01] 0.00 [−0.01, 0.01]
      MMSE −0.00 0.882 [−0.05, 0.05] 0.00 [−0.01, 0.01]
      R 2 = 0.115 [0.00, 0.18] ΔR 2 = 0.031[−0.04, 0.10]
      p = 0.238 p = 0.638

      PST, perceived stereotype threat. A significant b-weight indicates that the semi-partial correlation is also significant. b represents unstandardized regression weights. sr 2 represents the semi-partial correlation squared.

      First, PST and Education were introduced as predictors of the proportion of critical lures. The analysis yielded no significant main effect (Model 1). When the interaction between Education and PST was introduced, the results indicated that the model predicted 8% of the total variance (p = 0.078). More precisely, neither the PST (b = 0.01, p = 0.455, sr 2 = 0.01) nor the Education (b = −0.01, p = 0.364, sr 2 = 0.01) predicted the proportion of critical lures (Model 2). However, results indicated a moderating role of Education with 8% of the total variance associated with the PST × Education interaction (b = 0.01, p = 0.013, sr 2 = 0.08); the relation between PST and the production of critical lures depends on an individual’s education level.

      When Age, MMSE, State-, and Trait-Anxiety were entered in the previous model as controlled variables, the overall fit of the model increases nonsignificantly (R 2 = 0.11, p = 0.238; ΔR 2 = 0.031, p = 0.638), but PST × Education interaction still was the only significant predictor (b = 0.01, p = 0.020, sr 2 = 0.07) confirming the moderating role of Education on the relationship between PST and the production of critical lures (Model 3).

      One way to visualize and summarize simply the moderated effect of Education is to follow the Johnson-Neyman technique (Johnson and Fay, 1950). From Model 2 in Table 3, we used this technique to determine the education level intervals in which the relationship between PST and the proportion of critical lures is significant or not significant (see Figure 1). When the level of education is between 6.9 and 15.8 years, the relationship between PST and the proportion of critical lures is not significant; when the level is above 15.8 years, an increase in PST is significantly related to an increase in the proportion of critical lures, and when the level is below 6.9 years, an increase in PST is significantly related to a decrease in the proportion of critical lures. For instance, for the higher educated group (>16 years), the slope of PST on the proportion of critical lures is greater than b = 0.06 (and significant). One should note, however, that lower levels of Education such as 6.9 years and less are scarce in adults now aged from 60 and over.

      Representation of the slope of perceived stereotype threat on the production of critical lures according to the level of education. The range of observed values of Education is [8.00, 20.00].

      Discussion

      The present study was aimed at identifying the moderating role of education on the relationship between the perceived age-based stereotype threat and false memory. As expected, our results showed that the production of critical lures was best predicted by the PST × Education interaction. To our knowledge, this is the first study that shows an increase of false memories’ production in highly educated older adults as their perception of an age-based stereotype threat increases, without relying on any experimental stereotype threat manipulation. Our results are consistent with previous research that has shown that highly educated older adults are more susceptible to false memories (Smith et al., 2017) under the induced stereotype threat. These results support the idea that highly educated individuals may place a high value on their memory and feel especially threatened by age-based stereotypes in such a false memory task. Consistent with the cognitive hypothesis (Schmader and Johns, 2003), they may engage most of their cognitive resources in order to distance themselves from the stereotype, thereby leaving only few resources to correctly perform the task. The increase of false memories in these highly educated participants along with the increase of their perception of an age-based stereotype threat is also consistent with the increase of false alarms observed by Brubaker and Naveh-Benjamin (2018) in adults aged 65 and over under the stereotype threat and the increase of false memories showed by Thomas and Dubois (2011) and Smith et al. (2017). This is particularly interesting since, in contrast to most previous studies, we did not use experimental stereotype threat induction. This suggests that naturally occurring stereotype threat in testing situations (e.g., Brubaker and Naveh-Benjamin, 2018) may have as deleterious effects on cognitive performance as an induced stereotype threat.

      The literature shows that the occurrence of false memories is usually the result of global, heuristic, relational, familiarity-based, or gist-based processing rather than in distinctive, item-specific, or verbatim processing (e.g., Smith and Hunt, 1998; Brainerd and Reyna, 2002; McCabe et al., 2004). Our results indicate that participants with a strong PST may have engaged in a familiarity-based or gist-based strategy rather than in a recollection or verbatim processing during the free recall task. This higher reliance on familiarity leads participants to falsely recall critical lures. This is consistent with the idea that under threat, participants may rely more on global, heuristic, or automatic processes than on analytic and controlled processes (Mazerolle et al., 2012). This strategy is quite maladaptive in the false memory context but is particularly effective when trying to recall studied words from DRM lists. Indeed, with regards to correct recalls, our results showed that education is the only predictor of participants’ performance; the more educated they are, the more they recall previously studied words. This positive effect of education is consistent with previous research showing that education, as a major component of cognitive reserve, is a protective factor against the decline of cognitive abilities in aging (e.g., Nyberg and Pudas, 2019; Clouston et al., 2020). However, when MMSE total score was controlled for, it became the only significant predictor of correct recalls, offsetting the effect of education. In addition, we showed a strong positive correlation between MMSE total score and the proportion of correct recalls. These results are consistent with previous studies that have highlighted the positive relationship between MMSE total score and episodic memory performance (e.g., Taylor et al., 1992; Aartsen et al., 2002). Our results also pointed to a positive correlation between MMSE total score and education. This is consistent with previous studies showing that higher levels of education are associated with better cognitive functioning (e.g., Crum, 1993; Albert et al., 1995; but see Bleecker et al., 1988) or that education is a good predictor of the MMSE total score (e.g., Bertolucci et al., 1994). Finally, we highlighted a significant negative correlation between age and education level which is consistent with previous studies showing that earlier born cohorts of adults received less education than those born later (e.g., Matthews et al., 2012).

      Contrary to our expectations and to previous research, our results did not show any effect of a perceived of stereotype threat either on correct recalls (e.g., Hess et al., 2009; Barber and Mather, 2013b; Armstrong et al., 2017) or on false memories (Thomas and Dubois, 2011; Smith et al., 2017). One explanation might be that, in contrast with previous studies, we did not use a blatant stereotype threat induction using short texts describing cognitive and memory declines in aging. To be closer to natural situations of memory assessments, we chose to assess participants’ PST naturally occurring in a testing situation. Our participants reported scores of PST as high as those reported by the participants in Gaillard et al. (2011) who underwent an experimental stereotype threat induction. As participants were informed both in the consent form and in the instructions that the experiment was about memory, one cannot rule out the possibility that this constitutes a subtle induction of stereotype threat (Nguyen and Ryan, 2008; Marquet et al., 2016). Another explanation may lie in the fact that we used a DRM associated with a free recall task that, despite our specific instructions regarding the recall task, may have elicited a promotion focus in our participants. Since it has been argued that the stereotype threat induces a prevention focus (see Barber, 2017), the lack of regulatory fit may have led to these results. However, our study did not directly address the regulatory focus theory since we did not manipulate nor assess promotion or prevention foci in our participants. Future research using subtle induction of stereotype threat would benefit from investigating this issue further.

      In conclusion, this study showed that an age-based stereotype threat may naturally occur under testing situations in adults aged between 60 and 70 and affect their susceptibility to false memories. The present study also highlighted the central role of education in the evaluation of memory in older adults. While it predicted correct recalls, it also moderated the effects of PST on false memories. These results encourage more consideration of the education level as a study variable in research on aging, and not just as a simple controlled variable.

      Data Availability Statement

      The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

      Ethics Statement

      Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

      Author Contributions

      A-LG, CE, and FC contributed to conception and design of the study. J-MG conducted the statistical analyses. A-LG wrote the first draft of the manuscript. CE, FC, and J-MG provided comments on the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision and read and approved the submitted version.

      Conflict of Interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      We thank Manon Bonneau, Soukaina Hasnaoui, Tiphaine Housty, Marine Morillon, and Michèle Taridec for their assistance in recruitment and data collection. We also thank Johana Desbordes for early discussions on this topic. These data were presented at the 59th annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society.

      References Aartsen M. J. Smits C. H. M. van Tilburg T. Knipscheer K. C. P. M. Deeg D. J. H. (2002). Activity in older adults: cause or consequence of cognitive functioning? A longitudinal study on everyday activities and cognitive performance in older adults. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 57, 153162. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.2.p153, PMID: 11867663 Albert M. S. Jones K. Savage C. R. Berkman L. Seeman T. Blazer D. . (1995). Predictors of cognitive change in older persons: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Psychol. Aging 10, 578589. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.10.4.578, PMID: 8749585 Armstrong B. Gallant S. N. Li L. Patel K. Wong B. I. (2017). Stereotype threat effects on older adults’ episodic and working memory: a meta-analysis. Gerontologist 57(Suppl. 2), S193S205. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnx056, PMID: 28854607 Barber S. J. (2017). An examination of age-based stereotype threat about cognitive decline: implications for stereotype-threat research and theory development. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 6290. doi: 10.1177/1745691616656345 Barber S. J. Mather M. (2013a). Stereotype threat can both enhance and impair older adults’ memory. Psychol. Sci. 24, 25222529. doi: 10.1177/0956797613497023, PMID: 24150969 Barber S. J. Mather M. (2013b). Stereotype threat can reduce older adults’ memory errors. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 66, 18881895. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.840656, PMID: 24131297 Barber S. J. Mather M. (2014). “Stereotype threat in older adults: when and why does it occur and who is most affected?” in The Oxford handbook of emotion, social cognition, and problem solving in adulthood. eds. Verhaeghen P. Hertzog C. (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press), 302319. Bertolucci P. H. Brucki S. M. Campacci S. R. Juliano Y. (1994). The Mini-Mental State Examination in a general population: impact of educational status. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 52, 17. PMID: 8002795 Bleecker M. L. Bolla-Wilson K. Kawas C. Agnew J. (1988). Age-specific norms for the Mini-Mental State Exam. Neurology 38, 15651568. doi: 10.1212/wnl.38.10.1565, PMID: 3419600 Brainerd C. J. Reyna V. F. (2002). Fuzzy-trace theory and false memory. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 11, 164169. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00192 Brubaker M. S. Naveh-Benjamin M. (2018). The effects of stereotype threat on the associative memory deficit of older adults. Psychol. Aging 33, 1729. doi: 10.1037/pag0000194, PMID: 29494175 Chasteen A. L. Bhattacharyya S. Horhota M. Tam R. Hasher L. (2005). How feelings of stereotype threat influence older adults’ memory performance. Exp. Aging Res. 31, 235260. doi: 10.1080/03610730590948177, PMID: 16036721 Clouston S. A. Smith D. M. Mukherjee S. Zhang Y. Hou W. Link B. G. . (2020). Education and cognitive decline: an integrative analysis of global longitudinal studies of cognitive aging. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 75, e151e160. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbz053, PMID: 31059564 Corson Y. Verrier N. (2007). Emotions and false memories: valence or arousal? Psychol. Sci. 18, 208211. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01874.x, PMID: 17444912 Corson Y. Verrier N. Bucic A. (2009). False memories and individual variations: the role of field dependence-independence. Pers. Individ. Differ. 47, 811. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.01.036 Craik F. I. McDowd J. M. (1987). Age differences in recall and recognition. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 13, 474479. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.13.3.474 Crum R. M. (1993). Population-based norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination by age and educational level. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 269, 23862391. doi: 10.1001/jama.1993.03500180078038, PMID: 8479064 Deese J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. J. Exp. Psychol. 58, 1722. doi: 10.1037/h0046671, PMID: 13664879 Dehon H. Brédart S. (2004). False memories: young and older adults think of semantic associates at the same rate, but young adults are more successful at source monitoring. Psychol. Aging 19, 191197. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.191, PMID: 15065942 Derouesné C. Poitreneau J. Hugonot L. Kalafat M. Dubois B. Laurent B. (1999). Le Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE): Un outil pratique pour l’évaluation de l’état cognitif des patients par le clinicien version française consensuelle. Presse Med. 28, 11411148. PMID: 10399508 Desrichard O. Köpetz C. (2005). A threat in the elder: the impact of task-instructions, self-efficacy and performance expectations on memory performance in the elderly. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 35, 537552. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.249 Gaillard M. Desmette D. Keller J. (2011). Regulatory focus moderates the influence of age-related stereotypic expectancies on older adults’ test performance and threat-based concerns. Eur. Rev. Appl. Psychol. 61, 2329. doi: 10.1016/j.erap.2010.11.001 Grühn D. Gilet A. -L. Studer J. Labouvie-Vief G. (2011). Age-relevance of person characteristics: Persons’ beliefs about developmental change across the lifespan. Dev. Psychol. 47, 376387. doi: 10.1037/a0021315, PMID: 21142359 Hess T. M. Auman C. Colcombe S. J. Rahhal T. A. (2003). The impact of stereotype threat on age differences in memory performance. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 58, P3P11. doi: 10.1093/geronb/58.1.P3, PMID: 12496296 Hess T. M. Hinson J. T. Hodges E. A. (2009). Moderators of and mechanisms underlying stereotype threat effects on older adults’ memory performance. Exp. Aging Res. 35, 153177. doi: 10.1080/03610730802716413, PMID: 19280445 Hess T. M. Hinson J. T. Statham J. A. (2004). Explicit and implicit stereotype activation effects on memory: do age and awareness moderate the impact of priming? Psychol. Aging 19, 495505. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.3.495, PMID: 15382999 Higgins E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. Am. Psychol. 52, 12801300. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.52.12.1280, PMID: 9414606 Hummert M. L. (2011). “Age stereotypes and aging” in Handbook of the psychology of aging. 7th Edn. eds. Schaie K. W. Willis S. L. (Elsevier Academic Press), 249262. Hummert M. L. Garstka T. A. Shaner J. L. Strahm S. (1994). Stereotypes of the elderly held by young, middle-aged, and elderly adults. J. Gerontol. 49, P240P249. doi: 10.1093/geronj/49.5.p240, PMID: 8056949 Hunt R. R. Einstein G. (1981). Relational and item-specific information in memory. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 20, 497514. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(81)90138-9 JASP Team (2020). JASP (Version 0.13.1) [Computer software]. Johnson P. O. Fay L. C. (1950). The Johnson–Neyman technique, its theory and application. Psychometrika 15, 349367. doi: 10.1007/BF02288864, PMID: 14797902 Kite M. E. Stockdale G. D. Whitley B. E. Jr. Johnson B. T. (2005). Attitudes toward younger and older adults: an updated meta-analytic review. J. Soc. Issues 61, 241266. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2005.00404.x Lamont R. A. Swift H. J. Abrams D. (2015). A review and meta-analysis of age-based stereotype threat: negative stereotypes, not facts, do the damage. Psychol. Aging 30, 180193. doi: 10.1037/a0038586, PMID: 25621742 Levy B. (2009). Stereotype embodiment: a psychosocial approach to aging. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 18, 332336. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01662.x, PMID: 20802838 Marquet M. Missotten P. Adam S. (2016). Âgisme et surestimation des difficultés cognitives des personnes âgées: une revue de la question [Ageism and overestimation of cognitive difficulties in older people: a review]. Geriatr. Psychol. Neuropsychiatr. Vieil. 14, 177186. doi: 10.1684/pnv.2016.0609, PMID: 27277150 Matthews F. Marioni R. Brayne C. Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study (2012). Examining the influence of gender, education, social class and birth cohort on MMSE tracking over time: a population-based prospective cohort study. BMC Geriatr. 12:45. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-12-45, PMID: 22889350 Mazerolle M. Régner I. Morisset P. Rigalleau F. Huguet P. (2012). Stereotype threat strengthens automatic recall and undermines controlled processes in older adults. Psychol. Sci. 23, 723727. doi: 10.1177/0956797612437607, PMID: 22609539 McCabe D. P. Presmanes A. G. Robertson C. L. Smith A. D. (2004). Item-specific processing reduces false memories. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 11, 10741079. doi: 10.3758/BF03196739, PMID: 15875978 Nguyen H. -H. D. Ryan A. M. (2008). Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence. J. Appl. Psychol. 93, 13141334. doi: 10.1037/a0012702, PMID: 19025250 Nyberg L. Pudas S. (2019). Successful memory aging. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 70, 219243. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103052, PMID: 29949727 Payne J. D. Nadel L. Allen J. J. Thomas K. G. Jacobs W. J. (2002). The effects of experimentally induced stress on false recognition. Memory 10, 16. doi: 10.1080/09658210143000119, PMID: 11747571 Popham L. E. Hess T. M. (2015). Age differences in the underlying mechanisms of stereotype threat effects. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 70, 223234. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt093, PMID: 24077743 R Core Team (2019). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Available at: https://www.R-project.org/ (Accessed December 04, 2020). Rahhal T. A. Hasher L. Colcombe S. J. (2001). Instructional manipulations and age differences in memory: now you see them, now you don’t. Psychol. Aging 16, 697706. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.16.4.697, PMID: 11766922 Robinson K. J. Roediger H. L. (1997). Associative processes in false recall and false recognition. Psychol. Sci. 8, 231237. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00417.x Roediger H. L. McDermott K. (1995). Creating false memories: remembering words not presented in lists. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 21, 803814. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803 Schmader T. Johns M. (2003). Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 440452. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.440, PMID: 14498781 Seibt B. Förster J. (2004). Stereotype threat and performance: how self-stereotypes influence processing by inducing regulatory foci. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 3856. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.1.38, PMID: 15250791 Smith A. M. Gallo D. A. Barber S. J. Maddox K. B. Thomas A. K. (2017). Stereotypes, warnings, and identity-related variables influence older adults’ susceptibility to associative false memory errors. Gerontologist 57(Suppl. 2), S206S215. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnx057, PMID: 28854608 Smith R. E. Hunt R. R. (1998). Presentation modality affects false memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 5, 710715. doi: 10.3758/BF03208850 Spielberger C. D. Bruchon-Schweitzer M. Paulhan I. (1993). Inventaire d’Anxiété Etat-Trait Forme Y (STAI-Y). Paris: Les Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée. Steele C. M. Aronson J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69, 797811. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797, PMID: 7473032 Storbeck J. Clore G. L. (2005). With sadness comes accuracy; with happiness, false memory. Mood and the false memory effect. Psychol. Sci. 16, 785791. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01615.x, PMID: 16181441 Storbeck J. Clore G. L. (2011). Affect influences false memories at encoding: evidence from recognition data. Emotion 11, 981989. doi: 10.1037/a0022754, PMID: 21517165 Taylor J. L. Miller T. P. Tinklenberg J. R. (1992). Correlates of memory decline: a 4-year longitudinal study of older adults with memory complaints. Psychol. Aging 7, 185193. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.7.2.185, PMID: 1610506 Thomas A. K. Dubois S. J. (2011). Reducing the burden of stereotype threat eliminates age differences in memory distortion. Psychol. Sci. 22, 15151517. doi: 10.1177/0956797611425932, PMID: 22030349 Tun P. A. Wingfield A. Rosen M. J. Blanchard L. (1998). Response latencies for false memories: gist-based processing in normal aging. Psychol. Aging 13, 230241. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.13.2.230, PMID: 9640584 Wong J. T. Gallo D. A. (2016). Stereotype threat reduces false recognition when older adults are forewarned. Memory 24, 650658. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2015.1036885, PMID: 26218527 World Medical Association (2013). World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA 310, 21912194. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.281053, PMID: 24141714
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016jgchain.com.cn
      haokuke.com.cn
      www.flchain.com.cn
      pschain.com.cn
      www.qiang1122.net.cn
      www.mymzmj.org.cn
      www.nz8news.com.cn
      ozflxj.com.cn
      www.tealove.com.cn
      www.xfhypy.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p