Edited by: Cunmei Jiang, Shanghai Normal University, China
Reviewed by: Lutz Jäncke, University of Zurich, Switzerland; Clara Eline James, University of Applied Sciences and Arts of Western Switzerland, Switzerland
This article was submitted to Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
Healthy aging may be accompanied by cognitive decline that includes diminished attentional control, an executive function that allows us to focus our attention while inhibiting distractors. Previous studies have demonstrated that background music can enhance some executive functions in both young and older adults. According to the
香京julia种子在线播放
Music listening induces strong and consistent emotions in the listener (
Valence and arousal dimensions also seem to interact in inducing musical emotions (
Background music has been shown to have both beneficial and detrimental effects on a variety of cognitive functions in healthy young adults (see
In particular, the impact of background music on attentional control, an executive function that allows one to focus attention on a specific stimulus, while inhibiting distractors from the environment (
The role of music in attentional control in elderly populations has only been investigated very recently (
It is important to study the effect of background music in older adults since attentional control can be impaired in normal cognitive aging (
In addition to attentional declines in the elderly, it is possible that the presentation of a visual or auditory stimulus during the completion of a cognitive task might be more distracting for older adults than young adults, even if they are told to ignore the distraction (
However, it is also possible that background music added to a visual task could impair the performance of young adults. Indeed, for attentional tasks that are time-critical, as well as for spatial attentional tasks, shared attentional resources are involved when processing stimuli from different modalities (i.e., auditory and visual), and this is the case for adults of all ages (for a review, see
In sum, normal aging is accompanied by cognitive decline that affects attentional control. Thus, it is important to find easy and pleasant ways for older adults to maximize their attentional control in everyday situations, for example with background music. However, the beneficial effect of background music on different executive functions is not fully understood, possibly due to the fact that the arousal level of music is not always controlled in previous studies. More particularly, the comparison between young and older adults in the effect of background music on attentional control specifically needs more investigation.
This study aimed to determine if the influence of background music, and more specifically its arousal level, might improve visuo-spatial attentional control in older adults and whether this effect is similar across older and young adults. To do this, we compared the effect of stimulating and relaxing music on performance on the flanker task, with a silence condition representing the base level performance.
Regarding the effect of background music, we expected faster answers and fewer errors for older adults under the stimulating music condition compared to both the relaxing music and silence conditions. As for young adults, knowing that results in the literature about the effect of background music on attentional control are still heterogeneous, there were no hypotheses concerning the effect of background music on their performance on the flanker task.
Nineteen older adults and 21 younger adults participated in this experiment. They all provided informed consent and received financial compensation for their participation. All participants were francophone Quebecers and reported to have normal audition, as well as normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. They also reported information about their music listening habits. None reported neurological, neurodevelopmental, or diagnosed psychiatric disorders. Depression and anxiety questionnaires were used to ensure that participants did not have clinically significant levels of anxio-depressive symptoms.
Young adults completed both the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI;
Older adults completed the Geriatric Anxiety Inventory (GAI;
In addition, basic executive functioning was assessed using the color-word interference test, from the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System battery (D-KEFS;
The two groups were significantly different in age (see
Comparison between older and younger adults on demographic variables.
Age groups | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Older adults | Young adults | Effect size ( |
||||
N (M, F) | 19 (1, 18) | 21 (2, 19) | 1 | 0.26 | =0.61 | =0.08 |
Age (years) | 67.26 (3.16) | 23.95 (3.51) | 38 | −40.82 | <0.001 | =0.99 |
Years of education | 16.16 (2.69) | 16.48 (1.86) | 38 | 0.44 | =0.66 | =0.07 |
Years of musical training | 1.37 (2.17) | 2.81 (4.69) | 38 | 1.23 | =0.23 | =0.2 |
Except for sex, this table presents means (and standard deviations). M = male, F = female. Group composition was compared for sex, using a chi square test, and for age, years of education, and years of musical training using independant t-tests.
The music listening habits of our sample did not appear to be different between young and older adults, neither as principal activity (reported by 13/21 young adults with a mean of 2.69 h/week and 13/19 older adults with a mean of 2.76 h/week) nor as background music (reported by all young adults with a mean time of 8.1 h/week and 13/19 older adults with a mean time of 8.79 h/week).
All participants performed an arrow version of Eriksen’s flanker task (
The course of a flanker task trial. Symbols are not to scale; they have been enlarged to be visible in this diagram.
Each trial contained five steps (see
The experiment comprised 21 blocks containing 32 trials each (with an equal number of congruent and incongruent trials) for a total of 672 trials. Both blocks and trials were presented in a randomized order for each participant. Of the 21 blocks, seven were allocated to each of the three auditory conditions (stimulating music, relaxing music, and silence). For the two musical conditions, each block was associated with a different musical excerpt. Participants could take breaks between each block to rest. The total duration of one block varied between 80 and 90 s, depending on the RT of the participant. Without the breaks between each block, the total duration of the entire task was approximately 30 min. To familiarize participants with the task, it was preceded by a practice block that included feedback to inform the participants about their performance. The practice block was presented with background music characterized by an intermediate tempo (i.e., 110 beats per minute, BPM). The flanker task and the music were presented using MATLAB (MATLAB Release 2018a, The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, Massachussetts, United States) with the “Psychophysics Toolbox Version 3” extension (
All participants performed the flanker task under three auditory conditions: stimulating music, relaxing music, and silence. The music was pleasant sounding instrumental works composed in a major mode, chosen from the classical repertoire. Inter-rater agreement between three researchers was used to select the seven most stimulating (e.g., William Tell Overture: Final, composed by Giochino Rossini), as well as the seven most relaxing (e.g., Suite Bergamasque, Clair de Lune composed by Claude Debussy), musical excerpts from a larger pool of musical material in use in our laboratory. Excerpts of 100 s were chosen from the original pieces, so that the arousal and valence levels, as well as the tempi, were stable throughout each excerpt. The stimulating musical excerpts had a mean tempo of 153.14 BPM (
After completing the flanker task, participants were asked to listen carefully to each musical excerpt without time restriction and to evaluate how much the piece was considered to be (a) arousing, i.e., relaxing or stimulating, (b) unpleasant or pleasant, and (c) unfamiliar or familiar, using a continuous visual analogue scale from 0 (extreme left) to 100 (extreme right). Thus, a low score on the arousal dimension would mean that the musical excerpt was judged as relaxing.
Group composition was compared for sex, using a chi square test, and for age, years of education, and years of musical training using independent
Performance on the flanker task was analyzed using RT and error rate (ER). Average RT values for successful trials were calculated in milliseconds for each flanker congruency type of trial (i.e., congruent and incongruent), each auditory condition (stimulating music, relaxing music, and silence), and each participant separately. The averages and standard deviations of ER as percentages (excluding missed trials) were also calculated for each participant, as well as for each flanker congruency and auditory condition. RT and ER scores were entered into separate mixed-design analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with Age Group (older and young adults) as a between-subject factor, Auditory Condition (stimulating music, relaxing music, and silence), and Flanker Congruency trial type (congruent and incongruent) as within-subject factors. When interactions between repeated measure factors were significant, a standard contrasts analysis was used to determine if the difference between congruent and incongruent trials (i.e., flanker effect) was the same between auditory conditions.
To confirm that the musical conditions differed in perceived arousal level and to explore whether there was a difference between older and younger adults’ judgments, a mixed-design ANOVA with the between subject factor Age Group (older and younger adults) and the within subject factor Music Condition (stimulating music and relaxing music) was conducted. Two other exploratory mixed design ANOVAs were conducted with the judgments of valence and familiarity.
All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics 24 (IBM Corp., 2016). Behavioral mean results (music evaluation and flanker performance) as well as statistical results are presented in
Results of the analyses of variance (ANOVA) for the evaluation of arousal, valence, and familiarity.
Predictor | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1, 38 |
1453.3 |
<0.001 |
0.98 |
||
ValenceMusic Condition |
1, 38 |
32.28 |
<0.001 |
0.46 |
|
FamiliarityMusic Condition |
1, 38 |
1.32 |
=0.258 |
0.033 |
Results of the ANOVA for the flanker task RT.
Predictor | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Omnibus analysisAge Group |
1, 38 |
55.02 |
<0.001 |
0.59 |
|
Contrasts analysisRelaxing vs. Stimulating |
1, 38 |
10.61 |
=0.002 |
0.22 |
Results of the ANOVA for the flanker task ER.
Predictor | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Omnibus analysisAge Group |
1, 38 |
9.86 |
=0.003 |
0.21 |
|
1, 38 |
1.13 |
=0.294 |
0.03 |
As expected, stimulating music was judged to be significantly more arousing than relaxing music by both older and young adult groups, the size of this effect being large (see
Judgments of arousal and valence. Mean rating (and standard errors) are presented as a function of music conditions and age groups on both valence and arousal dimensions.
Relaxing music was considered significantly more pleasant than stimulating music by both older and young adults, with this effect being large (see
Older adults were significantly more familiar (
Reaction time performance on the flanker task revealed a significant and general slowing in older adults compared to young adults (large effect, see
Mean reaction time (RT) in ms and error rate (ER) in % (and standard errors) are presented for older and young adults. Values of
Flanker effects in ms (and standard errors) are presented for all participants (combined across age groups) as a function of Auditory Condition. Values of
Older adults made fewer errors overall compared to young adults, and this was a large effect (see
Interaction between Age Group and Flanker Congruency in ER. Mean ER in % (and standard errors) were obtained for congruent and incongruent trials and separately for older and young adults. Values of
This study aimed to explore the effect of the arousal level of background music on visuo-spatial attentional control in young and older adults. To do this, both groups performed an arrow version of the flanker task under three auditory conditions: stimulating music, relaxing music, and silence.
The effects on ER seemed quite limited, probably due to the ceiling effect observed in both older and young adults (success rate > 95% for all participants). All participants presented expected slower RT and increased errors in the incongruent trials compared to the congruent ones, suggesting greater difficulty in inhibiting the distracting and incongruent arrows (
In addition, results obtained in the flanker task might have been influenced by the female dominance of our sample. A previous study demonstrated that visual selective attention performance of women is more affected by invalid cues, while men benefit from those invalid cues (
In our study, the influence of background music during a visuo-spatial attention task revealed impaired attentional control performance during relaxing music exposure compared to silence and stimulating music. These results are not consistent with the
It is difficult to reconcile these results demonstrating a difference between relaxing and stimulating music with the existing literature. However, based on previous studies demonstrating positive correlations between the tempo of music and cognitive performance across a number of domains, including reading speed (
The contradiction between our results and the
Regarding age, the greater flanker effects observed for the relaxing music condition was similar between both groups. This is in line with very recent work demonstrating that classical music with different arousal and valence levels has the same impact on attention processing in both young and older adults, even if the latter experience a decline in this particular executive function (
We analyzed participants’ arousal, valence, and familiarity evaluations of the musical excerpts. Our results indicated that the arousal level of the musical excerpts was judged as expected by both age groups. Also, all musical excerpts were evaluated as pleasant, but unexpectedly, relaxing music pieces were felt to be more pleasant than stimulating music pieces. This finding is inconsistent with previous studies demonstrating that stimulating music generated higher ratings of pleasantness by listeners (
Relaxing and stimulating music did not differ in terms of familiarity level here, suggesting that the observed effect of background music on attentional control is likely due to the variations in arousal and valence levels only. However, we did find that music excerpts used in the current experiment were rated as more familiar and more pleasant for older adults than young adults. These results might be explained by the fact that older adults listen more to classical music, while young adults listen mostly to popular music (
Moreover, our participants were mostly women, and gender is known to have a moderate influence on the emotions induced by music (
Although the evaluation of the valence dimension showed unexpected results [i.e., (1) relaxing music evaluated as more pleasant than stimulating music and (2) overall higher valence scores in the older compared to younger adults], the evaluation of the arousal dimension of our stimulating/relaxing musical excerpts were judged as expected. This allowed us to evaluate the effect of the arousal dimension of background music on attentional control, as measured by the flanker task.
In conclusion, we observed the expected performance of older and young adults in the flanker task, with slower RT and greater ER for incongruent trials compared to congruent trials. Our results, not supported by the
This study presents some limitations. First, only classical music was used and older adults found it more pleasant and familiar than young adults, potentially inducing a differential impact of these stimuli on our participants as a function of age. Future studies should control for the impact of age on emotional judgments of musical stimuli when comparing older and young adults. Second, although we screened older adults for cognitive impairments, given that hearing and vision loss can impact the cognitive performance of older adults, ideally these perceptual functions should be measured as well and, if necessary, entered as covariates in the analysis. Third, the music conditions differed not only in arousal but also in valence. Even if those two dimensions often interact together (
In order to improve the evaluation of arousal, future studies should use real-time objective measurements of arousal through the recording of electrodermal activity, while participants listen to the music and execute the task. Also, it would be interesting to control for inter-individual variability, in general, arousal level by measuring it before the beginning of the experiment. It would also be important to take into account the gender of participants in studying music-induced emotions. Moreover, to improve the ecological validity of the results, future work could also investigate the influence of longer periods of background music listening (in contrast to our 100 s excerpts) on visuo-spatial attentional control performance. An important extension to the current research would be the inclusion of other modalities of attentional control during background music listening, in order to draw more general conclusions about attentional control, and not limited to visuo-spatial attentional control as in the current paper. As mentioned previously, since some studies found an effect of background music on cortical activity in absence of a behavioral effect (
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Comité d’éthique de la recherche en arts et en sciences, Université de Montréal. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
AC elaborated the theoretical frame and formulated the research question, as well as the objectives and hypotheses. AC contributed to the creation of the research protocol and methodology. AC contributed to the data collection and analysis and wrote the article. CH-A contributed to the creation of the research protocol and methodology, as well as the data collection. CH-A contributed to the revision and correction of the article. NF contributed to the establishment of the flanker task parameters, as well as to the revision and correction of the article. NG contributed to the elaboration of the theoretical frame and the formulation of the research question, objectives, and hypotheses. NG supervised the creation of the research protocol and methodology, as well as the data collection and analysis and the article redaction. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
We would like to thank Falco Enzler for his help in programming the MATLAB script for the flanker task. We also want to thank Simone Dalla Bella, Ph.D., and Simona Brambati, Ph.D. who evaluated Amélie’s master’s thesis. And most importantly, we would like to thank our participants, without whom the research would be impossible.