Front. Psychol. Frontiers in Psychology Front. Psychol. 1664-1078 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02405 Psychology Original Research 360 Degrees of Facial Perception: Congruence in Perception of Frontal Portrait, Profile, and Rotation Photographs Třebický Vít 1 2 * Fialová Jitka 1 2 Stella David 1 2 Štěrbová Zuzana 1 2 Kleisner Karel 1 2 Havlíček Jan 1 2 1National Institute of Mental Health, Klecany, Czechia 2Faculty of Science, Charles University, Prague, Czechia

Edited by: Ian Stephen, Macquarie University, Australia

Reviewed by: Barnaby James Wyld Dixson, The University of Queensland, Australia; Danielle Leigh Wagstaff, Federation University, Australia

*Correspondence: Vít Třebický vit.trebicky@natur.cuni.cz

This article was submitted to Evolutionary Psychology, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology

07 12 2018 2018 9 2405 12 09 2018 15 11 2018 Copyright © 2018 Třebický, Fialová, Stella, Štěrbová, Kleisner and Havlíček. 2018 Třebický, Fialová, Stella, Štěrbová, Kleisner and Havlíček

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Studies in social perception traditionally use as stimuli frontal portrait photographs. It turns out, however, that 2D frontal depiction may not fully capture the entire morphological diversity of facial features. Recently, 3D images started to become increasingly popular, but whether their perception differs from the perception of 2D has not been systematically studied as yet. Here we investigated congruence in the perception of portrait, left profile, and 360° rotation photographs. The photographs were obtained from 45 male athletes under standardized conditions. In two separate studies, each set of images was rated for formidability (portraits by 62, profiles by 60, and 360° rotations by 94 raters) and attractiveness (portraits by 195, profiles by 176, and 360° rotations by 150 raters) on a 7-point scale. The ratings of the stimuli types were highly intercorrelated (for formidability all rs > 0.8, for attractiveness all rs > 0.7). Moreover, we found no differences in the mean ratings between the three types of stimuli, neither in formidability, nor in attractiveness. Overall, our results clearly suggest that different facial views convey highly overlapping information about structural facial elements of an individual. They lead to congruent assessments of formidability and attractiveness, and a single angle view seems sufficient for face perception research.

2D 3D head standardized photography assessment morphology attractiveness formidability Grantová Agentura České Republiky10.13039/501100001824 Ministerstvo Školství, Mládeže a Tělovýchovy10.13039/501100001823

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      When artists create portraits, they rarely depict a full frontal view of the face of a given sitter. Instead, they tend to portray people in some degree of profile, emphasizing one cheek and dimensionality of a face (Murphy, 1994). Interestingly, vast majority of studies on facial perception uses frontal portraits (un/altered photographs, morphs, or line drawings) as stimuli (e.g., Thornhill and Gangestad, 1999; Rhodes, 2006; Kościnski, 2009; Calder et al., 2011; Little et al., 2011; Valentová et al., 2013; Little, 2014). Given, however, that in our daily lives we experience faces from multiple angles, it is far from certain that a frontal view is the optimal depiction and several studies even suggested that an individual's appearance can significantly vary depending on the viewing angle (Rule et al., 2009; Jenkins et al., 2011; Tigue et al., 2012; Kościnski and Zalewska, 2017; Sutherland et al., 2017). Faces are, after all, complex and highly variable morphological structures (Enlow et al., 1996) and some facial features are apparent only from some viewing angles (Danel et al., 2018). For example, Danel et al. (2018) reported only a moderate correlation in sexually dimorphic features between lateral and frontal facial configuration in both men and women. When frontal and lateral facial configurations were compared as to their averageness, a significant association was found only in women. It is therefore plausible to assume that complementary information may be provided by different viewing angles. A single frontal view could potentially obscure relevant visual cues used in assessing certain dimensions (e.g., determinants of facial masculinity, such as protrusion of the brow ridge and angularity of the jaw), thus reducing judgment accuracy.

      In research on body perception, the use of other than just frontal view is becoming increasingly common (Tovée and Cornelissen, 2001; Perilloux et al., 2012; Sell et al., 2017; Cornelissen et al., 2018). The use of multiple body angles views allows for assessments of multivariate trait interactions (Brooks et al., 2015). Varying viewing angles of bodies allow raters to assess the shapes and sizes of various morphological characteristics, such as body fat, lean mass distribution, or breast morphology (Dixson et al., 2011, 2015) which all contribute to the resulting attractiveness rating.

      Research on facial perception that employs other than frontal facial views remains, however, at best unsystematic (Kościnski, 2009) and mutual relations between the frontal and lateral dimensions of facial features have so far received very little attention (Danel et al., 2018). Profile views have been used primarily in orthodontics and aesthetic medicine because it is known that they have an impact on facial attractiveness judgments (Spyropoulos and Halazonetis, 2001; Johnston et al., 2005; Maple et al., 2005; Soh et al., 2007; Shafiee et al., 2008; Nomura et al., 2009). Results from several studies that investigated the averageness of facial profiles show patterns analogous to frontal images (Spyropoulos and Halazonetis, 2001; Minear and Park, 2004; Valentine et al., 2004; Valenzano et al., 2006). Some researchers, meanwhile, tried to overcome the limitations of a single view stimulus by presenting raters with both frontal and lateral views of targets on a single screen (e.g., Dixson and Rantala, 2015; Dixson et al., 2016; Valentova et al., 2017), while other studies found a medium to high correlation between the rating of attractiveness of frontal and lateral depictions (ranging from r = 0.52 to 0.83) (Diener et al., 1995; Valenzano et al., 2006; Davidenko, 2007; Shafiee et al., 2008; Kościnski and Zalewska, 2017).

      Until recently, most studies used as stimuli static, two-dimensional images (photographs). Thanks to technological progress, including a considerable increase in computers' computing powers, 3D scanning and 3D reconstruction technology is now becoming more accessible to facial perception research (Toole et al., 1999; Caharel et al., 2009; Chelnokova and Laeng, 2011; Meyer-Marcotty et al., 2011; Jones et al., 2012; Lefevre et al., 2012; Tigue et al., 2012; Berssenbrügge et al., 2014; Mydlová et al., 2015; Holzleitner and Perrett, 2016; Hu et al., 2017; Kordsmeyer et al., 2018). Potential bias associated with a single 2D image (e.g., profile) might be minimized by the use of 3D images, which represent various viewing angles. To our best knowledge, however, only one study directly compared ratings based on 2D and 3D facial images (Tigue et al., 2012). Authors found a high correlation between 2D and 3D stimuli (r = 0.71), with mean ratings significantly higher for 3D images. On the other hand, it should be noted that in this study, only opposite-sex ratings were performed (female faces were rated by male participants), on a single scale (attractiveness), and the only 2D depictions used were frontal portraits.

      Current evidence suggests a rather high level of congruence in judged characteristics (especially attractiveness) between frontal and lateral or frontal and 3D views of faces. It should, however, be taken into account that the development of morphological features between the frontal and lateral view does not always correlate (Danel et al., 2018) and one could thus expect that some socially relevant traits may be easier to assess from other than frontal view (Tigue et al., 2012).

      In the two studies, we estimated the congruence in perception of three different views of male heads (frontal portrait, left profile, and 360° rotation photographs). We employed two characteristics relevant in the context of intra- and inter-sexual selection, namely the rating of formidability and attractiveness. We also explored whether the type of device used (mobile phones, laptop, and desktop computers) influences the ratings.

      Materials and Methods

      All procedures employed in this study conform to the ethical standards of the relevant committee on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of National Institute of Mental Health, Czech Republic (Ref. num. 28/15). All participants were informed about the goals of the study and gave their informed consent. The present study is part of a larger project which investigates multimodal perception of traits associated with sexual selection and characteristics related to competition outcome.

      Targets

      We collected photographs of 45 male Mixed Martial Arts (MMA) athletes (mean age = 26.6, SD = 5.86, range = 18–38). All athletes were from the Czech Republic. They were invited via social media advertisements, leaflets distributed at domestic MMA tournaments, gyms, and with the assistance of Mixed Martial Arts Association Czech Republic (MMAA). All targets were provided with brief description of the project and approved their participation by signing informed consent. As compensation for their participation, they received 400 CZK (approx. €15).

      Acquisition and Settings of the Photographs

      To capture images of the targets' head from all 360°, we built a turning plywood platform (120 cm in diameter) using flat ball bearings. The platform had 36 steps around its perimeter, i.e., one step for every 10°, making it basically a large turntable. To achieve standardization—all photographs were acquired on site—the platform was placed inside a purpose-built portable photographic booth to control for changes in ambient illumination and for color reflections (see e.g., Rowland and Burriss, 2017; Thorstenson, 2018). Booth dimensions were 140 × 140 × 255 cm. Its frame was made of sectioned aluminum profiles. The outside of the booth and the inside of its roof was covered with black duvetyn cloth (a dense fabric), while the internal side of the walls, the seamless backdrop, and surface of the turning platform were covered with a bright white velvet (medium density fabric).

      To achieve standardized lighting conditions, we used one 800 W studio strobe (Photon Europe MSN HSS-800) aimed into a white reflective umbrella used as a light modifier (Photon Europe, 109 cm diameter), mounted on a 175 cm high light stand, and tilted 10° downwards toward the booth. The light was positioned 125 cm from the target. This lighting setup ensured even exposure across the whole scene, which was further verified before each session by a digital light meter (Sekonic L-308S).

      Images were acquired using a 24-megapixel, full-frame (35.9 × 24 mm CMOS sensor, a 35 mm film equivalent) digital SLR camera Nikon D610 equipped with a fixed focal length lens Nikon AF-S NIKKOR 85 mm f/1.8 G (Třebický et al., 2016). Exposure values were set to ISO 100, shutter speed 1/200 s, and aperture f/11. Photographs were shot into 14-bit uncompressed raw files (NEF) and AdobeRGB color space. Color calibration was performed using X-Rite Color Checker Passport color targets and white balance patch photographed at the beginning of each session. The camera was mounted in portrait orientation directly onto the light stand, which carried also the strobe light positioned 125 cm from the target so as to achieve a perception close to social interpersonal distance (Hall, 1966; Baldassare and Feller, 1975; Sorokowska et al., 2017), to maintain a constant perspective distortion (Třebický et al., 2016; Erkelens, 2018), and to avoid potential perception bias based on interpersonal distance (Bryan et al., 2012). Camera's distance from each target was verified with a digital laser rangefinder (Bosch PLR 15) as distance between the sensor plane (marked ϕ on camera body) to the middle of target's forehead. Camera's height was adjusted for each target so as to position the center of his head in the middle of the frame. Focus point was set on target's right eye and focus distance was locked for further images of the target. This setting of camera's distance, focal length, and sensor size yielded a 35 × 53 cm field of view (23.85° angle of view) and the aperture setting resulted in a 9 cm depth of field (4 cm before and 5 cm behind the focal plane).

      Targets were seated on a 63 cm high bar stool (Ikea Franklin) positioned in the middle (rotation axis) of the turning platform. We asked them not to lean against the stool's back support and to sit with their back straight and hands hanging freely alongside their body. They were asked to adopt a neutral facial expression (with no smile or frown), to look directly into the camera, and to remain in this position for all subsequent photographs. When necessary, targets were instructed to adjust their posture and head position, so they were facing the camera straight on, without any head pitch, yaw, or roll. On top of that, they were instructed to wear only black underwear shorts we provided them with (i.e., without T-shirt) and to remove any adornments (glasses, earrings, piercings or other jewelry).

      One full 360° rotation yielded 36 photographs. After each photograph, research assistant manually turned the turning platform by one step (10°) clockwise. We captured two full rotations to obtain one full set of images while eliminating all possible movements, blinks, etc. of targets. Capturing both full rotations took approx. 10 min.

      Stimuli Processing

      All image processing was carried out in Adobe Lightroom Classic CC (Version 2017). First, all images were converted into DNG raw files and DNG color calibration profiles were assembled (using X-Rite Color Checker Passport LR plugin) and applied to all photographs. For each target, a final set of 36 images covering full 360° head rotation was selected and postprocessed by combining suitable images (correct head position, open eyes, closed mouth, etc.) from the two captured rotations. To ensure consistency in exposure across all selected photographs, percentages of Red, Green, and Blue channel values were checked across three background areas (above, left, right) and eventual small differences in exposure were manually adjusted to the same level. In the next step, the calibrated images were exported into lossless 16-bit AdobeRGB TIFF files in their real size of 35 × 53 cm and 168 pixels per inch (ppi) resolution (a native ppi of 4K screens used for rating sessions, see Rating Session in section Formidability Rating). This resulted in life-sized images of targets' heads. Horizontal and vertical positions of images were adjusted using LR Transform tool to position target's head in the center of the frame with eyes in a horizontal line. Final images were batch-cropped to 1:1.1 (2,095 × 2,305) side ratio to fit head rotations of all targets. Images were then converted into sRGB color space and exported as 8-bit JPEG files (2,095 × 2,305 px @ 168 ppi).

      Building 360° Head Rotations

      We used Sirv (www.sirv.com), an online suite for creating and managing image spins, to build 360° head rotations. With all image adjustments and optimization to image size and quality done by Sirv turned off, we uploaded the images of all targets and created the individual spins. See Supplementary Materials for sample 360° head rotation (360 rotation video.MP4).

      Portraits and Profiles

      Analogously to previous research investigating morphological differences between portraits and profiles (Danel et al., 2018), we have selected from the set of 36 images for each target a frontal and left profile image. See Supplementary Materials for sample frontal portrait (Frontal portrait.JPEG) and left profile (Left profile.JPEG).

      Raters Formidability Rating

      Portraits were evaluated by 62 raters (30 men), mean age = 23.1 (SD = 3.45, range = 18–39); profiles by 60 raters (30 men), mean age = 22.8 (SD = 3.55, range = 18–36); and 360° rotations by 94 raters (46 men), mean age = 22.1 (SD = 3.09, range = 18–38) (see Table 2). Raters were mainly Charles University (Prague, Czech Republic) students recruited via social media advertisements, mailing list of participants assembled in previous studies or invited on site. All raters were provided with brief description of the project and approved their participation by signing informed consent. Rating took place in a lab (see section Rating Sessions, Formidability Rating) and when the rating was completed, they received for their participation 100 CZK (approx. €4) and a debriefing leaflet. Using a two-way ANOVA, we found no age differences between sexes, stimuli type ratings, or their interaction [Sex: F(1, 210) = 0.371, p = 0.543; Stimuli type: F(2, 210) = 1.777, p = 0.172; Sex × Stimuli type: F(2, 210) = 0.006, p = 0.994].

      Attractiveness Rating

      Portraits were evaluated by 195 raters (30 men), mean age = 29.6 (SD = 6.05, range = 18–48); profiles by 176 raters (32 men), mean age = 29.2 (SD = 6.26, range = 18–53); and 360° rotations by 150 raters (35 men), mean age = 29 (SD = 6.27, range = 18–46) (see Table 2). Raters were recruited mainly via advertisements among followers of National Institute of Mental Health (facebook.com/nudzcz) and Human Ethology group (facebook.com/etologie) Facebook pages. Ratings were carried out online. All raters provided their informed consent by clicking on the “I agree” button to consent with their participation in the study and were not financially reimbursed. Two-way ANOVA showed no age difference between sexes, the stimuli type ratings or their interaction [Sex: F(1, 515) = 2.553, p = 0.111; Stimuli type: F(2, 515) = 0.162, p = 0.85; Sex × Stimuli type: F(2, 515) = 0.084, p = 0.864]. Table 2 provides detailed descriptive statistics.

      Rating Sessions Formidability Rating

      Formidability ratings were performed in two separate sessions. In the first session, we collected the ratings of 360° rotations. In the second session, raters were randomly divided to rate either a set of portrait or profile images. Each rater thus judged a full set of only one type of stimuli.

      Ratings took place in a quiet perception lab, in standardized conditions across all raters (with artificial lighting and closed window blinds to eliminate changes in ambient lighting). Raters were seated in the same eye level with stimuli's eyes, 125 cm from the screen, i.e., in the same distance as the camera was from the target in order to simulate approximate social interpersonal distance (Hall, 1966; Baldassare and Feller, 1975), and in the center of the projected photograph (Cooper et al., 2012). This was implemented so as to increase the ecological validity of the rating.

      Images were presented to raters on 27′′ Dell U2718Q UltraSharp IPS screens (3,840 × 2,160, 99% sRGB color space coverage) turned to a vertical position to accommodate life-size images. Screens were connected to Asus ROG G20 PC running Microsoft Windows 10 with environment scaling set to 100%. Screens were color- and luminance-calibrated with X-Rite i1 Display Pro probes. The probes were connected during the whole rating session to adjust screens for ambient light. Qualtrics survey suite (Qualtrics, Provo, UT) with Blank theme run through Google Chrome (in full screen mode and 100% scaling) was used for data collection.

      All raters received a set of brief demographics questions (e.g., sex, age, and education status) followed by a block containing stimuli. Images were presented in a randomized order. Raters were asked to rate formidability (“Jak moc by byl tento muž úspěšný, kdyby se dostal do fyzického souboje?”/“If this man was involved in physical confrontation, how successful he would be?”) of each target on a 7-point verbally anchored scale (from “1 – velice neúspěšný”/“very unsuccessful,” to “7 – velice úspěšný”/“very successful”). The 360° rotations spun automatically clockwise once (automatic rotation took approx. 2 s) and raters were instructed to turn the heads around for further inspection by dragging mouse left or right before rating. Portrait and profile photographs were simply projected on the screen. Time for rating was not limited.

      Attractiveness Rating

      Ratings were collected on-line via Qualtrics survey suite (Qualtrics, Provo, UT). All raters were first presented with a brief study description and informed consent. Then they completed a set of demographics questions, which was followed by one randomly selected block of stimuli (portraits, profiles, or 360° rotations). Each rater thus assessed a full set of only one type of stimuli. Images were presented in a randomized order. Raters were asked to rate attractiveness (“Jak atraktivní je muž na fotografii?”/“How attractive is the man on photograph?”) of each target on a 7-point verbally anchored scale (from “1 – velice neatraktivní”/“very unattractive”, to “7 – velice atraktivní”/“very attractive”). The 360° rotations spun automatically once clockwise (automatic rotation took approx. 2 s) and raters were instructed to turn the heads around for further inspection by dragging mouse left or right before rating, while portrait and profile photographs were simply projected on the screen. Time for rating was not limited.

      We used Qualtrics Blank theme and custom CSS code to set the image size to 800 px width with centered margin alignments (.Skin #SkinContent.QuestionBody {width: 800px; display: block; margin-left: auto; margin-right: auto;}.Skin #SkinContent.QuestionText {width: 800px; display: block margin-left: auto; margin-right: auto;}) to standardize stimulus size and position across all devices used. First, raters were asked to switch their web browsers into Full Screen mode and adjust page scaling to achieve the largest image size possible while seeing the rating scale without having to scroll down, i.e., if a Full HD 16:9 screen (1,920 × 1,080) was used for rating in Full Screen mode, browser scaling would remain on native 100%.

      Devices used for attractiveness rating

      When raters completed rating the images, they were asked to specify the type of device they used (mobile phone, tablet device, laptop computer, desktop computer or other), screen size or brand and model name of the device (to later identify screen size and resolution). This data was used to test a possible effect of the device used on the rating.

      In total, attractiveness was rated by 521 raters: 233 used laptop computers, 135 desktop computers, 116 mobile phones, 19 tablet devices, 1 other device, and 17 did not specify device type. See Tables S1 and S2 for data on screen sizes and resolutions.

      In subsequent analyses, we used only data from the three most frequently represented device categories: mobile phones, laptops, and desktop computers. This resulted in a sample of 484 raters.

      Statistical Analysis

      All statistical tests were performed in JASP 0.9.0.1 (JASP Team, 2018) and jamovi 0.9.1.7 (jamovi project, 2018). McDonald's ω statistics was used for estimating inter-rater agreement (Dunn et al., 2014). To test for potential age differences between rater groups, a two-way ANOVA was carried out, with raters' sex and stimuli types entered as two independent variables and age as a dependent variable. Two-way ANOVA was further used to compare sex differences in mean formidability and attractiveness rating, where raters' sex and stimuli types were entered as two independent variables and the rating of formidability or attractiveness as a dependent variable. Effect sizes for two-way ANOVAs are reported in η2. Associations between the ratings of different stimuli types were tested by bivariate correlations using Pearson's r coefficient with 95% CIs [lower limit, upper limit]. For exploratory purposes, we also tested the influence of device on attractiveness rating using a two-way ANOVA with stimulus type and device type entered as independent variables and mean attractiveness rating as a dependent variable. A Holm's post-hoc test was performed and effect sizes for the comparison are reported in Cohen's d.

      Data Availability

      Datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are available as Supplementary Material of this article (Dataset formidability.XLSX, Dataset attractiveness.XLSX).

      Results Formidability Rating

      McDonald's ω scores of male and female ratings showed a high inter-rater agreement across all three stimuli types (ranging from 0.732 to 0.876). In subsequent analyses, we have therefore used mean formidability ratings given to the individual stimuli separately by male and female raters. Further, we found a high correlation between ratings assigned by men and women for portraits (r = 0.941, 95% CI [0.895, 0.967], p < 0.001), profiles (r = 0.962, 95% CI [0.931, 0.979], p < 0.001) and 360° rotations (r = 0.972, 95% CI [0.95, 0.985], p < 0.001).

      Ratings of all three stimuli types were highly correlated (Table 1, Figure 1). Two-way ANOVA showed no main effect of rater's sex [F(1, 264) = 0.00014, p = 0.991, η2 < 0.001], stimulus type [F(2, 264) = 0.473, p = 0.624, η2 = 0.004], or rater's sex × stimulus type interaction [F(2, 264) = 0.01, p = 0.99, η2 < 0.001] on formidability ratings (Figure 2). For descriptive statistics, see Table 2.

      Correlations between stimuli types.

      Scale Stimuli Pearson's r [95 % CI]
      Formidability Portrait – Profile 0.829 [0.708, 0.903]
      Portrait – 360° rotation 0.974 [0.952, 0.985]
      Profile – 360° rotation 0.882 [0.794, 0.934]
      Attractiveness Portrait – Profile 0.706 [0.520, 0.828]
      Portrait – 360° rotation 0.956 [0.921, 0.976]
      Profile – 360° rotation 0.782 [0.634, 0.875]

      All correlations are significant at p < 0.001.

      Correlations between portraits, profiles, and 360° rotations in perceived formidability (upper line) and attractiveness (lower line). Dashed lines indicate 95% CI.

      Differences in mean ratings of formidability (Left) and attractiveness (Right) between stimuli types (portraits, profiles, and 360° rotations). Violin plots show rating distributions, box plots its 25th and 75th percentile. Dark gray violin plots represent female and white violin plots male ratings, respectively. Mean formidability ratings did not differ between sexes, while males rated all stimuli types as more attractive compared to females.

      Descriptive statistics.

      Scale Stimuli Sex N Age Rating McDonald's ω
      Mean SD Range Mean SD Range
      Formidability Portraits Total 62 23.113 3.448 18–39 4.186 1.009 2.25–6.22 0.815
      Female 32 23.219 3.933 18–39 4.193 1.068 2.25–6.22 0.847
      Male 30 23 3.006 20–31 4.179 0.977 2.4–6.07 0.732
      Profiles Total 60 22.816 3.553 18–36 4.085 0.961 2.07–6.27 0.856
      Female 30 22.967 3.222 19–32 4.073 0.994 2.07–6.27 0.876
      Male 30 22.667 3.907 18–36 4.098 0.946 2.07–6.2 0.844
      360° rotations Total 94 22.127 3.094 18–38 4.046 0.998 2.19–6.04 0.798
      Female 48 21.854 4.699 18–38 4.049 0.988 2.19–5.96 0.736
      Male 46 21.957 2.556 19–29 4.042 1.022 2.26–6.04 0.857
      Attractiveness Portraits Total 195 29.6 6.048 18–48 2.803 0.656 1.63–4.67 0.892
      Females 165 29.491 5.854 18–48 2.79 0.654 1.65–4.22 0.9
      Males 30 30.2 7.107 18–41 2.876 0.696 1.63–4.67 0.831
      Profiles Total 176 29.188 6.257 18–53 2.904 0.796 1.63–4.87 0.962
      Female 144 28.951 6.1 18–46 2.84 0.803 1.63–4.82 0.96
      Male 32 30.25 6.924 18–53 3.194 0.784 1.94–4.87 0.964
      360° rotations Total 150 29 6.271 18–46 2.926 0.688 1.69–4.34 0.957
      Female 115 28.687 6.353 18–46 2.909 0.715 1.69–4.34 0.954
      Male 35 30.029 5.968 20–43 2.981 0.616 1.8–4.17 0.966
      Attractiveness Rating

      McDonald's ω scores of male and female ratings showed a high inter-rater agreement in all three stimuli types (ranging from 0.831 to 0.966), which is why in subsequent analyses, we used the mean formidability ratings given to a particular stimulus separately by male and female raters. Ratings by women and men were highly correlated: r = 0.952, 95% CI [0.915, 0.974], p < 0.001; r = 0.969, 95% CI [0.944, 0.983], p < 0.001; r = 0.962, 95% CI [0.932, 0.979], p < 0.001 for portraits, profiles, and 360° rotations, respectively.

      Attractiveness ratings of all three stimuli types were highly correlated (Table 1, Figure 1). Two-way ANOVA showed main effect of rater's sex [F(1, 264) = 3.87, p = 0.05, η2 = 0.014], men rated attractiveness higher as compared to women, but the effect of stimulus type [F(2, 264) = 1.516, p = 0.222, η2 = 0.011], and rater's sex × stimuli interaction [F(2, 264) = 1.118, p = 0.329, η2 = 0.008] on attractiveness ratings was not significant (Figure 2). For descriptive statistics, see Table 2.

      Influence of Device Type on Attractiveness Rating

      To explore whether the type of device used for viewing and rating influences attractiveness rating, we performed a two-way ANOVA with stimuli type and device type as independent factors. The results showed main effects of both device types [F(2, 475) = 7.429, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.030] and stimuli types [F(2, 475) = 4.27, p = 0.015, η2 = 0.017], but no significant interaction between them [F(4, 475) = 1.065, p = 0.373, η2 = 0.008]. Holm's post-hoc comparison showed that raters using mobile phones rated the images as significantly more attractive compared to desktop [t(475) = 3.817, pHolm < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.557] and laptop users [t(475) = 3.023, pHolm = 0.005, Cohen's d = 0.392], whereby the ratings assigned by laptop and desktop users did not differ [t(475) = 1.357, pHolm = 0.175, Cohen's d = 0.145]. 360° rotations were rated significantly higher than portraits [t(475) = 2.912, pHolm = 0.011, Cohen's d = 0.418], but there was no statistical difference between 360° rotations and profiles [t(475) = 1.753, pHolm = 0.16, Cohen's d = 0.212]; and between portraits and profiles [t(475) = 1.366, pHolm = 0.173, Cohen's d = 0.159] (Figure 3). For descriptive statistics, see Table 3. Further, attractiveness ratings between all three types of devices were highly correlated: r = 0.883, 95% CI [0.839, 0.915], p < 0.001; r = 0.885, 95% CI [0.842, 0.917], p < 0.001; r = 0.949, 95% CI [0.93, 0.964], p < 0.001 for mobile phones–laptops, mobile phones–desktops, and laptops–desktops, respectively (Figure 4).

      Differences in the mean ratings of attractiveness between device types. Violin plots show rating distributions, box plots its 25th and 75th percentiles. Dark gray violin plot represents mobile phones, light gray violin plot laptop computers, and white violin plot desktop computers, respectively. Asterisks indicate the level of significance, **p = 0.005, ***p < 0.001.

      Devices and rating descriptive statistics.

      N Mean attractiveness rating SD
      Device Mobile phone 116 3.021 0.625
      Laptop computer 232 2.876 0.668
      Desktop computer 136 2.777 0.634

      Correlations of attractiveness ratings between device types (mobile phones, laptop computers and desktop computers). Dashed lines indicate 95% CI.

      Discussion

      The main aim of this study was to examine whether perception of formidability and attractiveness varies depending on the angle under which a face is viewed. To this purpose, we used standardized sets of frontal portraits, left profiles, and 360° head rotations of male facial images. We found strong correlations between the three types of stimuli and no significant differences in the mean ratings of formidability and attractiveness. Our results thus showed that ratings based on the three different face views were highly congruent and both perceived formidability and attractiveness ratings appear to be view-invariant. As a subsidiary aim, we have also tested the effect of the device used on attractiveness rating. While there were no differences between ratings performed on desktop and laptop computers, ratings performed using the mobile phones were higher (targets were perceived as more attractive).

      Majority of facial perception research uses as stimuli frontal images (Kościnski, 2009), which is in striking contrast with our daily life experience. Moreover, there is a long-standing debate on how human visual system recognizes objects viewed from different angles (Hayward, 2003) and whether object recognition is view-specific, i.e., linked to a specific viewing orientation (Tarr and Bülthoff, 1995), or view-invariant (Biederman and Gerhardstein, 1993). Some evidence suggests that human visual system may be view-specific and process objects differently depending on the viewing angle (Jeffery et al., 2007; but see Jiang et al., 2006). If this were the case, results from perceptual studies that rely solely on frontal portraits could not be generalized to the other viewpoints. Our results, however, at least when it comes to social perception, do not support this hypothesis.

      Our data shows patterns analogous in direction and magnitude to those reported in previous studies that compared assessments based on different stimuli views of both faces and bodies (e.g., frontal × profile, frontal × 3D or oblique poses), which likewise showed strong correlations in ratings (Diener et al., 1995; Tovée and Cornelissen, 2001; Valenzano et al., 2006; Davidenko, 2007; Shafiee et al., 2008; Perilloux et al., 2012; Tigue et al., 2012; Dixson et al., 2015; Kościnski and Zalewska, 2017). A related study by Tigue et al. (2012) reported that attractiveness of frontal and 3D depictions of women's faces as rated by men were highly correlated (r = 0.71) but 3D stimuli received significantly higher mean ratings. Authors suggest that their findings may be an effect of novelty of the 3D visualization. Our study, on the other hand, found no differences between the mean ratings of 2D (frontal and profile) and 3D images. It should be noted, however, that we opted for an alternative to standard 3D visualization. By combining several individual photographs presented in sequence as a spin (360° rotation), we avoided possible bias based simply on differences in capture technology (such as noticeable differences in lighting and colors between 2D and 3D stimuli).

      The 360° rotation photographs allowed us to present raters with an all-around view of stimuli heads without running the cost of acquiring 3D capture technologies. Although the resulting visualizations are indeed photorealistic, there is a notable drawback related to implementing this procedure. The capturing and subsequent processing of the images is rather time-consuming and physically demanding, especially for the photographed targets, because one spin takes approx. 5 min and during this time, targets have to sit completely still with fixed gaze, so that controlling for head tilts, yawns, and rolls thus becomes even more critical (Penton-Voak et al., 2001; Hehman et al., 2013; Sulikowski et al., 2015). The use of 3D stimuli captured with actual 3D scanning and 3D reconstructions technology would allow for a variety of target applications including stimuli capture and presentation. For instance, resulting models could be rotated to arbitrary angles relative to their position during capture, rather than simply displayed in an identical head position in all photographs. Such 3D stimuli would produce more realistic face reconstructions: the main obstacle is the relatively high initial investment into a 3D scanner. Moreover, although 3D facial models are remarkably human-like, they are certainly distinguishable from, and less familiar than, photographs and that could potentially reduce their validity in terms of being a realistic visualization of humans (Crookes et al., 2015). Future studies should investigate whether perception of 3D models differs from 360° rotation photographs.

      Interestingly, we found that the device used for viewing the stimuli has a significant influence on the rating. Raters using mobile phones gave on average higher attractiveness ratings than users of laptop or desktop computers. To our best knowledge, no previous study investigated the influence of the device used for viewing on ratings. Although the screen size and resolution of mobile phones are increasing, screen size of handheld devices does limit the size of images that can be viewed on it. That negatively influences the amount of detailed visual information available to the observer, hence potentially limiting the visibility of cues that may affect some aspects of social perception (such as attractiveness). All this may result in ratings higher than those based on viewing images on larger screens which do show more detail. For instance, several studies have reported that more homogenous skin is perceived as more attractive (Jones et al., 2004; Fink et al., 2006; Tsankova and Kappas, 2016; Jaeger et al., 2018; Tan et al., 2018). It is thus possible that lower visibility of such types of imperfections on mobile phones may lead to higher scores. This is a potentially important issue since ever more researchers opt for online data collection. One ought to take into consideration the kind of devices raters decide to use for their viewing and rating, because if a specific subgroup of raters systematically chooses to use a particular kind of device, it could bias the results. In our case, mobile phones were used by nearly one quarter of raters. The results we report are correlations and we just assume that differences in ratings were influenced by the kind of device raters used. In theory, it is possible that a particular group of raters simultaneously tended to give higher ratings and used mobile phones for viewing and rating. It is likely, however, that the two phenomena are independent of one another, because we found no differences between rater groups in other characteristics.

      A potential limitation of our study is the fact that we used a rather specific sample of targets, namely MMA athletes. This fact might limit the generalization of our results. One could expect that MMA fighters would be perceived as highly formidable opponents, as rather specific in appearance (“cauliflower” ears, broken noses, eyebrow scares, etc.), which is why their ratings of formidability and attractiveness might be less variable and/or skewed. In our study, however, raters were not explicitly told that the targets presented to them are MMA fighters. What we found was that mean formidability rating of all three stimuli types on a 7-point scale ≈ 4 (ranging from 2 to 6.2) and skewness of all three stimuli types were between 0.097 and 0.189 (Table 2) and data followed normal distribution. For attractiveness, mean ratings for all three stimuli types were between 2.8 and 2.93 (ranging from 1.7 to 4.8) and skewness between 0.111 and 0.578 (Table 2), hence comparable to average ratings of male facial attractiveness in other studies (e.g., Saribay et al., 2018). It thus seems that the specific nature of our sample does not impede generalization of our finding. Nevertheless, future studies based on less specific samples should further investigate this issue.

      To conclude, the findings presented here, along with other recent studies, provide converging evidence that single and multiple view facial images convey highly overlapping information and a single angle view contains enough information about the spatial structural elements of a face to congruently assess formidability and attractiveness, at least in the case of male faces. These results also suggest that studies which use different types of stimulus depiction are, generally speaking, comparable: this ought to simplify the interpretation of individual studies.

      Author Contributions

      VT, JF, and JH developed the study concept. KK contributed to the study design. Data collection was performed by VT, JF, DS, and ZŠ. VT performed data analysis and interpretation, VT and JF drafted the manuscript, and DS, ZŠ, KK, and JH provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

      Conflict of Interest Statement

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      We thank Klára Coufalová, Radim Pavelka, Tereza Nevolová, Žaneta Slámová, Pavel Šebesta, Dagmar Schwambergová, and other members of Human Ethology group (www.etologiecloveka.cz) for their help with data collection, Mixed Martial Arts Association Czech Republic (MMAA), Anna Pilátová for English proofreading and all volunteers for their participation.

      Supplementary Material

      The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: /articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02405/full#supplementary-material

      References Baldassare M. Feller S. (1975). Cultural variations in personal space: theory, methods, and evidence. Ethos 3, 481503. 10.1525/eth.1975.3.4.02a00020 Berssenbrügge P. Berlin N. F. Kebeck G. Runte C. Jung S. Kleinheinz J. . (2014). 2D and 3D analysis methods of facial asymmetry in comparison. J. Cranio-Maxillofacial Surg. 42, e327e334. 10.1016/j.jcms.2014.01.02824507934 Biederman I. Gerhardstein P. C. (1993). Recognizing depth-rotated objects: evidence and conditions for three-dimensional viewpoint invariance. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 19, 11621182. 10.1037/0096-1523.19.6.11628294886 Brooks R. C. Shelly J. P. Jordan L. J. W. Dixson B. (2015). The multivariate evolution of female body shape in an artificial digital ecosystem. Evol. Hum. Behav. 36, 351358. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.02.001 Bryan R. Perona P. Adolphs R. (2012). Perspective distortion from interpersonal distance is an implicit visual cue for social judgments of faces. PLoS ONE 7:e45301. 10.1371/journal.pone.004530123028918 Caharel S. Jiang F. Blanz V. Rossion B. (2009). Recognizing an individual face: 3D shape contributes earlier than 2D surface reflectance information. Neuroimage 47, 18091818. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.06519497375 Calder A. J. Rhodes G. Johnson M. Haxby J. (eds.). (2011). Oxford Handbook of Face Perception. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Chelnokova O. Laeng B. (2011). Three-dimensional information in face recognition: an eye-tracking study. J. Vis. 11:27. 10.1167/11.13.2722131448 Cooper E. A. Piazza E. A. Banks M. S. (2012). The perceptual basis of common photographic practice. J. Vis. 12:8. 10.1167/12.5.822637709 Cornelissen P. L. Cornelissen K. K. Groves V. McCarty K. Tovée M. J. (2018). View-dependent accuracy in body mass judgements of female bodies. Body Image 24, 116123. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.12.00729414144 Crookes K. Ewing L. Gildenhuys J. D. Kloth N. Hayward W. G. Oxner M. . (2015). How well do computer-generated faces tap face expertise? PLoS ONE 10:e0141353. 10.1371/journal.pone.014135326535910 Danel D. P. Valentova J. V. Sanchez O. R. Leongomez J. D. Varella M. A. C. Kleisner K. (2018). A cross-cultural study of sex-typicality and averageness: correlation between frontal and lateral measures of human faces. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 30:e23147. 10.1002/ajhb.2314730328226 Davidenko N. (2007). Silhouetted face profiles: a new methodology for face perception research. J. Vis. 7:6. 10.1167/7.4.617461690 Diener E. Wolsic B. Fujita F. (1995). Physical attractiveness and subjective well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69, 120129. Dixson B. J. Lee A. J. Sherlock J. M. Talamas S. N. (2016). Beneath the beard: do facial morphometrics influence the strength of judgments of men's beardedness? Evol. Hum. Behav. 38, 164174. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.08.004 Dixson B. J. Rantala M. J. (2015). The role of facial and body hair distribution in women's judgments of men's sexual attractiveness. Arch. Sex. Behav. 45, 877889. 10.1037/0022-3514.69.1.12026292838 Dixson B. J. Vasey P. L. Sagata K. Sibanda N. Linklater W. L. Dixson A. F. Arch. Sex. Behav. (2011) 40:1271. 10.1007/s10508-010-9680-6 Dixson B. J. W. Duncan M. J. M. Dixson A. F. (2015). The role of breast size and areolar pigmentation in perceptions of women's sexual attractiveness, reproductive health, sexual maturity, maternal nurturing abilities, and age. Arch. Sex. Behav. 44, 16851695. 10.1007/s10508-015-0516-2 Dunn T. J. Baguley T. Brunsden V. (2014). From alpha to omega: a practical solution to the pervasive problem of internal consistency estimation. Br. J. Psychol. 105, 399412. 10.1111/bjop.1204624844115 Enlow D. H. Hans M. H. G. McGrew L. (eds.) (1996). Essentials of Facial Growth. Philadelphia, PA: Saunders Saunders. Erkelens C. (2018). Multiple photographs of a perspective scene reveal the principles of picture perception. Vision 2:26. 10.3390/vision2030026 Fink B. Grammer K. Matts P. J. (2006). Visible skin color distribution plays a role in the perception of age, attractiveness, and health in female faces. Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 433442. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.08.007 Hall E. T. (1966). The Hidden Dimension. New York, NY: Doubleday and Co. Hayward W. G. (2003). After the viewpoint debate: where next in object recognition? Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 425427. 14550482 Hehman E. Leitner J. B. Gaertner S. L. (2013). Enhancing static facial features increases intimidation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 747754. 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.02.015 Holzleitner I. J. Perrett D. I. (2016). Perception of strength from 3D faces is linked to facial cues of physique. Evol. Hum. Behav. 37, 217229. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.11.004 Hu S. Xiong J. Fu P. Qiao L. Tan J. Jin L. . (2017). Signatures of personality on dense 3D facial images. Sci. Rep. 7, 73. 10.1038/s41598-017-00071-528250433 Jaeger B. Wagemans F. M. A. Evans A. M. van Beest I. (2018). Effects of facial skin smoothness and blemishes on trait impressions. Perception 47, 608625. 10.1177/030100661876725829580151 jamovi project (2018). jamovi (Version 0.9). Available online at: https://www.jamovi.org JASP Team (2018). JASP (Version 0.9.0.1). Available online at: https://jasp-stats.org/ Jeffery L. Rhodes G. Busey T. (2007). Broadly tuned, view-specific coding of face shape: opposing figural aftereffects can be induced in different views. Vision Res. 47, 30703077. 10.1016/j.visres.2007.08.01817920099 Jenkins R. White D. Van Montfort X. Mike Burton A. (2011). Variability in photos of the same face. Cognition 121, 313323. 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.00121890124 Jiang F. Blanz V. O'Toole A. J. (2006). Probing the visual representation of faces with adaptation. Psychol. Sci. 17, 493500. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01734.x16771799 Johnston D. J. Hunt O. Johnston C. D. Burden D. J. Stevenson M. Hepper P. (2005). The influence of lower face vertical proportion on facial attractiveness. Eur. J. Orthod. 27, 349354. 10.1093/ejo/cji02315961569 Jones A. L. Kramer R. S. S. Ward R. (2012). Signals of personality and health: the contributions of facial shape, skin texture, and viewing angle. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 38, 13531361. 10.1037/a002707822288693 Jones B. C. Little A. C. Burt D. M. Perrett D. I. (2004). When facial attractiveness is only skin deep. Perception 33, 569576. 10.1068/p346315250662 Kordsmeyer T. L. Hunt J. Puts D. A. Ostner J. Penke L. (2018). The relative importance of intra- and intersexual selection on human male sexually dimorphic traits. Evol. Hum. Behav. 39, 424436. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.03.008 Kościnski K. (2009). Current status and future directions of research on facial attractiveness. Anthropol. Rev. 72, 4565. 10.2478/v10044-008-0015-3 Kościnski K. Zalewska M. (2017). Compatibility of facial perception between frontal and profile view,. in 4th Annual Conference Polish Society for Human and Evolution Studies, Krakow. Lefevre C. E. Lewis G. J. Bates T. C. Dzhelyova M. Coetzee V. Deary I. J. . (2012). No evidence for sexual dimorphism of facial width-to-height ratio in four large adult samples. Evol. Hum. Behav. 33, 623627. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.03.002 Little A. C. (2014). Facial attractiveness. WIREs Cogn. Sci. 5, 621634. 10.1002/wcs.1316 Little A. C. Jones B. C. Debruine L. M. (2011). Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B-Biological Sci. 366, 16381659. 10.1098/rstb.2010.040421536551 Maple J. R. Vig K. W. Beck F. M. Larsen P. E. Shanker S. (2005). A comparison of providers' and consumers' perceptions of facial-profile attractiveness. Am. J. Orthod. Dentofac. Orthop. 128, 690696. 10.1016/j.ajodo.2004.09.03016360907 Meyer-Marcotty P. Stellzig-Eisenhauer A. Bareis U. Hartmann J. Kochel J. (2011). Three-dimensional perception of facial asymmetry. Eur. J. Orthod. 33, 647653. 10.1093/ejo/cjq14621355063 Minear M. Park D. (2004). A lifespan database of adult facial stimuli. Behav. Res. Methods instrum. Comput. 36, 630633. 10.3758/BF03206543 Murphy K. E. (1994). Preference for profile orientation in portraits. Empir. Stud. Arts 12, 17. 10.2190/MUD5-7V3E-YBN2-Q2XJ Mydlová M. Dupej J. Koudelová J. Velemínská J. (2015). Sexual dimorphism of facial appearance in ageing human adults: a cross-sectional study. Forensic Sci. Int. 257:519.e1-519.e9. 10.1016/j.forsciint.2015.09.00826548377 Nomura M. Motegi E. Hatch J. P. Gakunga P. T. Ng'ang'a P. M. Rugh J. D. . (2009). Esthetic preferences of european american, hispanic american, japanese, and african judges for soft-tissue profiles. Am. J. Orthod. Dentofac. Orthop. 135, S87S95. 10.1016/j.ajodo.2008.02.01919362272 Penton-Voak I. S. Jones B. C. Little A. C. Baker S. Tiddeman B. P. Burt D. M. . (2001). Symmetry, sexual dimorphism in facial proportions and male facial attractiveness. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 268, 16171623. 10.1098/rspb.2001.170311487409 Perilloux C. Cloud J. M. Buss D. M. (2012). Women's physical attractiveness and short-term mating strategies. Pers. Individ. Dif. 54, 490495. 10.1016/j.paid.2012.10.028 Rhodes G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 57, 199226. 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.19020816318594 Rowland H. M. Burriss R. P. (2017). Human colour in mate choice and competition. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 372:20160350. 10.1098/rstb.2016.035028533465 Rule N. O. Ambady N. Adams R. B. (2009). Personality in perspective: judgmental consistency across orientations of the face. Perception 38, 16881699. 10.1068/p638420120266 Saribay S. A. Biten A. F. Meral E. O. Aldan P. Třebický V. Kleisner K. (2018). The Bogazici face database: standardized photographs of Turkish faces with supporting materials. PLoS ONE 13:e0192018. 10.1371/journal.pone.019201829444180 Sell A. N. Lukaszewski A. W. Townsley M. (2017). Cues of upper body strength account for most of the variance in men's bodily attractiveness. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 284:20171819. 10.1098/rspb.2017.181929237852 Shafiee R. Korn E. L. Pearson H. Boyd R. L. Baumrind S. (2008). Evaluation of facial attractiveness from end-of-treatment facial photographs. Am. J. Orthod. Dentofac. Orthop. 133, 500508. 10.1016/j.ajodo.2006.04.04818405813 Soh J. Chew M. T. Wong H. B. (2007). An Asian community's perspective on facial profile attractiveness. Community Dent. Oral Epidemiol. 35, 1824. 10.1111/j.1600-0528.2007.00304.x17244134 Sorokowska A. Sorokowski P. Hilpert P. Cantarero K. Frackowiak T. Ahmadi K. . (2017). Preferred interpersonal distances: a global comparison. J. Cross. Cult. Psychol. 48, 577592. 10.1177/0022022117698039 Spyropoulos M. N. Halazonetis D. J. (2001). Significance of the soft tissue profile on facial esthetics. Am. J. Orthod. Dentofac. Orthop. 119, 464471. 10.1067/mod.2001.11365611343017 Sulikowski D. Burke D. Havlíček J. Roberts S. C. (2015). Head tilt and fertility contribute to different aspects of female facial attractiveness. Ethology 121, 10021009. 10.1111/eth.12412 Sutherland C. A. M. Young A. W. Rhodes G. (2017). Facial first impressions from another angle: how social judgements are influenced by changeable and invariant facial properties. Br. J. Psychol. 108, 397415. 10.1111/bjop.1220627443971 Tan K. W. Tiddeman B. Stephen I. D. (2018). Skin texture and colour predict perceived health in Asian faces. Evol. Hum. Behav. 39, 320335. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.02.003 Tarr M. J. Bülthoff H. H. (1995). Is human object recognition better described by geon structural descriptions or by multiple views? Comment on Biederman and Gerhardstein (1993). J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 21, 14941505. 10.1037/0096-1523.21.6.14947490590 Thornhill R. Gangestad S. W. (1999). Facial attractiveness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 452460. 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01403-510562724 Thorstenson C. A. (2018). The social psychophysics of human face color: review and recommendations. Soc. Cogn. 36, 247273. 10.1521/soco.2018.36.2.247 Tigue C. C. Pisanski K. O'Connor J. J. Fraccaro P. J. Feinberg D. R. (2012). Men's judgments of women's facial attractiveness from two- and three-dimensional images are similar. J. Vis. 12:3. 10.1167/12.12.323132932 Toole A. J. O. Price T. Vetter T. Bartlett J. C. Blanz V. (1999). 3D shape and 2D surface textures of human faces: the role of “ averages ” in attractiveness and age. Image Vision Comput. 18, 919. 10.1016/S0262-8856(99)00012-8 Tovée M. J. Cornelissen P. L. (2001). Female and male perception of female physical attractiveness in front-view and profile. Br. J. Psychol. 92, 391402. 10.1348/000712601162257 Třebický V. Fialová J. Kleisner K. Havlíček J. (2016). Focal length affects depicted shape and perception of facial images. PLoS ONE 11:e0149313. 10.1371/journal.pone.014931326894832 Tsankova E. Kappas A. (2016). Facial skin smoothness as an indicator of perceived trustworthiness and related traits. Perception 45, 400408. 10.1177/030100661561674826621963 Valentine T. Darling S. Donnelly M. (2004). Why are average faces attractive? The effect of view and averageness on the attractiveness of female faces. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 11, 482487. 15376799 Valentová J. Roberts S. C. Havlíček J. (2013). Preferences for facial and vocal masculinity in homosexual men: the role of relationship status, sexual restrictiveness, and self-perceived masculinity. Perception 42, 187197. 10.1068/p690923700957 Valentova J. V. Varella M. A. C. Havlíček J. Kleisner K. (2017). Positive association between vocal and facial attractiveness in women but not in men: a cross-cultural study. Behav. Process. 135, 95100. 10.1016/j.beproc.2016.12.005 Valenzano D. R. Mennucci A. Tartarelli G. Cellerino A. (2006). Shape analysis of female facial attractiveness. Vision Res. 46, 12821291. 10.1016/j.visres.2005.10.02416356527

      Funding. This research was supported by Czech Science Foundation (GAČR P407/16/03899S), Charles University Research Centre UNCE 204056 and the Sustainability for the National Institute of Mental Health project, grant number LO1611, with financial support from the Ministry of Education, Youth, and Sports of the Czech Republic under the NPU I program.

      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016www.liwibw.com.cn
      idcqek.com.cn
      www.lykxgm.org.cn
      www.idrwig.com.cn
      qiang1122.net.cn
      nsiyo.com.cn
      sktqbd.com.cn
      www.rmchain.com.cn
      prodent.com.cn
      www.nijmsu.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p