Front. Psychol. Frontiers in Psychology Front. Psychol. 1664-1078 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01010 Psychology Original Research Maggot Instructor: Semi-Automated Analysis of Learning and Memory in Drosophila Larvae Tomasiunaite Urte 1 Widmann Annekathrin 1 2 * Thum Andreas S. 1 3 * 1Department of Biology, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany 2Department of Molecular Neurobiology of Behavior, Georg-August-University Göttingen, Göttingen, Germany 3Department of Genetics, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany

Edited by: Martin Giurfa, UMR5169 Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition Animale (CRCA), France

Reviewed by: Teiichi Tanimura, Kyushu University, Japan; Dennis Mathew, University of Nevada, Reno, United States

*Correspondence: Annekathrin Widmann, annekathrin.widmann@uni-goettingen.de Andreas S. Thum, andreas.thum@uni-leipzig.de

These authors have contributed equally to this work.

This article was submitted to Comparative Psychology, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology

20 06 2018 2018 9 1010 21 03 2018 31 05 2018 Copyright © 2018 Tomasiunaite, Widmann and Thum. 2018 Tomasiunaite, Widmann and Thum

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

For several decades, Drosophila has been widely used as a suitable model organism to study the fundamental processes of associative olfactory learning and memory. More recently, this condition also became true for the Drosophila larva, which has become a focus for learning and memory studies based on a number of technical advances in the field of anatomical, molecular, and neuronal analyses. The ongoing efforts should be mentioned to reconstruct the complete connectome of the larval brain featuring a total of about 10,000 neurons and the development of neurogenic tools that allow individual manipulation of each neuron. By contrast, standardized behavioral assays that are commonly used to analyze learning and memory in Drosophila larvae exhibit no such technical development. Most commonly, a simple assay with Petri dishes and odor containers is used; in this method, the animals must be manually transferred in several steps. The behavioral approach is therefore labor-intensive and limits the capacity to conduct large-scale genetic screenings in small laboratories. To circumvent these limitations, we introduce a training device called the Maggot Instructor. This device allows automatic training up to 10 groups of larvae in parallel. To achieve such goal, we used fully automated, computer-controlled optogenetic activation of single olfactory neurons in combination with the application of electric shocks. We showed that Drosophila larvae trained with the Maggot Instructor establish an odor-specific memory, which is independent of handling and non-associative effects. The Maggot Instructor will allow to investigate the large collections of genetically modified larvae in a short period and with minimal human resources. Therefore, the Maggot Instructor should be able to help extensive behavioral experiments in Drosophila larvae to keep up with the current technical advancements. In the longer term, this condition will lead to a better understanding of how learning and memory are organized at the cellular, synaptic, and molecular levels in Drosophila larvae.

Drosophila larvae aversive olfactory conditioning optogenetics olfactory receptor neurons electric shock mushroom body TH1584/1-1 TH1584/3-1 Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft10.13039/501100001659

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      Various technical and conceptual successes have helped recent research to gradually understand how a brain organizes learning and memory. Although, we still cannot understand and address a number of basic mechanisms, recent achievements are fascinating. Part of this development is due to the work on less complex insect brains, such as that of the fruit fly Drosophila and its larva (Heisenberg, 2003; Gerber and Stocker, 2007; Gerber et al., 2009; Busto et al., 2010; Diegelmann et al., 2013; Waddell, 2013, 2016; Cognigni et al., 2017; Widmann et al., 2017).

      The benefits that the Drosophila larva offers for the analysis of learning and memory are based on several factors. First, the elementary organization of the larval central nervous system consists of only about 10,000 neurons (Dumstrei et al., 2003; Nassif et al., 2003). Second, the availability and robustness of behavioral assays that also allow to specifically address distinct memory phases (Aceves-Pina and Quinn, 1979; Scherer et al., 2003; Widmann et al., 2016). Third, the existence of transgenic techniques, which allow manipulation of neuronal networks, small sets of neurons, or even individually identified neurons (Luan et al., 2006; Pfeiffer et al., 2010; Li et al., 2014). Especially the establishment of a large set of single-cell split-Gal4 lines specific for the larval memory center – the mushroom body (MB) – has to be highlighted (Saumweber et al., 2018). Finally, the establishment of the larval connectome that includes the reconstruction of every individual neuron with all its synapses and synaptic partners (Ohyama et al., 2015; Berck et al., 2016; Jovanic et al., 2016; Schlegel et al., 2016; Eichler et al., 2017). These advantages now allow, for the first time, projects that can purposefully investigate – by using thousands of newly established genetic tools – how learning and memory are organized at the level of the brain, the nerve cell and the synapse.

      The study of large amounts of different transgenic animals is simplified by the use of automated methods for behavioral research. However, in contrast to the adult Drosophila, these techniques are unavailable for the analysis of learning and memory in larvae (Colomb et al., 2009; Schnaitmann et al., 2010; Aso and Rubin, 2016; Ichinose and Tanimoto, 2016). The majority of behavioral learning assays in use are based on the principle of classical conditioning (aka Pavlovian conditioning) (Pavlov, 1927). In such studies, a biologically active stimulus (e.g., appetitive stimulus: food; aversive stimulus: electric shock), the unconditioned stimulus (US), is paired with a previously neutral stimulus (e.g., an odor), the conditioned stimulus (CS).

      For almost 40 years (Aceves-Pina and Quinn, 1979), standard assays have been used on agar or agarose-filled Petri dishes and are very robust, easy to learn, inexpensive and require no complex technology (Gerber and Stocker, 2007). At the same time, however, such assays are time-consuming and labor-intensive, as the larvae have to be manually transferred to different Petri dishes during the entire experiment. In total, depending on the applied training regime, the conditioning of one group of animals using standard assays requires an average of 45–60 min. Consequently, this condition makes standard assays suitable to a limited extent for use in large behavioral screens. However, given the establishment of thousands of different genetic tools manipulating precisely the larval brain at the cellular and molecular level, such screens are becoming more important (Li et al., 2014; Saumweber et al., 2018). To use these resources extensively for larval learning and memory research, behavioral experiments or at least parts of them should be automated.

      Thus, we designed the Maggot Instructor, a device to train Drosophila larvae in an automated fashion. The applied behavioral protocol uses electric shock as US paired with the artificial activation of a single olfactory receptor neuron (ORN) as CS (instead of a real odor). Drosophila larvae receive olfactory stimuli via the dorsal organ, a single sensillum located on the right and left sides of the head, with each housing 21 ORNs (Singh and Singh, 1984; Oppliger et al., 2000; Fishilevich et al., 2005; Kreher et al., 2005). For a specific odor, the dedicated ORNs or combinations of ORNs perceive the respective sensory information and signal it further to the larval main olfactory center – the antennal lobe (AL) (Fishilevich et al., 2005; Kreher et al., 2005; Ramaekers et al., 2005). All ORNs connect directly in a one-to-one fashion to 21 uniglomerular projection neurons (PNs). Most of the uniglomerular PNs in turn are directly connected to single-claw Kenyon cells (KC) in the MB calyx region (Eichler et al., 2017). Therefore, for almost every input channel, a direct connection from an ORN (first order) to a PN (second order) to a KC (third order) exists. As a consequence, optogenetically, individual ORN input channels can be activated to generate odor-specific learning and memory in the MB via simultaneous application of a US (Honda et al., 2014). However, in addition to this labeled line pathway, 14 additional multiglomerular PNs exists and initially about 100 KCs (in young L1 larvae) are randomly associated to two or more PNs (Berck et al., 2016; Eichler et al., 2017). These neurons can process odor information at different levels in a more integrative fashion.

      To artificially activate the defined neurons, sophisticated optogenetic methods, which benefit from the semitransparent cuticle of the larvae, have been introduced (Schroll et al., 2006; Dawydow et al., 2014; Rohwedder et al., 2015). By using a two-part expression system, such as the Gal4/UAS system (Brand and Perrimon, 1993), proteins like channelrhodopsin2 (ChR2) or its improved variant ChR2-XXL (Schroll et al., 2006; Dawydow et al., 2014), a light-activated cation channel, can be possibly expressed to depolarize neurons by blue light in a time-wise precisely controlled manner. Single-cell specificity for ORNs can be achieved by using an established set of Or-Gal4 lines that use different Or promoter gene fragments to direct Gal4 expression to individual neurons (Fishilevich et al., 2005). Double-activation learning and memory experiments also become possible by replacing sugar reward (the US) by thermogenetic activation of octopaminergic (OA) neurons with the dTrpA1 channel and odor stimuli (the CS) by optically activating an ORN with ChR2 (Honda et al., 2014). This experiment is feasible as OA and dopamine (DA) neurons mediate sugar reward information in the larval brain (Selcho et al., 2014; Rohwedder et al., 2016; Saumweber et al., 2018). By contrast, the perception of electric shock by the Drosophila larva remains unelucidated. However, the DA system is also sufficient and necessary for aversive olfactory learning and memory in the larvae (Selcho et al., 2009). Four DA neurons innervating the vertical lobe, the lateral appendix, and the lower peduncle of the MB are possibly crucial for signaling aversive stimuli (Eichler et al., 2017).

      The current model suggests that during training, a certain pattern of KCs activated by an odor (or in our case by artificial activation by light) occurs simultaneously with a modulatory signal about the aversive or appetitive US mediated by different sets of DA neurons (Heisenberg, 2003; Waddell, 2013, 2016). Coincident activation of KCs will in turn change the synaptic connectivity of KCs onto extrinsic MB output neurons (MBONs). Thus, during learning, MBONs change their response properties and act as odor-specific neurons that report the presence of a particular odor as an alerting signal for the conditioned behavior. The Maggot Instructor automates this step by executing the behavioral training protocol independently in a high-throughput manner.

      Materials and Methods Fly Stocks (Keeping and Crossing)

      Fly strains were reared on standard Drosophila medium at 25°C in complete darkness. Or42b-Gal4 (Bloomington Stock No: 9972), Or47a-Gal4 (Bloomington Stock No: 9982), UAS-ChR2-XXL (Bloomington Stock No: 58374) and w1118 (obtained from R. Stocker) were used. Strains crossed with w1118 served as controls. For all the behavioral experiments, the flies were transferred to new vials and allowed to lay eggs for 2 days. Third instar feeding-stage larvae aged 96–144 h were used for behavioral experiments.

      Assay Plates and Odors

      Petri dishes (85 mm diameter; Cat. No. 82.1472, Sarstedt, Nümbrecht) were used as the test plates, as described previously (Pauls et al., 2010b; Huser et al., 2012, 2017; Gerber et al., 2013). The test plates and training chambers were filled with 2.5% agarose (Sigma-Aldrich, Cat. No. A9539, CAS No. 9012-36-6). In several behavioral experiments 0.01 M lithium chloride (Sigma-Aldrich, Cat. No. 298328, CAS No. 85144-11-2) was mixed with 2.5% agarose. Throughout the test, the Petri dishes were covered with perforated lids for an equal distribution of odors. All the experiments were performed at about 21°C. As olfactory stimuli in the test we used 10 μl amyl acetate (AM, Sigma-Aldrich, Cat. No. 46022; CAS No. 628-63-7; diluted at 1:100, 1:250, 1:500, 1:600 and 1:750 in paraffin oil, Sigma-Aldrich, Cat. No. 76235, CAS No. 8012-95-1), benzaldehyde (BA, undiluted; Sigma-Aldrich, Cat. No. 12010, CAS No. 100-52-7) and ethyl acetate (EA, Sigma-Aldrich, Cat. No. 270989; CAS No. 141-78-6; diluted 1:1000 in paraffin oil, Sigma-Aldrich, Cat. No. 76235, CAS No. 8012-95-1). Odorants were loaded into custom-made Teflon containers (4.5-mm diameter) with perforated lids (Scherer et al., 2003) and were used for no longer than 5 h after preparation.

      Experimental Setup/Compact Real-Time Input Output (cRIO)

      The Maggot Instructor consists of a training box wired with a computer that controls the type and timing of the applied stimuli via a cRIO system and an automated training device (ATD) (Graetzel et al., 2010; Kain et al., 2012; Dylla et al., 2017). cRIO (NI 9074) from National Instruments was used as a controlling device for the automated training protocol. cRIO was also used to regulate and monitor the technical aspects, such as the fine adjustment of parameters (e.g., light intensity, voltage, or temperature). The software Build Digital Output Sequence with Frequency Output (BDOS) was used for programming cRIO (Dylla et al., 2017). All settings in cRIO were transmitted to the training box (see below), where the parameters, including electric shock or light intensity were adjusted appropriately. Larval training was carried out in an elongated metal box (the training box), which was separated into 10 training chambers with the same size and can be regulated in parallel or individually. Each chamber consists of a case with an electrode at the front and rear end, a Peltier element underneath the chamber and odor inlets and outlets on all four sides. The training chamber is closed by a lid, which contains a white and a blue LED.

      Training Protocol

      Only L3 larvae that are in the feeding stage were used. This requirement was achieved by collecting the larvae from the top layer only of the food substrate. Ten groups with 30 larvae each were collected, washed with tap water, and stored in a water drop for up to 30 min before the experiment. To avoid artificial activation of ORNs in the experimental animals, these steps were performed under red light. Before the experiment, the training chambers were filled with 2.5% agarose to cover the entire bottom with a substrate layer of about 1 cm thickness. After the preparation, the larvae were transferred to the training chambers. The larvae from every genotype were used in each run. For several runs, the training chambers were consistently varied for each genotype. Several runs were possible per training chamber with the same agarose substrate. To prevent the larvae from escaping the training chambers, a custom-made plastic frame covered with a plastic net was inserted into each training chamber. This technique was established by Khurana et al. (2009). This method also prevented the larvae from climbing the training chamber and thus avoiding electric shock. The training chambers were also moistened with about 1 ml of tap water to ensure the proper hydration of the larvae. Afterward, the lids of each training chamber and the cover of the Maggot Instructor were closed. The device was switched on, and the previously defined training protocol was started. All the training steps including CS (if not otherwise mentioned at a light intensity of about 86,000 lux) and US (if not otherwise mentioned electric shock of 120 V) application, then ran automatically. The training lasted for 60 min.

      After training, the cover of the Maggot Instructor and the lids of each training chamber were removed. For the test, the larvae from each training chamber were placed on a fresh, pure agarose assay plate with an odor container on the one side and a second container without olfactory cue on the other side. The sides were randomly changed for every training chamber. All the larvae from one training chamber located on the plastic frames and the agarose cover bottom were collected and transferred. The larvae were placed in the center of the Petri dish, the lid was closed, and the larvae were given 5 min to freely move on the test plate. Ten test plates were analyzed in parallel (one for each training chamber). A Preference Index was calculated by subtracting the number of larvae on the control container side (CC) from the number of larvae on the odor side (ODOR) and dividing the result by the total number of larvae on both sides and in the middle zone (TOTAL):

      Preference Index=(#ODOR#CC)/#TOTAL

      The positive values indicate attraction to the odor, whereas the negative values represent aversion.

      Statistical Analysis

      All data processing, statistical analyses, and visualizations were conducted with GraphPad Prism 7.0a. Figure alignments were performed with Adobe Photoshop CC. The groups that showed no violation of the assumption of normal distribution (Shapiro–Wilk test) and homogeneity of variance (Bartlett’s test) were analyzed with parametric statistics. One-way ANOVA was applied followed by planned pairwise comparisons between the relevant groups with a Tukey’s honestly significant difference post hoc test (comparisons between groups larger than two). Experiments with data that significantly differed from the assumptions above were analyzed with the non-parametric Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s multiple pairwise comparison. To compare single genotypes against chance level, we used one sample t-test or Wilcoxon signed-rank test. The significance level of statistical tests was set to 0.05. Data were presented as box plots, with 50% of the values of a given genotype being located within the boxes and the whiskers representing the entire set of data. Outsiders are indicated as dots. The median performance index was indicated as a bold line and the mean as a cross within the box plot.

      Results Maggot Instructor: A Custom-Made, Automated Approach to Train Larvae

      A comprehensive set of standardized behavioral assays is available to analyze learning and memory in Drosophila larvae (Gerber and Stocker, 2007; Widmann et al., 2017). These approaches all require the larvae to be transferred manually several times from one Petri dish to another during the procedure and are thus labor intensive. To overcome this limitation, we aimed to develop a new, robust, and easy-to-handle device, which we named Maggot Instructor, to train Drosophila larvae in an automated fashion. The device consists of a training box connected to a computer that controls the type and timing of the applied stimuli via a cRIO system and an ATD (Figures 1A,D) (Dylla et al., 2017). Both are programmed by simple and flexible customizable training protocols using a BDOS software (Dylla et al., 2017). The training box consists of 10 separate training chambers that can be regulated in parallel or individually (Figures 1A,B). Therefore, one can train up to 10 groups of larvae in this device in parallel to increase the throughput. Each training chamber consists of a case, in which an electrode is incorporated at the front and the rear end (Figure 1C, above). In addition, a Peltier element is placed underneath the chamber and the odor inlets and outlets on all four sides (Figure 1C, above). The training chamber is closed at the top by a lid equipped with a white and a blue LED (Figure 1C, below). Therefore, the larvae can be exposed to the following stimuli: cold, heat, air, electric shock, and light (white and blue). Additional technical details are included in Figure 1, in Section “Materials and Methods,” or are available upon request. Our initial study focused on a protocol that automatically conditions the larvae by optogenetic activation of ORNs (CS) via blue light and stimulation through electric shock (US).

      Semi-automated conditioning device. (A) Schematic overview of the Maggot Instructor. Setup consists of a computer, compactRIO (compact Real-time Input Output), maggot stimulator and a training box. The training box is split in ten training chambers to parallelize larval training. Each training chamber has a source of light and electric shock. (B) Shows the training box on top and its cover at the bottom. (C) Shows a training chamber at the top and its lid that includes two LEDs at the bottom. (D) Shows the compactRIO system and the connected custom-made automated training device. b, w, v, and t show the connections for the blue and white light, the voltage channel and the temperature channel, respectively.

      Training Procedure

      As shown in several studies, Drosophila larvae can establish an aversive olfactory memory by associating an odor with an electric shock (Aceves-Pina and Quinn, 1979; Heisenberg et al., 1985; Tully et al., 1994; Khurana et al., 2009; Pauls et al., 2010a). The current model suggests that the olfactory information is signaled from ORNs via PNs to MB KCs (Ramaekers et al., 2005). MB KCs, which are third-order olfactory neurons, are also stimulated via DANs, which signal a negative reinforcement (Selcho et al., 2009). When both stimuli coincide, synaptic plasticity occurs. These changes imply that in the following test, MBONs can be addressed by the learned odor to trigger the learned behavior (Figures 2A,B). In the standard assays, odors are used as CS. However, extensive preliminary tests have shown that using odors lead to different problems, including sticking to the agarose substrate in the training chamber (data not shown). Agarose is required to provide a substrate on which larvae can crawl easily and to prevent the larvae from drying out (Apostolopoulou et al., 2014). For this reason, we decided to train the larvae not with real odors but through the optogenetic activation of individual ORNs. Honda et al. (2014) have shown that the artificial optogenetic activation of a single ORN is sufficient to induce an associative olfactory memory in Drosophila larvae.

      Aversive learning paradigm. (A) The neuronal circuit involved is depicted as the olfactory pathway (CS, on top) and the electric shock pathway (US, bottom). Olfactory information is perceived by only 21 olfactory receptor neurons (ORN) and further processed at the antennal lobe (AL). Second order projections neurons (PN) signal onto third order Kenyon Cell of the mushroom body (MB). There are 21 uni-glomerular PNs and 14 multi-glomerular PNs. Electric shocks are perceived and processed by yet unknown neurons. Further downstream likely four dopaminergic neurons (DAN) signal onto the MB lobes, where CS and US converge. (B) The applied protocol uses blue light activation of the single ORN 47a via Channelrhodopsin2-XXL (ChR2-XXL). Further downstream at the MB this information converges with the applied electric shock dependent activation of DANs. (C) Composition of one training cycle. One cycle comprises a 60 s blue light phase, in which last 30 s an electric shock is applied, and a 300 s darkness phase. The training cycle is repeated ten times. (D) Schematic description of the testing agarose plate. During the testing phase larvae were placed in the beginning in the neutral zone and were left on the plate for 5 min to make a decision between the presented odor (odor container; pink) and control container (empty or containing paraffin oil; turquoise). After testing, all larvae on the odor container side, the control container side, and in the neutral zone were counted. (E) Timescale of the larvae training and testing procedure. CS, conditioned stimulus (blue light); US, unconditioned stimulus (electric shock).

      The two-odor reciprocal training paradigm is a widely used method to study associative olfactory learning and memory in larvae (Aceves-Pina and Quinn, 1979; Gerber and Stocker, 2007; Schipanski et al., 2008; Eschbach et al., 2011; von Essen et al., 2011; El-Keredy et al., 2012; Widmann et al., 2017). The use of a similar design would therefore allow for the comparison of larval odor-taste and odor-electric shock learning and memory in general. However, in an early study, we have shown that this design features several caveats (Pauls et al., 2010a). (i) The method yields relative low performance scores and thus may cause difficulty in the comparative studies of genetically manipulated larvae. (ii) This drawback may be partially overcome by increasing the number of training cycles but trigger starvation-dependent effects. (iii) The two-odor design causes a sequence effect as differences are observed in the performance depending on whether the first (CS1) or second odor (CS2) has been punished. To overcome these concerns, we decided to use exactly the same one-odor non-reciprocal training design parameters, which we have established in our previous work (Pauls et al., 2010a).

      The automated training protocol consists of a 60 s blue light phase, in which an electric shock is applied during the last 30 s, followed by a 300 s resting phase in complete darkness (Figures 2C,E). The training trial is repeated 10 times (from now on called 10-cycle training). Immediately thereafter, the larvae are tested for 5 min for their odor preference for a specific odor over paraffin oil, which serves as the control (Figure 2D). The test therefore requires a manual step.

      Pairing Optogenetic Or47a Activation With Electric Shock Reduces Larval Preferences for Amyl Acetate

      To demonstrate that Drosophila larvae can be trained in an automated fashion via the Maggot Instructor, different parameters had to be tested in advance. We used the artificial blue-light dependent activation of Or42b-Gal4 and Or47a-Gal4 crossed with UAS-ChR2-XXL to specifically activate ORN 42b and 47a, respectively (Dawydow et al., 2014; Honda et al., 2014). Both lines were reported to be single-cell-specific (Fishilevich et al., 2005). ORN 47a was reported to specifically encode the odor amyl acetate (AM), whereas ORN 42b encodes the odor ethyl acetate (EA) (Kreher et al., 2005; Hoare et al., 2011).

      We initially focused our analysis on ORN 47a and checked whether the larvae that express ChR2-XXL in ORN 47a can perceive odors. The larvae were tested for their naïve olfactory choice behavior between an odor-filled container on one side and a container without olfactory cue on the other side of a Petri dish (Figure 3). This test was performed with either AM or benzaldehyde (BA) as odor stimuli (Figures 3A,B). Or47a-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL larvae are attracted by the odor AM (Figure 3C). This behavioral response shows no significant difference from both the control groups (Or47-Gal4/+ and UAS-ChR2-XXL/+) (Figure 3C). Similarly, BA is attractive to Or47a-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL larvae, and the response is comparable in both control groups (Or47-Gal4/+ and UAS-ChR2-XXL/+) (Figure 3D). We concluded that the expression of ChR2-XXL in ORN 47a exerts no influence on the naïve odor perception of the larvae.

      Naïve olfactory choice for amyl acetate and benzaldehyde. (A) Schematic representation of naïve olfactory choice for amyl acetate. Olfactory perception is analyzed by putting about 30 larvae in the middle of a Petri dish with an amyl acetate containing odor container (AM, red) on one side and an paraffin oil containing container (CC, turquoise) on the other side. After 5 min larvae are counted to calculate an olfactory preference index. (B) Schematic representation of naïve olfactory choice for amyl acetate. Olfactory perception is analyzed by putting 30 larvae in the middle of a Petri dish with a benzaldehyde containing odor container (BA, green) on one side and an empty container (CC, turquoise) on the other side. After 5 min larvae are counted to calculate an olfactory preference index. (C) The behavioral response for amyl acetate (1:500 dilution) of Or47a-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL, Or47a-Gal4/+ and UAS-ChR2-XXL/+ larvae were statistically not significant from each other (Kruskal–Wallis, p = 0.0.118). All three groups showed an olfactory preference index statistically significantly different from zero (one sample t-test, p < 0.0001, for all three groups). (D) The behavioral response for benzaldehyde (undiluted) in Or47a-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL, Or47a-Gal4/+ and UAS-ChR2-XXL/+ larvae were statistically not significant from each other (one-way ANOVA, p = 0.5757). All three groups showed an olfactory preference index statistically significantly different from zero (one sample t-test, p = 0.0196, p = 0.0012, p < 0.0001, respectively). Differences between groups are depicted below the respective box plots, at which ns indicates p ≥ 0.05. Small circles indicate outliers. Sample size is indicated with the letter n.

      We then tested whether the activation of ORN 47a, together with an electric shock leads to a reduction in the odor preference for AM (Figure 4). This reduction would indicate that an aversive olfactory memory was formed. We performed five different experiments in which the light intensity and the voltage of the electric shock remained unchanged during training, but the dilution of AM in paraffin oil in the test was either 1:100, 1:250, 1:500, 1:600, or 1:750 (Figures 4BF). During training via the Maggot Instructor, all larvae received the 10-cycle training as described before (Figures 2E, 4A). As a result, we observed that for the dilutions 1:100, 1:250, and 1:500, the Or47a-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL larvae showed a reduced olfactory preference for AM compared with both genetic control groups (Or47a-Gal4/+ and UAS-ChR2-XXL/+) (Figures 4BD). No difference was observed between the three groups when the dilution of AM was 1:600 or 1:750 in the test (Figures 4E,F). These results suggest that associative olfactory conditioning using the Maggot Instructor is feasible, and Drosophila larvae are very likely able to establish an aversive odor-electric shock memory. However, the memory can only be revealed at high odor concentrations. The olfactory preference for AM for both the control groups (Or47a-Gal4/+ and UAS-ChR2-XXL/+) statistically significantly differed from each other when a dilution of 1:500 was used (Figure 4D). Nevertheless, we decided to continually use this odor dilution as the experimental group (Or47a-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL) features a specific behavioral phenotype in comparison with both the control groups (Or47a-Gal4/+ and UAS-ChR2-XXL/+), and we have used the lowest possible odor concentration to avoid the harmful side effects.

      Pairing optogenetic Or47a activation with electric shock leads to the formation of odor-electric shock learning and memory in Drosophila larvae tested at lower amyl acetate dilutions. (A) Timescale of associative conditioning using 10 cycles, 120 V for electric shocks and continuous blue light with an intensity of 100%. For the olfactory preference test amyl acetate with different dilutions (1:100, 1:250, 1:500, 1:600, and 1:750) was used. (B) The expression of ChR2-XXL in ORN 47a led to a reduction of olfactory preference for amyl acetate at a dilution of 1:100 (Tukey post hoc test, p < 0.0001, p = 0.0003, respectively). All three groups showed (an olfactory preference for amyl acetate statistically significant from zero (one sample t-test, p < 0.0001 for all three groups). (C) The expression of ChR2-XXL in ORN 47a led to a reduction of olfactory preference for amyl acetate at a dilution of 1:250 (Dunn’s multiple pairwise comparison, p = 0.0.0035, p = 0.0307, respectively). All three groups showed olfactory preferences for amyl acetate statistically significant from zero (one sample t-test, p < 0.0001 for all three groups). (D) The expression of ChR2-XXL in ORN 47a led to a reduction of olfactory preference for amyl acetate at a dilution of 1:500 (one-way ANOVA, p < 0.0001). However, both control groups (Or47-Gal4/+ and UAS-ChR2-XXL/+) exhibited olfactory preferences, which are statistically significant form each other (Tukey post hoc test, p = 0.0001). All three groups showed an olfactory preference for amyl acetate statistically significant from zero (one sample t-test, p < 0.0001 for all three groups). (E) All three groups showed olfactory preferences for amyl acetate at a dilution of 1:600, which are statistically significant from zero (one sample t-test, p < 0.0001 for all three groups) but statistically not significant from each other (one-way ANOVA, p = 0.057). (F) All three groups showed olfactory preferences for amyl acetate at a dilution of 1:750, which are statistically significant from zero (one sample t-test, p = 0.0002, p < 0.0001, p = 0.0002, respectively) but statistically not significant from each other (one-way ANOVA, p = 0.0746). Differences between groups are depicted below the respective box plots, at which ns indicates p ≥ 0.05. Different lowercase letters indicate statistical significant differences at level p < 0.05. Small circles indicate outliers. Sample size is indicated with the letter n.

      The Performance After Maggot Instructor Training Depends on the Applied Electric Shock and Light Intensities

      Next, we performed a parametric analysis with varying voltage of the applied electric shock and intensity of the artificial blue light activation (Figures 5, 6). We used the established 1:500 AM dilution and the 10-cycle protocol (Figure 5A) and tested whether electric shocks applied at 60, 90, or 120 V cause different effects on learning and memory (Figures 5BD). As a result, we noted that for electric shocks of 60 and 120 V, in contrast to 90 V, Or47a-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL larvae showed a reduced olfactory preference for AM compared with both the genetic control groups (Or47a-Gal4/+ and UAS-ChR2-XXL/+) (Figures 5BD). Based on this results, we continually used 120 V for electric shocks, as all larvae survived this treatment and showed slightly stronger differences between the experimental group and both controls.

      Odor-electric shock learning and memory in Drosophila larvae depends on the applied voltage of the electric shock. (A) Timescale of associative conditioning using 10 cycles, different voltages for electric shocks (60, 90, and 120 V) and continuous blue light with an intensity of 100%. For the olfactory preference test amyl acetate with a dilution of 1:500 was used. (B) Using 60 V in the training procedure led to a reduction of olfactory preferences for Or47-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL larvae compared to both genetic controls (Or47-Gal4/+ and UAS-ChR2-XXL/+) (Tukey post hoc test, p = 0.001, p = 0.0168, respectively). Both genetic controls showed olfactory preferences, which are statically significant from zero (one sample t-test, p < 0.0001 for both groups), whereas Or47-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL larvae showed an olfactory preference, which is not statistically significant from zero (one sample t-test, p = 0.068). (C) Using 90 V in the training procedure led to a reduction of olfactory preferences for all three groups, which are statistically not significant from each other (one-way ANOVA, p = 0.5917). All three groups showed olfactory preferences, which are statically significant from zero (one sample t-test, p = 0.0375, p = 0.0004, p = 0.0025, respectively). (D) The olfactory preference for amyl acetate conditioned with 120 V was already analyzed in Figure 4D and is just shown for comparison. Differences between groups are depicted below the respective box plots, at which ns indicates p ≥ 0.05. Different lowercase letters indicate statistically significant differences at level p < 0.05. Small circles indicate outliers. Sample size is indicated with the letter n.

      Odor-electric shock learning and memory in Drosophila larvae is dependent on the intensity of the blue light. (A) Timescale of associative conditioning using 10 cycles, 120 V for electric shocks and continuous blue light with different intensities (50, 75, and 100%). For the olfactory preference test amyl acetate with a dilution of 1:500 was used. (B) Using a light intensity of 50% in the training procedure led to olfactory preferences, which are statistically significant within the three groups (one-way ANOVA, p = 0.0288). However, the difference was only statistically significant between Or47-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL and Or47-Gal4/+ larvae (Tukey post hoc test, p = 0.0222), whereas the olfactory preferences for Or47-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL and UAS-ChR2-XXL/+ larvae were not statistically significant from each other (Tukey post hoc test, p = 0.2906). (C) Using a light intensity of 75% in the training procedure led to olfactory preferences, which are statistically not significant from each other (one-way ANOVA, p = 0.0522). All three groups showed olfactory preferences, which are statically significant from zero (one sample t-test, p < 0.0001, p < 0.0001, p = 0.007, respectively). (D) The olfactory preference for amyl acetate conditioned with 120 V was already analyzed in Figure 4D and is just shown for comparison. Differences between groups are depicted below the respective box plots, at which ns indicates p ≥ 0.05. Different lowercase letters indicate statistically significant differences at level p < 0.05. Small circles indicate outliers. Sample size is indicated with the letter n.

      Next, we used the 1:500 AM dilution, 10-cycle, and 120 V protocol (Figure 6A) to test whether three different blue light intensities (50%, 75%, or 100%) cause different effects on learning and memory (Figures 6BD). We noted that for blue light intensities of 100%, Or47a-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL experimental larvae showed a reduced olfactory preference for AM compared with both genetic controls (Or47a-Gal4/+ and UAS-ChR2-XXL/+) (Figure 6D). By contrast, when trained with blue light intensities of 50% and 75%, the Or47a-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL larvae showed no significant reduction in their preference for AM compared with both or at least one genetic control (Figures 6B,C; for blue light intensities of 50%, a significant difference was observed between Or47a-Gal4/+ and Or47a-Gal4/ChR2-XXL). Based on this result, we used a blue light intensity of 100% for follow-up experiments.

      Lithium Chloride Application or Pulsed Blue-Light Causes no Improvement in the Training Protocol

      Previous studies that used LiCl reported an increase in larval memory scores for odor-electric shock learning as it makes the agarose substrate electrically conductive while being tasteless for larvae (Aceves-Pina and Quinn, 1979). However, this effect could not be confirmed by a study from our laboratory (Pauls et al., 2010a). Nonetheless, we determined whether the use of LiCl affects the automated Maggot Instructor training as its intake might cause harmful effects for the larvae and was reported to modulate adult behavior (Ries et al., 2017). The obtained data revealed that the use of LiCl is not necessary in our setup (Figure 7B), similar to our published data (Pauls et al., 2010a).

      The usage of lithium chloride (LiCl) or pulsed light does not have a significant effect of odor-electric shock learning and memory. (A) Timescale of associative conditioning using 10 cycles, 120 V for electric shocks and continuous blue light with an intensity of 100%. For the olfactory preference test amyl acetate with a dilution of 1:500 was used. (B) Mixing LiCl at a concentration of 0.01 M into agarose led to a olfactory preference for amyl acetate, which differs statistically significant between Or47-Gal4/+ and Or47-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL larvae (Dunn’s multiple comparison, p = 0.0094) but not between UAS-ChR2-XXL/+ and Or47-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL larvae (Dunn’s multiple comparison, p = 0.2697) and both control groups (Dunn’s multiple comparison, p = 0.6232). All three groups showed olfactory preferences for amyl acetate statistically significant from zero (Wilcoxon signed rank test, p = 0.0005, p = 0.001, p = 0.001, respectively). (C) Using pulsed blue light with an intensity of 100% for the optogenetic activation of Or47a led to olfactory preference for Or47-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL larvae, which is statistically significant to both control groups (Tukey post hoc test, p = 0.0254, p = 0.0346, respectively). All three groups showed olfactory preferences, which are statistically significant from zero (one sample t-test, p < 0.0001 for all three groups). (D) The olfactory preference for amyl acetate conditioned with a continuous blue light intensity of 100% and without adding LiCl was already analyzed in Figure 4D and is shown for comparison. Differences between groups are depicted below the respective box plots, at which ns indicates p ≥ 0.05. Different lowercase letters indicate statistical significant differences at level p < 0.05. Small circles indicate outliers. Sample size is indicated with the letter n.

      Prolonged blue-light activation of the sensory neurons via ChR2-XXL can lead to a decrease in firing of the cells (Dawydow et al., 2014). Therefore, we tested whether pulsed blue light activation of ORN 47a may produce a stronger behavioral effect. Instead, of a constant blue light activation of 60 s we used an alternating 1 s on-off regime. In this case, Or47a-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL experimental larvae showed a significant reduction in their odor preference compared with the Or47a-Gal4/+ and UAS-ChR2-XXL/+ control groups (Figure 7C). Direct comparison of the performance of Or47a-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL larvae at pulsed light (Figure 7C) and constant light (Figure 7D) showed a significant difference in the odor preference between both groups. This result indicates that the optogenetic activation with pulsed light featured a weaker effect on reducing odor preferences for AM than with constant light. Therefore, we continually used the 1:500 AM dilution, 10-cycle, 120 V, and 100% constant blue light protocol on the agarose filled training chambers without LiCl.

      Additional Control Experiments Support the Associative Nature of the Learning and Memory Phenotype

      The conditioning regime used by the Maggot Instructor lacks reciprocity. The regime defines learning and memory as a reduction in AM preference between an experimental group and two genetic control groups. We thus designed two additional control experiments to ensure that neither blue light activation nor electric shock stimulation alone specifically can change the AM preference of Or47a-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL larvae (Figures 8A,B). Although unlikely, significant differences between the experimental and control groups would suggest that the obtained phenotype would be based on non-associative effects rather than associative learning and memory. As expected, both results showed no reduction in the AM preference of the Or47a-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL larvae compared with the Or47a-Gal4/+ and UAS-ChR2-XXL/+ control groups (Figures 8C,D). These results show that the observed behavioral change in the experimental larvae after conditioning via the Maggot Instructor is based on associative learning and memory.

      Odor-electric shock learning and memory depends on the simultaneous blue light activation and electric shock stimulation. (A) Timescale of associative conditioning using 10 cycles, continuous blue light with an intensity of 100%, without electric shock. For the olfactory preference test amyl acetate with a dilution of 1:500 was used. (B) Timescale of associative conditioning using 10 cycles, 120 V for electric shocks, but without continuous blue light. For the olfactory preference test amyl acetate with a dilution of 1:500 was used. (C) Associative conditioning without electric shock stimulation but optogenetic Or47a activation led to olfactory preferences, which are statistically not significant within the three groups (one-way ANOVA, p = 0.4062). All three groups showed olfactory preferences, which are statistically significant from zero (one sample t-test, p < 0.0001 for all three groups). (D) Associative conditioning without optogenetic Or47a activation but electric shock stimulation led to olfactory preferences, which are statistically not significant within the three groups (one-way ANOVA, p = 0.3355). All three groups showed olfactory preferences, which are statistically significant from zero (one sample t-test, p < 0.0001 for all three groups). (E) The olfactory preference for amyl acetate conditioned with 120 V was already analyzed in Figure 4D and is just shown for comparison. Differences between groups are depicted below the respective box plots, at which ns indicates p ≥ 0.05. Different lowercase letters indicate statistical significant differences at level p < 0.05. Small circles indicate outliers. Sample size is indicated with the letter n.

      Artificial Activation of Distinct ORNs Establishes Odor-Specific Memories

      Next, we analyzed the odor specificity of the memory. Studies previously showed that artificial activation of a ORN during conditioning induces an odor-specific memory that overlaps with the response profile predicted for the respective ORN (Honda et al., 2014). Accordingly, we tested whether the artificial activation of ORN 47a can also establish odor-electric shock learning and memory for an odor that is not covered by the reported Or47a response profile. Considering Or47a, such case applies to BA (Kreher et al., 2005; Hoare et al., 2011; Munch and Galizia, 2016). As expected Or47a-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL larvae showed an odor preference for BA, and this preference is indistinguishable from the both genetic control groups (Or47a-Gal4/+ and UAS-ChR2-XXL/+) (Figure 9B). Based on this result we conclude that odor-electric shock learning and memory established after training via the Maggot Instructor is specific for the activated ORN and thus overlaps with its reported response profile. We confirmed this result independently by reproducing the finding published for Or42b. Honda et al. (2014) reported that the artificial activation of ORN 42b paired with an artificial activation of octopaminergic neurons that encode for a rewarding function establishes an appetitive olfactory memory specific for EA. Using our standardized training protocol but the odor EA (1:1000) in the test (Figure 10A) Or42b-Gal4/UAS-ChR2-XXL larvae also established an aversive odor-electric shock memory (Figure 10B).

      Artificial activation of ORN 47a establishes an odor-electric shock memory, which is specific for amyl acetate. (A) Timescale of associative conditioning using 10 cycles, 120 V for electric shocks and continuous blue light with an intensity of 100%. For the olfactory preference test benzaldehyde (undiluted) was used. (B) Using benzaldehyde in the test led to olfactory preferences, which are statistically not significant within the three groups (one-way ANOVA, p = 0.254). All three groups showed olfactory preferences, which are statistically significant from zero (one sample t-test, p = 0.0007, p = 0.0004, p < 0.0001, respectively). Differences between groups are depicted below the respective box plots, at which ns indicates p ≥ 0.05. Different lowercase letters indicate statistical significant differences at level p < 0.05. Small circles indicate outliers. Sample size is indicated with the letter n.

      Establishing an odor-electric shock memory through artificial activation of specific ORNs and simultaneous stimulation is a general property of Drosophila larvae. (A) Timescale of associative conditioning using 10 cycles, 120 V for electric shocks and continuous blue light with an intensity of 100%. For the olfactory preference test ethyl acetate (1:1000) was used. Here, continuous blue light activates Or42b, which has a reported response profile for ethyl acetate. (B) The artificial activation of Or42b and using ethyl acetate in the test led to olfactory preferences for Or42b/UAS-ChR2-XXL, which are statistically significant to both control groups (Or42b/+ and UAS-ChR2-XXL/+) (Tukey post hoc test, p = 0.0133, p = 0.0398, respectively). All three groups showed olfactory preferences, which are statistically significant from zero (one sample t-test, p < 0.0001, for all three groups). Differences between groups are depicted below the respective box plots, at which ns indicates p ≥ 0.05. Different lowercase letters indicate statistical significant differences at level p < 0.05. Small circles indicate outliers. Sample size is indicated with the letter n.

      Discussion The Maggot Instructor Trains Larvae in an Automated Fashion to Establish an Associative Olfactory Memory

      Drosophila larvae can establish different types of associative memory based on the pairing of two stimuli (US and CS) (Aceves-Pina and Quinn, 1979; Scherer et al., 2003; Gerber and Stocker, 2007; Widmann et al., 2017). In contrast to the almost exclusively manual assays that are currently in use, we showed that larvae can also be trained automatically with the help of the Maggot Instructor. Automation will allow one to conduct comprehensive behavioral screens of newly established genetic tools (Li et al., 2014; Saumweber et al., 2018). In several experiments, we have shown that genetically modified larvae, which still show a natural naïve odor preference (Figure 3), learn the temporal paired optogenetic activation of ORN 47a with an electric shock and store this experience as an aversive olfactory memory (Figures 47, 10). Our results showed that this memory is specific for the identity and concentration of odors as the odor-electric shock memory was only detectable at certain concentrations of AM (Figure 4) and not visible when BA was used in the test (Figure 9). The conclusion regarding the associative nature of the observed reduction in the AM preference is compelling as we also showed that other parameters per se, such as artificial activation and electric shock, caused no alteration in the tested olfactory behavior (Figure 8). Therefore, we conclude that training larvae via the Maggot Instructor leads to an odor-specific associative process. The formation of memory by artificial activation of ORNs is not limited to ORN 47a given that an EA memory can be formed through the activation of ORNS 42b (Figure 10). However, for each of the 21 ORNs, odor-specific associative processes have to be tested, as several studies have shown the presence of non-equivalency among larval ORNs (Mathew et al., 2013; Hernandez-Nunez et al., 2015; Newquist et al., 2016). ORN 42a, for instance, unlike many other larval ORNs was shown to respond to a wide range of odors (Kreher et al., 2005; Hoare et al., 2011; Mathew et al., 2013).

      Real World Stimulation or Artificial Activation of Distinct Neurons of the Learning and Memory Network

      To establish an associative olfactory memory in Drosophila larvae, the animals with natural stimuli, such as an odor and an electric shock, must be conditioned (Aceves-Pina and Quinn, 1979; Pauls et al., 2010a). However, the precise control of natural stimuli often presents difficulty. Therefore, thermogenetic and optogenetic effectors, such as TRPA1 and ChR2, that are expressed via transgenic techniques provide an alternative as they allow for the precise control of the activity of defined neurons in living larvae (Hamada et al., 2008; Dawydow et al., 2014). Associative olfactory conditioning theoretically includes the CS (odor) and/or the US (reward/punishment) pathways. Schroll et al. (2006) showed that light-induced activation of a set of DA neurons paired with an odor stimulus induces aversive memory formation, whereas activation of OA neurons induces appetitive memory formation. These results could be extended by demonstrating that in downstream of the OA neurons, the activity of four DA pPAM is also sufficient to trigger an appetitive memory (Rohwedder et al., 2016). For two of these DA neurons, activating them individually is enough for memory formation (Saumweber et al., 2018). In summary, these studies showed that substation experiments can be possibly carried out for the US in the larva, both for appetitive and for aversive learning, up to the single-cell level. This condition also holds true for the adult Drosophila. By contrast, a successful CS substitution at the level of ORN has thus far only been shown for the larva stage (Honda et al., 2014). Perhaps, the reason is the simpler neural network or the organization of parts of the larval olfactory pathway as a labeled line up to the MB (Ramaekers et al., 2005; Berck et al., 2016; Eichler et al., 2017). The optogenetic activation of ORN 24a and ORN 42b paired with the thermogenetic activation of most OA neurons induces an appetitive memory for acetophenone and EA, respectively (Honda et al., 2014). In this study, we showed for the first time the establishment of an aversive memory via CS substitution (Figure 10). Taken together the activation of ORN 42b serves the classical CS function. The pairing of ORN 42b activation via a natural odor or artificially via blue light and a reward or punishment causes the CS to trigger attraction or avoidance. As a consequence, appetitive and aversive associative learning processes can now be generated artificially, temporally, and spatially in various combinations in the larval brain and independent of natural stimuli. In this situation, the Maggot Instructor can be helpful. Thus, in future experiments, the order of CS and US, their precise timing (e.g., backward and forward conditioning; delay conditioning), and additional parameters, such as the number of training cycles or the strength of the CS and the US, can be analyzed in a controlled manner. The same condition applies to the neuronal networks. Activation experiments for PNs, sets of KCs, MBONs, and screens for identifying neurons of the US pathway would be conceivable.

      Meaning of the Artificial ORN Activation

      The associative olfactory learning and memory that we tested with ORN 47a was specific for AM (Figures 49). However, we opted not to analyze in-depth the odor specificity of the memory. The tuning curve for the receptor Or47a is very specific at low odor concentrations (10-4) and responded almost exclusively to AM when tested for 26 different odors (Kreher et al., 2008). This result was also confirmed by a second study, which has tested for 19 different odors (Hoare et al., 2011). We used these results to select Or47a for our experiments. At a higher concentration (10-2), the receptor specificity changes, and in addition to AM, one also sees responses to other odors, such as propyl acetate, isoamyl acetate, 1-octen-3-ol, and 2-heptanone. For the receptor Or42b, this condition is very similar. At low concentrations (10-4) Or42b shows high specificity for EA. At high concentrations (10-2) responses for ethyl butyrate, propyl acetate, 2,3-butanedione and potential AM are reported (Kreher et al., 2008; Hoare et al., 2011). The high throughput rate of the Maggot Instructor allows repetition of these physiological experiments at the behavioral level to identify the tuning curves for each ORN in relation to many odors after olfactory learning and memory. These experiments would provide more information on the neural principles of larval odor processing to better understand the odors that larvae can learn and remember.

      Technical Caveats

      The Maggot Instructor shortens the time necessary to perform an experiment. The manual training protocol consists of 60 s CS and US pairing followed by a 300 s resting phase in complete darkness (Figures 2C,E). This training trial is repeated 10 times and spans 60 min in total (Pauls et al., 2010a). Although the Maggot Instructor, compared with the manual protocol, requires about the same time to prepare the larvae before and test them after training, the training itself requires no handling. A standard experiment usually consists of an experimental group, a driver and reporter control, each with about 10 repetitions per genotype. This situation results in a time of approximately 3 (genotypes) × 10 (repetitions) × 60 min, or 30 h saved per complete experiment.

      Although this rough estimate shows the immense time saved, one must also mention that large genetic screens cannot be achieved immediately. The Maggot Instructor requires ChR2 to be expressed in individual ORNs. This goal can be achieved either via direct Or promoter ChR2 fusion constructs, via the LexA, the Q, or the Gal4/UAS system (Brand and Perrimon, 1993; Lai and Lee, 2006; Potter et al., 2010). However, as these tools are either non-existent, rare, or problematic, and as they affect other genetic modifications, genetic screens require a special strategy to deploy the Maggot Instructor. For example establishing the Or47::ChR2-XXL larvae would be possible. This construct can either be combined with a MB-Gal4 line to screen for the requirement of individual genes using available UAS-RNAi lines or with UAS-shits to use available Gal4 and split-Gal4 lines to identify the neuronal circuits and individual neurons required for learning and memory (Kitamoto, 2001; Pfeiffer et al., 2010; Li et al., 2014). Alternatively, one can combine Or-LexA with LexAop-ChR2-XXL to artificially activate individual ORNs (Selcho et al., 2017). However, to date, to our knowledge, only Or47b-LexA (Hueston et al., 2016), which is not expressed in the larval olfactory system, has been published; thus, one would have to establish in any case new genetic tools before one can use the Maggot Instructor for large genetic screens.

      Outlook

      In this work, we exclusively focused on the aversive olfactory memory reinforced with electric shock. The design of the Maggot Instructor, however, allows a whole series of other applications. Drosophila larvae can also associate odor information with light or heat punishment (von Essen et al., 2011; Khurana et al., 2012). The Maggot Instructor can apply these stimuli automatically. Furthermore, the Maggot Instructor offers the possibility to analyze associative visual learning and memory by pairing a light stimulus with electric shock. Such a protocol is already established as a manual assay (von Essen et al., 2011).

      Extensive double activation experiments are also now possible. Defined ORN activation (standardized CS) can then be paired with activation of individual sensory neurons expressing gustatory receptors, ionotropic receptors, transient receptor potential cation channels, and/or pickpocket ion channel genes (Clyne, 2000; Dunipace et al., 2001; Scott et al., 2001; Liu et al., 2003; Montell, 2005; Benton et al., 2009). In this manner, one could comprehensively identify the sensory neurons that encode for appetitive and aversive reinforcement in Drosophila larvae (e.g., Gr93a for aversive reinforcement and IR60c potentially for appetitive reinforcement) (Apostolopoulou et al., 2016; Croset et al., 2016).

      In summary, the range of applications of the Maggot Instructors extends well beyond the one shown here. Therefore, we confidently present in this work a very useful device that allows more rapid analysis of the behavioral, neuronal, and molecular fundamentals and different forms of larval learning and memory in the future.

      Author Contributions

      UT conceived the study, coordinated and contributed behavioral experiments, analyzed behavioral data, designed figures, and wrote the manuscript. AW conceived the study, coordinated behavioral experiments, analyzed behavioral data, designed figures, and wrote the manuscript. AT conceived of and coordinated study, analyzed the data, designed figures, and wrote the manuscript.

      Conflict of Interest Statement

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Funding. This work was supported by the DFG grants (TH1584/1-1 and TH1584/3-1) and the Zukunftskolleg of the University of Konstanz (all to AT).

      We thank Dr. Tilman Triphan, Dr. Wolf Hütteroth, Dr. Astrid Rohwedder, and Dr. Dennis Pauls for their fruitful comments on the manuscript. Additionally, we thank Lyubov Pankevych and Margarete Ehrenfried for fly care and maintenance. In addition, we want to express our special thanks to the workshop of the University of Konstanz for constructing the Maggot Instructor. We also thank Johanna Wörner for her help in establishing the device. Special thanks also go to Prof. Brian Smith for his help and advice.

      References Aceves-Pina E. O. Quinn W. G. (1979). Learning in normal and mutant Drosophila larvae. Science 206 9396. 10.1126/science.206.4414.93 17812455 Apostolopoulou A. A. Hersperger F. Mazija L. Widmann A. Wust A. Thum A. S. (2014). Composition of agarose substrate affects behavioral output of Drosophila larvae. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:11. 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00011 24478658 Apostolopoulou A. A. Kohn S. Stehle B. Lutz M. Wust A. Mazija L. (2016). Caffeine taste signaling in Drosophila larvae. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 10:193. 10.3389/fncel.2016.00193 27555807 Aso Y. Rubin G. M. (2016). Dopaminergic neurons write and update memories with cell-type-specific rules. eLife 5:e16135. 10.7554/eLife.16135 27441388 Benton R. Vannice K. S. Gomez-Diaz C. Vosshall L. B. (2009). Variant ionotropic glutamate receptors as chemosensory receptors in Drosophila. Cell 136 149162. 10.1016/j.cell.2008.12.001 19135896 Berck M. E. Khandelwal A. Claus L. Hernandez-Nunez L. Si G. Tabone C. J. (2016). The wiring diagram of a glomerular olfactory system. eLife 5:e14859. 10.7554/eLife.14859 27177418 Brand A. H. Perrimon N. (1993). Targeted gene expression as a means of altering cell fates and generating dominant phenotypes. Development 118 401415. 8223268 Busto G. U. Cervantes-Sandoval I. Davis R. L. (2010). Olfactory learning in Drosophila. Physiology 25 338346. 10.1152/physiol.00026.2010 21186278 Clyne P. J. (2000). Candidate taste receptors in Drosophila. Science 287 18301834. 10.1126/science.287.5459.1830 Cognigni P. Felsenberg J. Waddell S. (2017). Do the right thing: neural network mechanisms of memory formation, expression and update in Drosophila. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 49 5158. 10.1016/j.conb.2017.12.002 29258011 Colomb J. Kaiser L. Chabaud M. A. Preat T. (2009). Parametric and genetic analysis of Drosophila appetitive long-term memory and sugar motivation. Genes Brain Behav. 8 407415. 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2009.00482.x 19220480 Croset V. Schleyer M. Arguello J. R. Gerber B. Benton R. (2016). A molecular and neuronal basis for amino acid sensing in the Drosophila larva. Sci. Rep. 6:34871. 10.1038/srep34871 27982028 Dawydow A. Gueta R. Ljaschenko D. Ullrich S. Hermann M. Ehmann N. (2014). Channelrhodopsin-2-XXL, a powerful optogenetic tool for low-light applications. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111 1397213977. 10.1073/pnas.1408269111 25201989 Diegelmann S. Klagges B. Michels B. Schleyer M. Gerber B. (2013). Maggot learning and Synapsin function. J. Exp. Biol. 216(Pt 6), 939951. 10.1242/jeb.076208 23447663 Dumstrei K. Wang F. Nassif C. Hartenstein V. (2003). Early development of the Drosophila brain: V. Pattern of postembryonic neuronal lineages expressing DE-cadherin. J. Comp. Neurol. 455 451462. 10.1002/cne.10484 12508319 Dunipace L. Meister S. McNealy C. Amrein H. (2001). Spatially restricted expression of candidate taste receptors in the Drosophila gustatory system. Curr. Biol. 11 822835. 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00258-5 11516643 Dylla K. V. Raiser G. Galizia C. G. Szyszka P. (2017). Trace conditioning in Drosophila induces associative plasticity in mushroom body Kenyon cells and dopaminergic neurons. Front. Neural Circuits 11:42. 10.3389/fncir.2017.00042 28676744 Eichler K. Li F. Litwin-Kumar A. Park Y. Andrade I. Schneider-Mizell C. M. (2017). The complete connectome of a learning and memory centre in an insect brain. Nature 548 175182. 10.1038/nature23455 28796202 El-Keredy A. Schleyer M. Konig C. Ekim A. Gerber B. (2012). Behavioural analyses of quinine processing in choice, feeding and learning of larval Drosophila. PLoS One 7:e40525. 10.1371/journal.pone.0040525 22802964 Eschbach C. Cano C. Haberkern H. Schraut K. Guan C. Triphan T. (2011). Associative learning between odorants and mechanosensory punishment in larval Drosophila. J. Exp. Biol. 214(Pt 23), 38973905. 10.1242/jeb.060533 22071180 Fishilevich E. Domingos A. I. Asahina K. Naef F. Vosshall L. B. Louis M. (2005). Chemotaxis behavior mediated by single larval olfactory neurons in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 15 20862096. 10.1016/j.cub.2005.11.016 16332533 Gerber B. Biernacki R. Thum J. (2013). Odor-taste learning assays in Drosophila larvae. Cold Spring Harb. Protoc. 2013:pdb.prot071639. 10.1101/pdb.prot071639 23457337 Gerber B. Stocker R. F. (2007). The Drosophila larva as a model for studying chemosensation and chemosensory learning: a review. Chem. Senses 32 6589. 10.1093/chemse/bjl030 17071942 Gerber B. Stocker R. F. Tanimura T. Thum A. S. (2009). Smelling, tasting, learning: Drosophila as a study case. Results Probl. Cell Differ. 47 139185. 10.1007/400_2008_9 19145411 Graetzel C. F. Nelson B. J. Fry S. N. (2010). Frequency response of lift control in Drosophila. J. R. Soc. Interface 7 16031616. 10.1098/rsif.2010.0040 20462877 Hamada F. N. Rosenzweig M. Kang K. Pulver S. R. Ghezzi A. Jegla T. J. (2008). An internal thermal sensor controlling temperature preference in Drosophila. Nature 454 217220. 10.1038/nature07001 18548007 Heisenberg M. (2003). Mushroom body memoir: from maps to models. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4 266275. 10.1038/nrn1074 12671643 Heisenberg M. Borst A. Wagner S. Byers D. (1985). Drosophila mushroom body mutants are deficient in olfactory learning. J. Neurogenet. 2 130. 10.3109/01677068509100140 4020527 Hernandez-Nunez L. Belina J. Klein M. Si G. Claus L. Carlson J. R. (2015). Reverse-correlation analysis of navigation dynamics in Drosophila larva using optogenetics. eLife 4:e06225. 10.7554/eLife.06225 25942453 Hoare D. J. Humble J. Jin D. Gilding N. Petersen R. Cobb M. (2011). Modeling peripheral olfactory coding in Drosophila larvae. PLoS One 6:e22996. 10.1371/journal.pone.0022996 21857978 Honda T. Lee C. Y. Yoshida-Kasikawa M. Honjo K. Furukubo-Tokunaga K. (2014). Induction of associative olfactory memory by targeted activation of single olfactory neurons in Drosophila larvae. Sci. Rep. 4:4798. 10.1038/srep04798 24762789 Hueston C. E. Olsen D. Li Q. Okuwa S. Peng B. Wu J. (2016). Chromatin modulatory proteins and olfactory receptor signaling in the refinement and maintenance of fruitless expression in olfactory receptor neurons. PLoS Biol. 14:e1002443. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002443 27093619 Huser A. Eschment M. Gullu N. Collins K. A. N. Bopple K. Pankevych L. (2017). Anatomy and behavioral function of serotonin receptors in Drosophila melanogaster larvae. PLoS One 12:e0181865. 10.1371/journal.pone.0181865 28777821 Huser A. Rohwedder A. Apostolopoulou A. A. Widmann A. Pfitzenmaier J. E. Maiolo E. M. (2012). The serotonergic central nervous system of the Drosophila larva: anatomy and behavioral function. PLoS One 7:e47518. 10.1371/journal.pone.0047518 23082175 Ichinose T. Tanimoto H. (2016). Dynamics of memory-guided choice behavior in Drosophila. Proc. Jpn. Acad. Ser. B Phys. Biol. Sci. 92 346357. 10.2183/pjab.92.346 27725473 Jovanic T. Schneider-Mizell C. M. Shao M. Masson J. B. Denisov G. Fetter R. D. (2016). Competitive disinhibition mediates behavioral choice and sequences in Drosophila. Cell 167 858.e19870.e19. 10.1016/j.cell.2016.09.009 27720450 Kain J. S. Stokes C. de Bivort B. L. (2012). Phototactic personality in fruit flies and its suppression by serotonin and white. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109 1983419839. 10.1073/pnas.1211988109 23150588 Khurana S. Abu Baker M. B. Siddiqi O. (2009). Odour avoidance learning in the larva of Drosophila melanogaster. J. Biosci. 34 621631. 10.1007/s12038-009-0080-9 19920347 Khurana S. Robinson B. G. Wang Z. Shropshire W. C. Zhong A. C. Garcia L. E. (2012). Olfactory conditioning in the third instar larvae of Drosophila melanogaster using heat shock reinforcement. Behav. Genet. 42 151161. 10.1007/s10519-011-9487-9 21833772 Kitamoto T. (2001). Conditional modification of behavior in Drosophila by targeted expression of a temperature-sensitive shibire allele in defined neurons. J. Neurobiol. 47 8192. 10.1002/neu.1018 11291099 Kreher S. A. Kwon J. Y. Carlson J. R. (2005). The molecular basis of odor coding in the Drosophila larva. Neuron 46 445456. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.007 15882644 Kreher S. A. Mathew D. Kim J. Carlson J. R. (2008). Translation of sensory input into behavioral output via an olfactory system. Neuron 59 110124. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.06.010 18614033 Lai S. L. Lee T. (2006). Genetic mosaic with dual binary transcriptional systems in Drosophila. Nat. Neurosci. 9 703709. 10.1038/nn1681 16582903 Li H. H. Kroll J. R. Lennox S. M. Ogundeyi O. Jeter J. Depasquale G. (2014). A GAL4 driver resource for developmental and behavioral studies on the larval CNS of Drosophila. Cell Rep. 8 897908. 10.1016/j.celrep.2014.06.065 25088417 Liu L. Johnson W. A. Welsh M. J. (2003). Drosophila DEG/ENaC pickpocket genes are expressed in the tracheal system, where they may be involved in liquid clearance. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100 21282133. 10.1073/pnas.252785099 12571352 Luan H. Peabody N. C. Vinson C. R. White B. H. (2006). Refined spatial manipulation of neuronal function by combinatorial restriction of transgene expression. Neuron 52 425436. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.028 17088209 Mathew D. Martelli C. Kelley-Swift E. Brusalis C. Gershow M. Samuel A. D. (2013). Functional diversity among sensory receptors in a Drosophila olfactory circuit. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110 E2134E2143. 10.1073/pnas.1306976110 23690583 Montell C. (2005). Drosophila TRP channels. Pflugers Arch. 451 1928. 10.1007/s00424-005-1426-2 15952038 Munch D. Galizia C. G. (2016). DoOR 2.0–comprehensive mapping of Drosophila melanogaster odorant responses. Sci. Rep. 6:21841. 10.1038/srep21841 26912260 Nassif C. Noveen A. Hartenstein V. (2003). Early development of the Drosophila brain: III. The pattern of neuropile founder tracts during the larval period. J. Comp. Neurol. 455 417434. 10.1002/cne.10482 12508317 Newquist G. Novenschi A. Kohler D. Mathew D. (2016). Differential contributions of olfactory receptor neurons in a Drosophila olfactory circuit. eNeuro 3:ENEURO.004516.2016. 10.1523/ENEURO.0045-16.2016 27570823 Ohyama T. Schneider-Mizell C. M. Fetter R. D. Aleman J. V. Franconville R. Rivera-Alba M. (2015). A multilevel multimodal circuit enhances action selection in Drosophila. Nature 520 633639. 10.1038/nature14297 25896325 Oppliger F. Y. Guerin P. M. Vlimant M. (2000). Neurophysiological and behavioural evidence for an olfactory function for the dorsal organ and a gustatory one for the terminal organ in Drosophila melanogaster larvae. J. Insect Physiol. 46 135144. 10.1016/S0022-1910(99)00109-2 12770245 Pauls D. Pfitzenmaier J. E. Krebs-Wheaton R. Selcho M. Stocker R. F. Thum A. S. (2010a). Electric shock-induced associative olfactory learning in Drosophila larvae. Chem. Senses 35 335346. 10.1093/chemse/bjq023 20212010 Pauls D. Selcho M. Gendre N. Stocker R. F. Thum A. S. (2010b). Drosophila larvae establish appetitive olfactory memories via mushroom body neurons of embryonic origin. J. Neurosci. 30 1065510666. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1281-10.2010 20702697 Pavlov I. P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Pfeiffer B. D. Ngo T. T. Hibbard K. L. Murphy C. Jenett A. Truman J. W. (2010). Refinement of tools for targeted gene expression in Drosophila. Genetics 186 735755. 10.1534/genetics.110.119917 20697123 Potter C. J. Tasic B. Russler E. V. Liang L. Luo L. (2010). The Q system: a repressible binary system for transgene expression, lineage tracing, and mosaic analysis. Cell 141 536548. 10.1016/j.cell.2010.02.025 20434990 Ramaekers A. Magnenat E. Marin E. C. Gendre N. Jefferis G. S. Luo L. (2005). Glomerular maps without cellular redundancy at successive levels of the Drosophila larval olfactory circuit. Curr. Biol. 15 982992. 10.1016/j.cub.2005.04.032 15936268 Ries A. S. Hermanns T. Poeck B. Strauss R. (2017). Serotonin modulates a depression-like state in Drosophila responsive to lithium treatment. Nat. Commun. 8:15738. 10.1038/ncomms15738 28585544 Rohwedder A. Selcho M. Chassot B. Thum A. S. (2015). Neuropeptide F neurons modulate sugar reward during associative olfactory learning of Drosophila larvae. J. Comp. Neurol. 523 26372664. 10.1002/cne.23873 26234537 Rohwedder A. Wenz N. L. Stehle B. Huser A. Yamagata N. Zlatic M. (2016). Four individually identified paired dopamine neurons signal reward in larval Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 26 661669. 10.1016/j.cub.2016.01.012 26877086 Saumweber T. Rohwedder A. Schleyer M. Eichler K. Chen Y. C. Aso Y. (2018). Functional architecture of reward learning in mushroom body extrinsic neurons of larval Drosophila. Nat. Commun. 9:1104. 10.1038/s41467-018-03130-1 29549237 Scherer S. Stocker R. F. Gerber B. (2003). Olfactory learning in individually assayed Drosophila larvae. Learn. Mem. 10 217225. 10.1101/lm.57903 12773586 Schipanski A. Yarali A. Niewalda T. Gerber B. (2008). Behavioral analyses of sugar processing in choice, feeding, and learning in larval Drosophila. Chem. Senses 33 563573. 10.1093/chemse/bjn024 18511478 Schlegel P. Texada M. J. Miroschnikow A. Schoofs A. Huckesfeld S. Peters M. (2016). Synaptic transmission parallels neuromodulation in a central food-intake circuit. eLife 5:e16799. 10.7554/eLife.16799 27845623 Schnaitmann C. Vogt K. Triphan T. Tanimoto H. (2010). Appetitive and aversive visual learning in freely moving Drosophila. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 4:10. 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00010 20300462 Schroll C. Riemensperger T. Bucher D. Ehmer J. Voller T. Erbguth K. (2006). Light-induced activation of distinct modulatory neurons triggers appetitive or aversive learning in Drosophila larvae. Curr. Biol. 16 17411747. 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.023 16950113 Scott K. Brady R. Jr. Cravchik A. Morozov P. Rzhetsky A. Zuker C. (2001). A chemosensory gene family encoding candidate gustatory and olfactory receptors in Drosophila. Cell 104 661673. 10.1016/S0092-8674(01)00263-X 11257221 Selcho M. Millan C. Palacios-Munoz A. Ruf F. Ubillo L. Chen J. (2017). Central and peripheral clocks are coupled by a neuropeptide pathway in Drosophila. Nat. Commun. 8:15563. 10.1038/ncomms15563 28555616 Selcho M. Pauls D. Han K. A. Stocker R. F. Thum A. S. (2009). The role of dopamine in Drosophila larval classical olfactory conditioning. PLoS One 4:e5897. 10.1371/journal.pone.0005897 19521527 Selcho M. Pauls D. Huser A. Stocker R. F. Thum A. S. (2014). Characterization of the octopaminergic and tyraminergic neurons in the central brain of Drosophila larvae. J. Comp. Neurol. 522 34853500. 10.1002/cne.23616 24752702 Singh R. N. Singh K. (1984). Fine structure of the sensory organs of Drosophila melanogaster Meigen larva (Diptera: Drosophilidae). Int. J. Insect Morphol. Embryol. 13 255273. 10.1016/0020-7322(84)90001-1 Tully T. Cambiazo V. Kruse L. (1994). Memory through metamorphosis in normal and mutant Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 14 6874. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-01-00068.1994 8283252 von Essen A. M. Pauls D. Thum A. S. Sprecher S. G. (2011). Capacity of visual classical conditioning in Drosophila larvae. Behav. Neurosci. 125 921929. 10.1037/a0025758 21967373 Waddell S. (2013). Reinforcement signalling in Drosophila; dopamine does it all after all. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23 324329. 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.005 23391527 Waddell S. (2016). Neural plasticity: dopamine tunes the mushroom body output network. Curr. Biol. 26 R109R112. 10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.023 26859265 Widmann A. Artinger M. Biesinger L. Boepple K. Peters C. Schlechter J. (2016). Genetic dissection of aversive associative olfactory learning and memory in Drosophila larvae. PLoS Genet. 12:e1006378. 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006378 27768692 Widmann A. Eichler K. Selcho M. Thum A. S. Pauls D. (2017). Odor-taste learning in Drosophila larvae. J. Insect Physiol. 106(Pt 1), 4754. 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2017.08.004 28823531
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016hknbsoft.com.cn
      www.gwumsr.com.cn
      kpchain.com.cn
      hi04.org.cn
      ssdnkb.com.cn
      www.okpktg.com.cn
      mymzmj.org.cn
      www.plchain.com.cn
      mohsgn.com.cn
      stackcube.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p