Front. Psychol. Frontiers in Psychology Front. Psychol. 1664-1078 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02049 Psychology Original Research Infant Massage and Quality of Early Mother–Infant Interactions: Are There Associations with Maternal Psychological Wellbeing, Marital Quality, and Social Support? Porreca Alessio * Parolin Micol Bozza Giusy Freato Susanna Simonelli Alessandra Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, University of Padua Padua, Italy

Edited by: Francesco Pagnini, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Italy

Reviewed by: Colin M. Bosma, Harvard University, USA; Gabriela Markova, University of Vienna, Austria; Eleonora Volpato, Fondazione Don Gnocchi, Italy

*Correspondence: Alessio Porreca, alessio.porreca@libero.it

This article was submitted to Psychology for Clinical Settings, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology

17 01 2017 2016 7 2049 26 09 2015 19 12 2016 Copyright © 2017 Porreca, Parolin, Bozza, Freato and Simonelli. 2017 Porreca, Parolin, Bozza, Freato and Simonelli

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Infant massage programs have proved to be effective in enhancing post-natal development of highly risk infants, such as preterm newborns and drug or HIV exposed children. Less studies have focused on the role of infant massage in supporting the co-construction of early adult–child relationships. In line with this lack of literature, the present paper reports on a pilot study aimed at investigating longitudinally the quality of mother–child interactions, with specific reference to emotional availability (EA), in a group of mother–child pairs involved in infant massage classes. Moreover, associations between mother–child EA, maternal wellbeing, marital adjustment, and social support were also investigated, with the hypothesis to find a link between low maternal distress, high couple satisfaction and high perceived support and interactions of better quality in the dyads. The study involved 20 mothers and their children, aged between 2 and 7 months, who participated to infant massage classes. The assessment took place at three stages: at the beginning of massage course, at the end of it and at 1-month follow-up. At the first stage of assessment self-report questionnaires were administered to examine the presence of maternal psychiatric symptoms (SCL-90-R), perceived social support (MSPSS), and marital adjustment (Dyadic Adjustment Scale); dyadic interactions were observed and rated with the Emotional Availability Scales (Biringen, 2008) at each stage of data collection. The results showed a significant improvement in the quality of mother–child interactions, between the first and the last evaluation, parallel to the unfolding of the massage program, highlighting a general increase in maternal and child’s EA. The presence of maternal psychological distress resulted associated with less optimal mother–child emotional exchanges, while the hypothesis regarding couple satisfaction and social support influence were not confirmed. These preliminary results, if replicated, seem to sustain the usefulness of infant massage and the importance of focusing on early mother–infant interactions.

infant massage mother–child interactions child development parenting early relationships

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Theoretical Background Infant Massage

      The practice of infant massage represents a simple but effective way to enhance and strengthen healthy social and emotional relationships between adults and children in early infancy; this is true both from a relational and a practical point of view. On the one hand, in fact, massaging a baby requires and intensifies a series of multimodal and interactive competencies (such as emotional expression, eye-contact, physical touch, vocal communication, and turn-taking) that usually characterize adult–child daily repertoires; in this sense, it represents a privileged moment to cultivate and strengthen the relationship. On the other hand, instead, it is cost-saving and no contraindications were reported up to now; thus, it can be used frequently and without risk, accompanying and supporting a process existing per se, which is the one of adult–infant bonding.

      Together with the practices of breastfeeding, baby carrying, and co-sleeping, infant massage is part of a wider “caretaking-package” which involves a set of behaviors necessary to satisfy the child’s needs for contact, holding, communication, and feeding; these needs are simple and primitive but often unrecognized (Balsamo, 2007). A particular feature of these behaviors is that they specifically require tactile interactions with the infant and that they are able to convey a series of somatosensory messages about feelings, pressure, temperature, softness, or pain (Underdown, 2009). As already shown by the research on sensorial deprivation in early infancy, these experiences are fundamental for physical growth, neurological development and for the construction of healthy affective relationships (Robertson and Bowlby, 1952; Harlow and Zimmermann, 1958; Montagu, 1971; Blackwell, 2000; Kim et al., 2003).

      From a neural-developmental point of view, touch is one of the first sensorial systems to be activated in the fetus during pregnancy, becoming, thus, one of the primary means of communication with the surrounding environment (Relier, 1994; Hernandez-Reif et al., 2007). After delivery, touch becomes an important channel of communication during mother–infant interactions (De Chateau, 1976; Tronick, 1995; Stack, 2004; Moszkowski and Stack, 2007; Jean and Stack, 2009; Stack et al., unpublished). It occurs more than the 55% of time and communicates security and tenderness, reducing infants’ distress and promoting emotional regulation (Stack and Muir, 1990, 1992; Weiss et al., 2000; Jean et al., 2004; Jean and Stack, 2009). Empirical studies have widely stressed that sensitive caregiving allows the baby not to feel overwhelmed, moderating or accelerating his/her emotions and intervening at a neuro-physiological level on the reactivity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) system (Tronick, 1989; Nelson and Bosquet, 2000; Beebe and Lachmann, 2002). In this sense, early experiences of touch represent one of the means through which the adult works as an external regulator for the baby, helping the co-regulation of his/her behavioral and emotional states and consequently preventing him/her from experiencing developmental difficulties, such as sleep and feeding problems (De Chateau, 1976; Koniak-Griffin and Ludington-Hoe, 1988; Brazelton, 1990; Underdown, 2009).

      Infant touching and infant massage are also part of a “high-” or “proximal-contact” model of caretaking (Stork, 1986; Balsamo, 2002), that promotes intense physical and psychological contact between mother and child, with the aim to hold the child and to protect him/her from dangers. From this point of view, and given the relational and developmental influences of early experiences of touch, infant massage appears a useful technique to support parenting by promoting sensitivity and enhancing the construction of healthy affective bonds between adults and children (Bozza, unpublished).

      Research on infant massage as intervention has been applied especially to preterm infants. In this particular population the exposure to infant massage sessions resulted in an improvement in several health indexes, such as weight-gain, increases in length, head circumference, bone density, and body temperature (Scafidi et al., 1986; Scafidi et al., 1990; Kuhn et al., 1991; Wheeden et al., 1993; Moyer Mileur et al., 1995; Jinon, 1996; Dieter et al., 2001, 2003; Ke et al., 2001; Duan et al., 2002; Ferber et al., 2002; Liu, 2005; Lu et al., 2005; Na et al., 2005; Diego et al., 2008).

      Moreover, massage therapy resulted also capable of modifying the distribution of sleep/awake states, favoring longer periods of active alertness and reducing excitability (Scafidi et al., 1986, 1990; Field et al., 1987, 2004; Wheeden et al., 1993; Dieter et al., 2003).

      As far as it concerns other groups at high-risk, infant massage sessions have worked successfully on HIV- and drug-exposed newborns, leading to fewer medical complications, less irritability and increased weight, and to an improvement in the performance on social and emotional scales (Scafidi et al., 1990; Wheeden et al., 1993; Scafidi and Field, 1996; Dieter et al., 2001).

      Although most non-at-risk infants will receive adequate sensitive handling, the administration of infant massage with regular frequency can represent an useful way to support parenting and to promote caregivers’ sensitive touch in early infancy (Underdown, 2009). Some authors emphasized the bidirectional effects of giving and receiving massage, supporting the use of the technique as a safe and cost-effective intervention for adult–child relationships (Feijò et al., 2006). Firstly, in fact, maternal resources can be enhanced by massage lessons through the promotion of a better knowledge of the infant’s needs and characteristics; secondly, the child’s features can be better modulated thanks to a more satisfying contact with the caregiver (Oswalt et al., 2009).

      Massages can be given to children on a daily basis and they are economically saving when parents are enrolled as therapists (Field, 2002). The massage of infants promotes a general sense of wellbeing in the adult, helping the parent to feel close to the baby, and less fearful of touching and handling him/her. Moreover mothers who massaged their infants, both preterm and full term, reported less anxiety, less depressed mood, and improved mother–child interactions (Onozawa et al., 2001; Ferber et al., 2005; Feijò et al., 2006). Teaching parents to massage their infants, especially in risk conditions, often lowers anxiety levels related to the feelings of helplessness (Field, 2002). As far as it concerns the child, infant massage reduces and balances cortisol, epinephrine, and norepinephrine hormones which control stress levels (Acolet et al., 1993; Field et al., 1996). As a consequence, this practice could be considered as a facilitator of the normal development of these catecholamines that characterize early stages of life (Kuhn et al., 1991).

      Promoting infant massage lessons may represent an ideal way to support parenting and to support early emotional and social relationships between adults and children, useful both for high- and low-risk groups (Underdown, 2009). This technique offers a perspective which is simple but very useful, since it combines a behavioral intervention (i.e., the physical manipulation of the baby) with a profoundly emotional dimension (i.e., the close psychological and emotional proximity that massage enhances). Moreover, participating to massage classes constitutes an opportunity to meet other adults that are living the same experience, to share and compare different points of view, to create new bonds (Adamson, 1996).

      Parenting and Early Interactions from a Multi-Determined Point of View

      Parenting encompasses a broad range of nurturing and care-taking actions performed by caregivers toward the child. The actual behaviors that parents provide are among the most salient aspects of parenting, since the most of the experience of infants stems directly from interactions with caregivers occurring daily (Bornstein, 2002). Recently, increasing attention has been given to the emotional features that accompany parenting routines; more specifically, an important contribution to the investigation of parenting has been given by the theoretical frame of Emotional Availability (EA), which has pointed out the importance to create an emotional connection and to be able to share a wide range of affects during caretaking behaviors (Biringen, 2008; Biringen and Easterbrooks, 2012). As pointed out above, infant massage can be considered as a component of care-taking behaviors, that offers a valuable interactive context to parents, who can involve in pleasant and rewarding exchanges with their babies. Thanks to Belsky’s work on parenting (1984), nowadays it is well acknowledged that parenting, in all its components, is a multi-determined construct, encompassing both contextual variables and baby’s and parents’ individual characteristics. Empirical evidence attests that some of these factors are of particular relevance, such as the quality of marital relationship, maternal psychological wellbeing, and perceived social support.

      The transition to parenthood is an event of the family life cycle that asks the couple to face potentially stressful changes and challenges (Belsky and Rovine, 1990; Cowan and Cowan, 2000) both at an inner and behavioral level (Cowan and Hetherington, 2001), in order to meet child’s need and develop parenting competences. In these terms, it is a critical period for marital satisfaction, which goes through a small but reliable decline, persisting at least until the preschool age as reported by several studies on dyadic satisfaction (Belsky et al., 1983; Terry et al., 1991; Favez et al., 2012; Kohn et al., 2012; Trillingsgaard et al., 2014). Women seem to experience a steeper and more precipitous drop in marital quality (Shapiro et al., 2000; Twenge et al., 2003; Bower et al., 2013), especially when they are scarcely satisfied with the division of labor and they manage a greater amount of childcare activities (Belsky, 1986; Gjerdingen and Center, 2005; Dew and Wilcox, 2011; Chong and Mickelson, 2013). However, other studies indicate that mothers and fathers share similar perception of post-birth relationship functioning (Lawrence et al., 2008; Doss et al., 2009; Jackson et al., 2014).

      The transition toward parenthood is a very critical and stressful stage that may lead to serious psychological distress symptoms in pregnant women and women that recently gave birth to their offsprings, ranging from 4.8% (Glasheen et al., 2015) to 19% (Czarnocka and Slade, 2000; Saurel-Cubizolles et al., 2007; Gavin et al., 2011). Maternal psychological distress appears to be enduring (Horwitz et al., 2007) and, given that the early postpartum months are especially important for the establishment of a satisfactory dyadic relationship and for infant development (Hay and Kumar, 1995; Murray et al., 2015), it may negatively affect child outcomes (Goodman et al., 2011), mother–infant interactions (Singer et al., 2003), conjugal and family relationships (Whisman, 2001; Sutter-Dallay, 2006). Some studies highlight that marital conflict shows the most significant association with maternal distress in women who have recently given birth (Stemp et al., 1986).

      Depression represents one of the most frequent distress conditions in the context of the transition to motherhood; review and meta-analytic studies have demonstrated that depression is linked to a range of adverse behavioral and emotional outcomes for the child, in terms of psychopathology and negative affects and behaviors, especially for younger children (Goodman, 2007; Goodman et al., 2011). Maternal depression is linked with impaired mother–infant interactive patterns since early age (i.e., 4 months) in both partners, especially concerning self-and interactive contingency (Beebe et al., 2012; Murray et al., 2015); depressed mothers are described by Weinberg and Tronick (1997) as intrusive or withdrawn. Notably, improving maternal depression does not imply per se an enhancement of the quality of mother–infant interaction, indicating the importance of targeting not merely mother’s depression and adopting instead a broader approach (Forman et al., 2007). A pilot study (Onozawa et al., 2001) reports that depressed mothers beneficiate from attending a massage class, attenuating depression symptoms and learning to interact better with their babies, leading in turn to an improvement in dyadic interaction (maternal attitudes and behaviors toward the child and infants’ responses), more than mothers in a support group did. Another study (O’Higgins et al., 2008) attests a clinically significant reduction in depression for the majority of mothers, even though scores remained high.

      In a wider perspective, parenting and child development inextricably take place in the context of social relationships. Social support can be defined as the amount of advice received and personal needs fulfilled through the presence and interaction with significant others, within or outside the family, such as partners, relatives, or friends (Kaplan et al., 1977). Social support has been widely linked to individual wellbeing and positive mental health at an individual level (Mitchell and Trickett, 1980); in particular, women are more likely to give social support, draw on socially supportive networks in times of stress and, more importantly, benefit from it (Taylor, 2011).

      Moreover, according to Belsky’s (1984) model, social support is a key determinant of parenting quality; it plays a role (as mediator, moderator or as a direct influence), in influencing parent–child interactions (Jennings et al., 1991), attitudes about parenting, parenting behaviors and, in turn, child outcomes (Feiring et al., 1987; Melson et al., 1993; Bender and Losel, 1998). Pregnant women that can rely on others for support show better outcomes for stress, depression and anxiety (Glazier et al., 2004). As regards mothers, the advantages of social support, especially if it is provided from one’s own mother, can be detected in the domains mental health, reducing the risk of postpartum depression, and of marital satisfaction (Findler et al., 2008); in fact, social support is important for the couple too, being associated to partners’ cohesion and intimacy (Cutrona, 1996). Nevertheless, support to mothers peaks in the first weeks after childbirth, but it appears to decrease in the following months (Pinelli, 2000; Rowe and Jones, 2010). If social support is typically considered as a positive influence on parenting, it is also important to note that it can exert an adverse influence, in terms of stressful or non-supportive relationships that lead to decreased wellbeing (Ingersoll-Dayton et al., 1997).

      Interestingly, Herwig et al. (2004) have simultaneously considered maternal parenting and depression, couple satisfaction and social support in their associations with child outcomes, reporting the predictive role of maternal parenting and couple satisfaction and the indirect influence of depression and social support on child development.

      Aims and Hypothesis

      To our knowledge, to date no studies have examined the quality of mother–infant interactions in the context of infant massage through the application of the Emotional Availability Scales (EAS; Biringen, 2008), nor have adopted a longitudinal approach. The present preliminary research aimed to investigate mother–child EA during infant massage classes. EA refers to the capacity of a dyad to share an emotionally healthy relationship and has been widely used in research to assess the quality of parenting and of adult–child relationships. Moreover, we adopted a longitudinal perspective, in order to verify whether EA changed or remained stable during the unfolding of infant massage lessons. Finally, according to the extant literature that highlights the role of multiple factors in shaping the quality of early parenting practices, we investigated whether EA was associated to mothers’ perception of couple adjustment, social support and psychological wellbeing. An increase in dyadic EA was expected and we hypothesized that more optimal adult–child interactions would be associated with a lower degree of maternal psychological distress, and with a higher level of couple satisfaction and perceived social support.

      Materials and Methods

      The present study adopted a descriptive and correlational design for providing preliminary data on the longitudinal investigation of mother–infant EA during massage classes.

      Participants<sup><xref ref-type="fn" rid="fn01">1</xref></sup>

      The sample of the study was composed of 20 mother–child pairs selected from a larger group of participants enrolled in an infant massage course. The children’s (10 boys and 10 girls) age ranged from 2 to 7 months (M = 2.6, SD = 1.392). Sixty-five percent of the subjects could be assessed longitudinally. The remaining subjects (35%) were not tested longitudinally due to obstacles preventing mothers from attending the scheduled lessons. The mothers were all Caucasian; their age ranged from 27 to 38 years old (M = 31.75, SD = 2.751). Inclusion criteria for the study included: being primiparous, being subject to the first experience of infant massage, having attended all massage classes with the same conductor, absence of infant pathology.

      Socio-demographic information regarding different domains such as level of education and occupation was collected through a self-report questionnaire. Twenty percent of the mothers reported to be an only child, while the others reported one (40%) or more (40%) siblings. Concerning education, the 10% of the group declared to have a middle school certificate, while the others reported to have an high school diploma (35%) or an academic degree (50%). As far as it regards work, 15% of the mothers reported to be unemployed, while the remaining declared to have a job in the working class (10%), as employees (45%) or in other forms (30%).

      Procedure and Instruments

      Massage courses were presented during childbirth classes in different Venetian sanitary districts. Once enrolled the women were contacted after delivery and invited to participate to the course. Participation was free. The dyads were divided randomly in groups of 5, each of these represented a massage class. At the beginning of the course each mother was given a battery of self-report questionnaires to fill in at home, aimed at investigating socio-demographic information, maternal psychological well being, marital relationships and perceived social support (see the section Quality of Mother–Child Interactions during Massage Lessons). The participants were told that they would have been videotaped three times during the cycle of massage classes (during T1, T3, and T4). Informed consent was asked to both parents before videotaping the baby. Figure 1 resumes the research design, and the variables assessed during the different times of measurement.

      Times of measurement, intervention, and variables assessed.

      The Infant Massage Program

      The dyads were offered a cycle of four weekly lessons on infant massage. Each encounter lasted about an hour and a half. Massage courses were provided by a trained psychotherapist. During the course each room was warm and quiet in its atmosphere. Cameras were located in different angles of the room at a distance that allowed to capture the most salient aspects of each dyad’s interaction without interfering with the appropriate atmosphere.

      Every mother was told to bring a cushion big enough to contain the baby and some natural oil. Activities took place on the floor, sitting in circle on wickers, in front of their babies, holding cushions firm with their legs. This condition allowed the mothers to hold their babies and to enhance visual contact with them. The conductor sit in the center of the circle in order to be visible for all the participants, and reproduced on a doll the various techniques taught during each encounter. In this way, the conductor did not touch the babies but only showed the participants how to massage.

      The first lesson (T1) concerned an initial introduction to the massage course and to the research project. After a brief presentation of the equipe, the mothers were given notions in order to experience in the best way possible the encounters: they were invited to pay attention and to follow their children’s needs, to wait for optimal moments before beginning massage sessions, to feed their babies or to let them sleep whether necessary, to calm them down as they were used to. After a brief moment of relaxation, the mothers were invited to undress their infants from waist down, in order to be able to massage their legs and their abdomen. Instructions focused on how to handle the baby and how to touch, on how to pay attention and to become aware of the baby’s signals. The mothers were lead through a group discussion on massage benefits and they were invited to repeat the procedure at home. The first encounter was video-recorded.

      During the second lesson (T2) the conductor introduced another sequence of massage procedures, focusing on face, superior arts, and chest. Alternative positions to use massage were presented and parents were positively reinforced during their efforts. Moreover, the mothers were given the possibility to share their feelings about maternity with the group. The conductor stressed the fact that the massage represents a technique with a particular focus on mother–child relationship, a relationship that can be improved dedicating more time to practice at home and, in turn, to the relationship.

      The third lesson (T3) concerned the repetition of the entire sequence applied to the frontal body, also adding some suggestions on how to approach the back during massage. Again, some space during this encounter was left to allow the mothers to talk about their experience of the postpartum period. A bibliography was suggested to acquire more knowledge regarding the themes emerged during the cycle of lessons. This encounter was video-recorded.

      After a month the group met again for a so-called moment of reinforce and of review of the techniques previously learned. This session was videotaped (T4) and considered as a follow-up.

      Quality of Mother–Child Interactions during Massage Lessons

      The first (T1), the third (T3), and the fourth (T4) lessons were videotaped. For every dyad 20 min of mother–child interaction were recorded during each episode. The interactions were coded using the fourth version of the EAS (Biringen, 2008). The coding system is composed of six scales/dimensions, four for the adult (sensitivity, structuring, non-intrusiveness, and non-hostility) and two aimed at evaluating the child’s contribution (responsiveness, involvement of the adult).

      Adult sensitivity refers to quality of adult affects, clarity of perceptions and appropriate responsiveness, awareness of timing, flexibility, variety and creativity during play, acceptance of the child, amount of interactions, and adequate resolution of conflicts.

      Adult structuring concerns the use of proactive guidance, the success of attempts, the amount of guidance, the ability to set limits and to remain firm in the face of pressure, the use of both verbal and non-verbal suggestions and the ability to assume an adult role rather than a peer one.

      Adult non-intrusiveness refers to the ability to follow the child’s lead, to the use of non-interruptive ports of entry into interaction, to the modest use of commands and directives, to the appropriateness of teaching and adult talking, to the absence of interferences and of child’s signals that indicate that the adult is perceived as intrusive.

      Adult non-hostility refers to the lack of negativity in face or voice and to the lack of ridiculing or other disrespectful behaviors toward the child. A non-hostile adult does not threat to separate, is not frightening, maintains cool during challenging situations and does not use threats of hostile play themes during interactions.

      Child responsiveness takes into account quality of child’s affects and organization of behaviors, the ability and the willingness to respond to the adult’s bids without anxiety or role reversal. It also considers positive physical positioning, concentration on task and the presence of avoidance or of over responsiveness and role reversal.

      Child involvement of the adult concerns the use of simple and elaborative initiative to involve the adult, the affective use of the adult (rather than instrumental), the lack of negative/over involving behaviors, and the use of verbal and non-verbal channels.

      Each EA dimension is given a global score on a 7-point scale, with higher ratings referring to more optimal features. Scores between 5 and 7 are considered adequate and index of a healthy relationship. Scores around 4 indicate inconsistency, (i.e., behaviors that are appropriate in some way but that are not fully healthy). Scores of 3 or below indicate less optimal interactions were problematic behaviors might arise (scores of 1 or 2). The coding refers to the global quality of the interaction observed rather than on specific behaviors. To get a more specific profile of the adult–child relationship, the EA assessment system provides the coders also a Clinical-Screener that allows to attribute each member of the dyad to one of four “zones” (according to the scores given to maternal sensitivity and child responsiveness), which represent four possible categories of EA: the Emotionally Available zone, the Complicated zone, the Detached zone and the Problematic zone. Mother–child interactions were coded by two independent raters previously trained on the EA coding system in order to reach satisfactory reliability with the Biringen’s lab. Inter-rater reliability was calculated on the 20% of the videos using ICCs which ranged from 0.80 to 0.95.

      Self-report Measures

      At the beginning of the massage course the mothers were given a battery of self-report questionnaires to fill in at home before the second lesson. The instruments aimed to assess socio-demographic information, maternal psychological wellbeing, marital relationships and perceived social support.

      The Symptom Checklist-90 Revised (SCL-90-R; Derogatis, 1977; Italian version by Sarno et al., 2011) is a brief questionnaire designed to evaluate the presence of psychological distress and a range of psychopathological symptoms. It consists of 90 items and yields nine scores along primary symptom dimensions and three scores that refer to global distress indexes. The primary assessed symptom dimensions are somatization, obsessive-compulsive, interpersonal sensitivity, depression, anxiety, hostility, phobic anxiety, paranoid ideation, and psychoticism. The three global indexes refer to global psychological distress status (Global Severity Index – GSI), to the total number of symptoms reported (Positive Symptom Total – PST) and to the intensity of reported distress (Positive Symptom Distress Index – PSDI). According to T-scores, each item can be interpreted as below average scores, within the median range, above average scores and definitely above the average scores of the normative sample (thus indicating severe symptomatology).

      The Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS; Spanier, 1976; Italian version by Gentili et al., 2002) is a multidimensional tool that allows to assess conjugal adjustment. The sum of the 32 items lead to four dimension of conjugal adjustment (Dyadic Consensus, Dyadic Satisfaction, Dyadic Cohesion, and Affective Expression) and to a global score (Total Adjustment Score) that represents the degree of conjugal adjustment perceived by the partners.

      The Multidimensional Scale on Perceived Social Support (MSPSS; Zimet et al., 1988; Italian version by Prezza and Principato, 2002) is a self-report questionnaire about the perceived adequacy of support given by the partner, family and friends. The 12 items are given a score on a 7-point Likert Scale and their sum leads to three dimensions concerning support received by the family, by friends and by a significant other.

      Results

      Data were analyzed using IBM SPSS statistics vers. 23. Firstly, descriptive statistics (frequencies, mean scores, and percentages) were examined. Secondly, non-parametric tests were applied; more specifically, we adopted the Friedman and Wilcoxon signed-rank and the Spearman’s Rho to test for any associations between the different instruments adopted in the research design.

      Preliminary Analysis

      During preliminary analysis, Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was used to assess the reliability of the instruments. Descriptive statistics (average scores, frequencies, and percentages) were examined.

      Mother–Child Interactions during Massage Lessons

      The application of Cronbach’s alpha coefficient, to all the three periods considered, reported good reliability for EA maternal scales (0.85 ≤ α ≤ 0.89), for the EA child’s scales (0.73 ≤ α ≤ 0.80) and for all the six scales considered globally (0.87 ≤ α ≤ 0.90). Tables 1 and 2 report average scores, standard deviations and the distribution of the dyads assessed through the EAS and the EA clinical screener. As it is possible to observe from Table 1, regarding the EA Scales, during T1 the dyads reported on average score 4 (indicating inconsistency) in two maternal dimensions and in both child dimensions, while scores on the other dimensions resulted adequate (≥5). During T3 and T4 average scores resulted adequate (≥5) for all of the six dimensions. As far as it concerns the distribution of the dyads in the zones of the EA clinical screener, as it is possible to see in Table 2 during the periods considered a progressively larger amount of dyads fell in the Emotionally Available (EA) zone, while the “lower” zones were progressively less represented.

      Average scores and standard deviations of the Emotional Availability Scales (EAS) applied during T1, T3, and T4.

      M (SD)
      Variables T1 (N = 20) T3 (N = 19) T4 (N = 13)
      Sensitivity 5.15 (1.07) 6.03 (1.06) 6.85 (0.38)
      Structuring 4.93 (0.98) 5.76 (1.02) 6.73 (0.44)
      Non-intrusiveness 4.73 (0.82) 5.79 (0.75) 6.54 (0.63)
      Non-hostility 6.05 (0.94) 6.53 (0.79) 7.00 (0.00)
      Ch. Responsiveness 4.68 (1.05) 5.58 (0.95) 6.50 (0.50)
      Ch. Involvement 4.15 (1.09) 5.05 (1.04) 6.00 (0.79)

      Distribution of the dyads on the zones of the emotional availability (EA) clinical screener during T1, T3, and T4.

      T1 (N = 20)
      T3 (N = 19)
      T4 (N = 13)
      Mother Child Mother Child Mother Child
      EA Zone 7 (35%) 5 (25%) 15 (78.95%) 14 (73.68%) 13 (100%) 13 (100%)
      Complicated Zone 11 (55%) 10 (50%) 4 (21.05%) 5 (26.32%)
      Detached Zone 2 (10%) 4 (20%)
      Problematic Zone 1 (5%)
      Psychological Distress

      As far as it concerns psychological distress, Cronbach’s alpha was applied to the SCL-90-R symptom dimensions and to the global distress indexes. Good internal consistency was reported for obsessive-compulsive (0.82), interpersonal sensitivity (0.72), social phobia (0.72), paranoid ideation (0.78), psychoticism (0.80), and for the total of the items (0.90). Reliability was acceptable for somatization (0.67) and depression (0.70), poor for anxiety (0.56) and unacceptable for hostility (≤0.50), which was excluded from subsequent analysis. Table 3 reports average scores, standard deviations and the distribution among normative cut-offs for the SCL-90-R scores. As it is possible to see, the majority of the subjects fell within the SCL-90-R normative cut-off values while a smaller percentage of mothers reported values above the norm, suggesting the presence of significant psychological distress.

      Average scores, standard deviations, and distribution of the mothers in the SCL-90-R.

      N = 20 Norm Clinical
      M (SD) N (%) N (%)
      Somatization 746.40 (5.49) 18 (90) 2 (10)
      Obsessive-compulsive 49.40 (9.52) 15 (75) 5 (25)
      Interpersonal-Sensitivity 47.65 (6.95) 15 (75) 5 (25)
      Depression 46.80 (5.19) 18 (90) 2 (10)
      Anxiety 45.85 (4.83) 18 (90) 2 (10)
      Phobic 45.95 (3.94) 19 (95) 1 (5)
      Paranoid Ideation 46.50 (8.15) 17 (85) 3 (15)
      Psychoticism 47.85 (7.57) 17 (85) 3 (15)
      GSI 46.30 (5.60) 18 (90) 2 (10)
      PST 45.48 (6.57) 19 (95) 1 (5)
      PSDI 48.90 (6.69) 15 (75) 5 (25)
      Couple Adjustment

      Regarding couple adjustment, Cronbach’s alpha coefficient reported good internal consistency for the DAS total score (0.77) and for the subscales concerning Dyadic Consensus (0.69) and Dyadic Satisfaction (0.68). Reliability was unacceptable as far as it concerns Dyadic Cohesion and Affective Expression (α ≤ 0.50). Thus, these subscales were excluded from subsequent analysis. Normative cut-offs were computed from average scores and standard deviations reported in the article of the Italian validation of the DAS (Gentili et al., 2002). All the mothers in the present study reported scores that fell in the normative range (Table 4).

      Average scores, standard deviations, and distribution of the mothers’ scores in the DAS.

      N = 18 <Norm ≥Norm
      M (SD) N (%) N (%)
      Dyadic Consensus (DC) 54.39 (4.79) 18 (100)
      Dyadic Satisfaction (DS) 42.44 (4.33) 18 (100)
      Dyadic Adjustment_TOT 123.72 (9.47) 18 (100)
      Perceived Social Support

      The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient reported very good reliability for all the scales of the MSPSS, considering both the different subscales and the whole scale (0.76 ≤ α ≤ 0.97). Table 5 reports average scores and standard deviations for the MSPSS. Normative cut-offs were extracted from the validation study by Prezza and Principato (2002). As it is possible to see, the majority of the subjects reported the perception of satisfactory support provided by family, friends and a significant other.

      Average scores, standard deviations, and distribution of the mothers in the MSPSS.

      N = 20 ≥Norm <Norm
      M (SD) N (%) N (%)
      Support by a significant other 5.43 (0.65) 19 (95) 1 (5)
      Support by family 5.16 (1.18) 16 (80) 4 (20)
      Support by friends 4.46 (1.06) 17 (85) 3 (15)
      Perceived social support_ TOT 5.03 (0.81) 18 (90) 2 (10)

      As it is possible to see from the data above, from a descriptive point of view at the beginning of the massage course all the mothers seemed to be able to rely on satisfactory couple adjustment, showing scores within the normative range in all the DAS scales. A similar consideration could be made for social support; although few subjects reported MSPSS scores below average, the majority of the participants seemed to experience a sufficient amount of support provided by family, friends and a significant other. As far as it concerns psychological wellbeing, although the majority of the subjects reported scores that fell into the SCL-90-R normative range, some mothers reported the presence of significant psychological distress as well. It is noteworthy, however, that rarely this perception reached the clinical cut-off. Finally, considering mother–child interactions, it is possible to see how, during T1, most of the mother–child dyads fell in the complicated zone of EA, indicating the presence of an emotional connection but the existence of difficulties as well. The complicated zone, and the zones below were progressively less represented during the ongoing of massage classes. During the follow up (T4), all the dyads that completed the program fell in the emotional available zone.

      Change vs. Stability in Quality of Mother–Child Interactions

      To assess change vs. stability of EA during the infant-massage course the Friedman test was applied. This non-parametric statistical test can be considered a valid alternative of the parametric repeated measures ANOVA. The results reported a statistically significant increase in maternal sensitivity (X2 = 18.650, p = 0.001), structuring (X2= 17.190, p = 0.001), non-intrusiveness (X2 = 15.864, p = 0.001), and non-hostility (X2 = 7.400, p = 0.025), just as in child responsiveness (X2 = 15.650, p = 0.001) and involvement (X2 = 15.476, p = 0.001), indicating thus an improvement in all the EA dimensions during the massage course. To investigate the specific patterns of change in the scales, the Wilcoxon signed-rank test was applied to compare the different periods of infant massage course. This non-parametric statistical hypothesis test can be considered as an alternative to the paired Student’s t-test and can be used to compare repeated measures on a single sample to assess differences in the population mean ranks. The results reported a statistically significant increase in maternal sensitivity (Z = -2.553, p = 0.011), structuring (Z = -2.335, p = 0.020), non-intrusiveness (Z = -3.357, p = 0.001), just as in child responsiveness (Z = -2.626, p = 0.009), and involvement (Z = -2.120, p = 0.034) from T1 to T3. Regarding the transition from T3 to T4, statistically significant improvements were highlighted for maternal sensitivity (Z = -2.070), structuring (Z = -2.384, p = 0.017), non-intrusiveness (Z = -2.059, p = 0.040) and in child responsiveness (Z = -2.121, p = 0.034).

      Associations between Emotional Availability and Psychological Distress

      To test for associations between quality of mother–child interactions and maternal psychological distress, the Spearman’s Rho coefficient was applied to the SCL-90-R reliable scores (obsessive-compulsive, interpersonal sensitivity, social phobia, paranoid ideation, psychoticism, somatization, depression, anxiety, GSI, PST, and PSDI) and to the scores obtained through the EAS during T1, T3, and T4. Table 6 reports associations between EA and psychological distress. As it is possible to observe, regarding T1, correlations were found between maternal non-intrusiveness and psychoticicsm. During T3, negative correlations were found between psychoticism, maternal structuring, non-intrusiveness, non-hostility, and for child responsiveness. Moreover, anxiety resulted negatively associated with maternal non-intrusiveness and child responsiveness. As far it concerns T4, statistically significant inverse associations were found between maternal sensitivity and obsessive-compulsive, the GSI, and the PSDI.

      Associations between EA and psychological distress.

      Som OC I-S Dep Anx Phob Par Psy GSI PST PSDI
      Sens T1 -0.113 0.168 0.014 -0.198 -0.367 -0.197 -0.129 -0.134 -0.037 -0.029 -0.111
      T3 -0.123 0.227 -0.313 0.102 -0.201 -0.378 -0.108 -0.452 0.026 -0.118 0.127
      T4 -0.342 -0.628ˆ* -0.401 -0.516 -0.468 0.139 -0.314 -0.301 -0.627ˆ* -0.514 -0.628ˆ*
      Struct T1 0.058 -0.038 0.051 -0.029 -0.320 -0.098 0.020 -0.311 -0.061 -0.071 -0.050
      T3 -0.048 0.173 -0.369 -0.053 -0.323 -0.101 -0.111 -0.621ˆ** -0.091 -0.233 0.079
      T4 0.007 -0.247 0.116 -0.211 -0.264 0.215 0.034 -0.029 -0.210 -0.034 -0.420
      Nonint T1 0.108 0.157 0.073 0.123 -0.249 -0.020 -0.038 -0.451ˆ* 0.102 -0.030 0.179
      T3 -0.222 0.053 -0.371 -0.240 -0.506ˆ* 0.088 -0.237 -0.591ˆ** -0.252 -0.358 -0.084
      T4 0.027 -0.435 -0.234 -0.283 -0.130 0.463 -0.314 -0.175 -0.348 -0.247 -0.418
      Nonhos T1 0.060 0.035 0.177 -0.200 -0.201 -0.329 0.150 -0.189 -0.031 0.035 -0.129
      T3 -0.253 0.079 -0.413 -0.086 -0.337 -0.088 -0.280 -0.582ˆ** -0.220 -0.392 0.047
      T4
      Ch. Resp T1 0.023 0.195 0.330 0.282 0.207 -0.061 0.237 0.050 0.248 0.302 0.086
      T3 -0.260 0.088 -0.352 -0.088 -0.495ˆ* -0.281 -0.201 -0.599ˆ** -0.152 -0.282 0.020
      T4 0.386 -0.407 -0.237 -0.215 -0.022 0.418 -0.451 -0.350 -0.257 -0.236 -0.257
      Ch. Involv T1 0.226 -0.051 0.145 0.139 -0.018 0.071 0.088 0.053 0.081 0.039 0.112
      T3 -0.127 0.242 0.030 0.089 -0.298 -0.510 0.047 -0.367 0.051 -0.015 0.016
      T4 0.222 -0.168 -0.046 0.029 -0.184 0.330 -0.334 -0.250 -0.125 -0.125 -0.086
      Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (two-tailed). ∗∗Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed).
      Associations between Emotional Availability and Couple Adjustment

      To test for associations between quality of mother–child interactions and couple adjustment, the Spearman’s Rho coefficient was applied to the DAS reliable scores (Dyadic Consensus – DC, Dyadic Satisfaction – DS, and DAS total scores) and to the scores obtained through the EAS during T1, T3, and T4. Table 7 reports associations between EA, couple adjustment and perceived social support. As it is possible to see, regarding T1, statistically significant inverse correlations were reported between maternal sensitivity, Dyadic Consensus and the DAS total score, and between maternal structuring and Dyadic Satisfaction. During T3, significant negative correlations were reported between maternal structuring, Dyadic Consensus and the DAS total score, and between maternal non-intrusiveness, Dyadic Consensus and the DAS total score. No statistically significant associations between Couple adjustment and EAS scores during T4 were found.

      Associations between EA, couple adjustment, and perceived social support.

      DAS_DC DAS_DS DAS_Tot MSPSS Other MSPSS Family MSPSS Friends MSPSS
      Sens T1 -0.578ˆ* -0.306 -0.558ˆ* 0.027 0.207 -0.016 0.094
      T3 -0.443 -0.034 -0.387 -0.031 0.081 0.229 0.187
      T4 -0.543 -0.259 -0.402 0.059 0.321 -0.433 0.029
      Struct T1 -0.355 -0.477ˆ* -0.449 -0.177 0.098 -0.183 -0.116
      T3 -0.539ˆ* -0.171 -0.511 0.039 -0.045 -0.036 0.009
      T4 -0.112 -0.202 -0.027 0.087 0.446 -0.532 0.044
      Nonint T1 -0.270 -0.306 -0.250 0.122 0.364 0.079 0.238
      T3 -0.668ˆ** -0.351 -0.630 0.113 -0.009 -0.103 -0.015
      T4 -0.255 -0.353 -0.182 0.231 0.178 -0.473 0.025
      Nonhos T1 -0.265 -0.359 -0.379 -0.473ˆ* -0.214 -0.382 -0.460ˆ*
      T3 -0.291 -0.086 -0.313 0.185 0.056 0.112 0.102
      T4
      Ch. Resp T1 -0.211 -0.275 -0.255 -0.309 -0.054 0.092 -0.042
      T3 -0.378 -0.099 -0.374 0.088 0.049 0.086 0.049
      T4 0.107 -0.302 0.022 0.110 -0.066 -0.325 -0.097
      Ch. Involv T1 -0.354 -0.636ˆ** -0.421 -0.110 0.033 0.088 0.029
      T2 -0.338 -0.153 -0.324 -0.001 0.049 0.288 0.092
      T3 0.039 -0.374 -0.062 0.459 0.464 -0.189 0.258
      Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (two-tailed). ∗∗Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed).
      Associations between Emotional Availability and Perceived Social Support

      To test for associations between quality of mother–child interactions and the dimensions of perceived social support the Spearman’s Rho coefficient was applied to the scores of the MSPSS and to the EAS during T1, T3, and T4. As it is possible to observe in Table 7, regarding T1, negative associations were found between maternal non-hostility, the social support received by a significant other, and the total score of the MSPSS. No associations were found between EAS during T3 and T4 and the perceived social support.

      Associations between Psychological Distress, Couple Adjustment, and Perceived Social Support

      Spearman’s Rho was applied to test for associations between perceived social support, couple adjustment, and psychological distress. No statistically significant associations were found between couple adjustment and perceived social support, neither between couple adjustment and psychological distress. With respect to psychological distress and perceived social support, statistically significant correlations were found between paranoid ideation and the support received by a significant other (r = -0.492, p = 0.027), the total score of the MSPSS (r = -0.512, p = 0.021).

      Associations between Improvements in Emotional Availability, Psychological Distress, Couple Adjustment, and Perceived Social Support

      In order to assess whether there were associations between EA improvements among the three different timepoints and the other measures, the variance score of EA between timepoints (i.e., from T1 to T3, from T3 to T4, and from T1 to T4) was calculated subtracting the scores at later stages with scores at previous stages. Subsequently, Spearman’s Rho correlations were run between the variance scores of EA and the other measures (i.e., psychological distress, perceived dyadic satisfaction, and perceived social support). Some associations were detected for SCL-90-R, namely a negative relationship between higher psychological distress at three scales of the SCL-90-R (assessed at time 1) and lower variance scores between T1 and T4. More specifically, a negative relationship was detected between Interpersonal Sensitivity and variance score of Child Responsiveness (r = -537, p < 0.05), Social Phobia and variance score of Child Involvement (r = -519, p < 0.05), Psychoticism and variance score of Child Responsiveness (r = -531, p < 0.05). Taken together, these results indicate that the higher psychological distress mothers experience at T1, the less their interaction quality improved during massage lessons.

      Discussion

      The first aim of the present study was to investigate EA during infant massage classes and to observe if an improvement in mother–child interactions occurred. Up to date, only few studies applied the EAS in the context of infant massage (Hays, 2014) and, at least to our knowledge, none to unselected populations. Secondly, according to the literature that highlights the intervention of multiple factors in determining the quality of parenting practices (Belsky, 1984; Feiring et al., 1987; Jennings et al., 1991; Melson et al., 1993; Bender and Losel, 1998; Singer et al., 2003; Favez et al., 2006), we aimed to test whether aspects such as the maternal perception of couple adjustment, social support and psychological wellbeing were associated to mother–child EA. At the beginning of the courses, the dyads showed scores that ranged from adequate to complicated EA. However, the results reported an increase in all the six dimensions concurrently with the ongoing of the infant massage course. Mothers enrolled in the courses seemed to gradually become more sensitive and responsive toward their children’s bids, as well as more able to provide adequate scaffolding during the lessons. At the same time, they seemed also to become less intrusive, providing more space to the interactions and being less interfering, both psychologically and physically, although the specific setting of infant massage explicitly requires tactile stimulation and the physical manipulation of the infant’s body. Simultaneously, a significant increase in child responsiveness was recorded, suggesting an improvement in the ability to find an adequate balance between self- and interactive-regulation and in the possibility to organize affects and behaviors in a coordinated way in order to respond to the caregiver’s bids. This parallel increase of maternal sensitivity and child responsiveness, the major dimensions indicative of the adult’s and the child’s EA, seems to support the hypothesis of bi-directionality and reciprocity within adult–child relationships (Sander, 1977; Tronick, 1989; Beebe and Lachmann, 2002). Although these improvements are not directly attributable to the course of infant massage, also due to the absence of a control group, this seems a plausible hypothesis, especially considering the literature concerning short-term stability of EA (Robinson et al., 1993; Biringen et al., 1995; Bornstein et al., 2006). Moreover, several studies report a positive influence on adult–child relationships exerted by infant massage (Onozawa et al., 2001; Lee, 2006). More research is needed in future in order to discern more clearly how these improvements in adult–child interactions might be affected by the massage course itself, by developmental processes or by the progressive mutual adjustment that the dyad reaches after delivery.

      As far as it concerns maternal psychological wellbeing, as expected, a higher degree of adult psychopathology resulted associated with less optimal mother–child interactions, supporting the hypothesis that experiencing some kind of psychological distress might affect different domains of life, including the one of everyday interactions with one’s own child (Rogosch et al., 1992; Tronick and Weinberg, 1997; Anke, 2012). In particular, in our study, the major symptomatic scales negatively associated with the quality of mother–child exchanges were anxiety and psychoticism, considered as a graduated continuum from mild interpersonal alienation to first-rank symptoms of psychosis (Derogatis, 1977). Given the preliminary nature of this study, self-report measures were administered only during the first period considered; it would be interesting, in the future, to administer them also at the end of the massage course, in order to see whether an improvement in maternal psychological wellbeing occurs parallel to the improvement of mother–child interactions.

      Our hypotheses were not confirmed as far as it concerns the associations between mother–child interactions, couple adjustment and social support. The lack of associations seems to reflect in part the non-univocal results reported by the literature. In fact, although several studies reported the presence of associations between marital quality and parenting (Erel and Burman, 1995) the nature of these associations was not always clear (Grych, 2002). Sometimes they appeared positive (Carneiro et al., 2006), sometimes they were negative (Favez et al., 2006), in other cases they were absent (Barnett et al., 2008; Favez et al., 2013). This incongruence could be due to many methodological reasons, such as different samples, the different periods when the measures were administered as well to the use of different kinds of measures (observational vs. self-report). Moreover it is possible that the “spill over” effect (Engfer, 1988; Katz and Gottman, 1996) expected from marital quality and perceived social support toward adult–child interactions might differ according to the family system investigated (i.e., parental-dyadic vs. co-parental-dyadic) (McHale et al., 2000; Johnson, 2001; Lindsey and Caldera, 2006; Stroud et al., 2015). It is important to note that, differently from what it is often reported in the literature, all our mothers held satisfactory couple relationships. Maybe they were still experiencing the so-called “baby honeymoon” (Hobbs, 1965; Wallace and Gotlib, 1990) or maybe they were part of that portion of couples that do not face a decline in marital satisfaction after delivery (Holmes et al., 2013). Anyway, further analysis will be required in future, also expanding and improving research designs, in order to confirm or disconfirm these associations and to examine in depth their nature.

      The present study shows a series of limitations that might offer useful suggestions for future research. The first limit regards the sample; the small amount of the participants and the absence of a non-treated control group, in fact, prevent us to generalize the obtained results. A larger sample would allow to adopt more sound statistical analysis, while the presence of a control group would allow to compare the development of mother–child interactions between dyads that undergo infant massage courses and dyads without intervention, thus leading to a better explanation of the effective influence of infant massage upon the establishment of early adult–child interactions.

      Another limit regards the absence of a baseline assessment of mother–child interactions. Videotaping the dyads during massage lessons might have influenced the nature of mother–child interactions both in positive or in negative. On one side, in fact, the massage context might have acted as a buffering factor, preventing the mothers from enacting dysfunctional behaviors that otherwise could have been adopted; on the other hand, instead, finding themselves in a new situation and being asked to do something new (massaging their babies while being videotaped) might have made interactions more challenging for these women. In this sense, including a baseline assessment in future would favor a better control of the different intervening variables. Moreover, it should be taken into account that our study only included mothers and did not involve fathers. Expanding the research design in this direction in future would lead to two major consequences: first of all, the possibility to support and sustain also paternal functioning during the postpartum period; secondly the opportunity to increase and to deepen the comprehension of family processes.

      Finally, some considerations should be dedicated to the clinical implications of our study. These preliminary findings, in fact, seem to suggest the usefulness of infant massage for the strengthening and the enhancement of early healthy adult–child interactions. This cost-saving technique could provide a simple but effective way to favor the construction of early affective bonds; in this way, it could accompany a process existing per se and sustain the dyad during expected developmental challenges, whether necessary. Especially in a delicate interval such as the post-partum period this practice could become extremely important, since it could help the dyad to face the need of mutual adjustment, facilitating regulatory processes and the establishment of sleep-wake cycles. Moreover, a “guided” emotionally intense approach toward the infant could reassure the mothers, who often perceive the newborns as fragile and are afraid to touch them, making them more confident when handling their babies. From this perspective, infant massage constitutes a precious resource in terms of primary prevention, i.e., in terms of those interventions aimed at sustaining and enhancing the existing resources within the family system, since it can be offered as an enriching support also in the absence of adult psychopathology. A replication of these results in larger samples would thus encourage the diffusion of this non-invasive technique in terms both of relational support and enhancement of parenting abilities.

      Ethics Statement

      The study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Code of Ethics approved by the General Assembly of the Italian Association of Psychology held on March 27, 2015. Written consent was obtained from the participants.

      Author Contributions

      AS prepared the study design and supervised the research team; GB carried out the massage courses and recruited the sample. SF collected data and prepared data set. AP and MP wrote the introduction section of the manuscript, performed statistical analyses, and prepared tables and figures. AS, MP, and AP wrote the discussions section of the manuscript. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

      Conflict of Interest Statement

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      References Acolet D. Modei N. Ciannakoilopoulos X. Bond C. Clow A. Glover V. (1993). Changes in plasma cortisol and catecholamine concentrations in response to massage in preterm infants. Arch. Dis. Child 68 2931. 10.1136/adc.68.1_Spec_No.29 Adamson S. (1996). Teaching baby massage to new parents. Complement. Ther. Nurs. Midwifery 2 151159. 10.1016/S1353-6117(96)80049-7 Anke T. (2012). Mothers with severe mental illnes and infants-interactions and treatment. Tidsskr. Nor. Psykol. 49 10501060. Balsamo E. (2002). “Bambini immigrati e bisogni insoddisfatti: la via dell’etnopediatria,” in Mille Modi di Crescere. Bambini Immigrati e Modi di Cura ed. La Casa di Tutti i Colori (Milano: Franco Angeli). Balsamo E. (2007). Sono Qui Con te. L’arte del Maternage. Torino: Il Leone Verde. Barnett M. A. Deng M. Mills-Koonce R. Willoughby M. Cox M. (2008). Interdependence of parenting of mothers and fathers of infants. J. Fam. Psychol. 22 561573. 10.1037/0893-3200.22.3.561 Beebe B. Lachmann F. (2002). Infant Research e Trattamento Degli Adulti: un Modello Sistemico-Diadico Delle Interazioni. Milano: Raffaello Cortina Editore 2003. Beebe B. Lachmann F. Jaffe J. Markese S. Buck K. A. Chen H. (2012). Maternal postpartum depressive symptoms and 4-month mother–infant interaction. Psychoanal. Psychol. 29 383. 10.1037/a0029387 Belsky J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: a process model. Child Dev 55 8396. 10.2307/1129836 Belsky J. (1986). Infant day care: a cause for concern? Zero Three 7 17. Belsky J. Rovine M. (1990). Patterns of marital change across the transition to parenthood: pregnancy to three years postpartum. J. Marriage Fam. 52 519. 10.2307/352833 Belsky J. Spanier G. B. Rovine M. (1983). Stability and change in marriage across the transition to parenthood. J. Marriage Fam. 52 567577. 10.2307/351661 Bender D. Losel F. (1998). “Protektive Faktoren der psyschisch gesunden Entwicklung junger menschen: ein beitrag zur kontroverse um saluto- versus pathogenetische Ansätze,” in Gesundheits- oder Krankheitstheorie? Saluto- versus Pathogenetisceh Ansatze im Gesundheitswesen eds Margraf J. Siegrist J. Neumer S. (Berlin: Springer) 117145. Biringen Z. (2008). Emotional Availability (EA) Scales Manual 4th Edn. Boulder: Colorado State University. Biringen Z. Emde R. N. Campos J. J. Appelbaum M. I. (1995). Affective reorganization in the infant, the mother, and the dyad: the role of upright locomotion and its timing. Child Dev. 66 499514. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00886.x Biringen Z. Easterbrooks M. A. (2012). Emotional availability: concept, research, and window on developmental psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 24 18. 10.1017/S0954579411000617 Blackwell P. (2000). The influence of touch on child development: implications for intervention. Infants Young Child. 13 2539. 10.1097/00001163-200013010-00006 Bornstein M. H. (2002). “Parenting infants,” in Handbook of Parentin: Children and Parenting Vol. 1 ed. Bornestein H. M. (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates) 343. Bornstein M. H. Gini M. Suwalsky J. T. D. Putnick D. L. Haynes O. M. (2006). Emotional availability in mother-child dyads: short-term stability and continuity from variable-centered and person-centered perspectives. Merrill Palmer Q. 52 547571. 10.1353/mpq.2006.0024 Bower D. Jia R. Schoppe-Sullivan S. J. Mangelsdorf S. C. Brown G. L. (2013). Trajectories of couple relationship satisfaction in families with infants the roles of parent gender, personality, and depression in first-time and experienced parents. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 30 389409. 10.1177/0265407512457656 Brazelton T. B. (1990). The Earliest Relationship. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Carneiro C. Corboz-Warnery A. Fivaz-Depeursinge E. (2006). The prenatal lausanne trilogue play: a new observationsl assessment tool of the prenatal co-parenting alliance. Infant Ment. Health J. 27 207228. 10.1002/imhj.20089 Chong A. Mickelson K. D. (2013). Perceived fairness and relationship satisfaction during the transition to parenthood the mediating role of spousal support. J. Fam. Issues 18 126. 10.1177/0192513X13516764 Cowan C. P. Cowan P. A. (2000). When Partners Become Parents: The Big Life Change for Couples. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. Cowan P. Hetherington M. (2001). Family Transitions. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cutrona C. E. (1996). Social Support in Couples: Marriage as a Resource in Times of Stress Vol. 13. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications Czarnocka J. Slade P. (2000). Prevalence and predictors of post-traumatic stress symptoms following childbirth. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 39 3551. 10.1348/014466500163095 De Chateau P. (1976). The influence of early contact on maternal and infant behavior on primaparae. Birth Fam. J. 3 149155. 10.1111/j.1523-536X.1976.tb01186 Derogatis L. R. (1977). SCL-90-R, Administration, Scoring, and Procedures Manual for the R(evised) Version. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University, School of Medicine. Dew J. Wilcox W. B. (2011). If Momma ain’t happy: explaining declines in marital satisfaction among new mothers. J. Marriage Fam. 73 112. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00782.x Diego M. A. Field T. Hernandez-Reif M. (2008). Temperature increases in preterm infants during massage therapy. Infant Behav. Dev. 31 149152. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.07.002 Dieter J. Field T. Hernandez-Reif M. Emory E. K. Redzepi M. (2003). Stable preterm infants gain more weight and sleep less after five days of massage therapy. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 28 403411. 10.1093/jpepsy/jsg030 Dieter J. N. I. Field T. Hernandez Reif M. Emory E. K. Redzepi M. (2001). Massage therapy promotes weight gain in preterm infants after only five days. Acta Pediatrica Doss B. D. Rhoades G. K. Stanley S. M. Markman H. J. (2009). The effect of the transition to parenthood on relationship quality: an 8-year prospective study. J. pers. Soc. Psychol. 96 601619. 10.1037/a0013969 Duan L. Li W. Shi F. (2002). Studying impact of touching on growth and development of infants. Chin. Nurs. Res. 16:5. Engfer A. (1988). “The interrelatedness of marriage and the mother-child relationship,” in Relationships Within Families: Mutual Influences ed. Stevenson-Hinde R. H. J. (New York, NY: Oxford University Press) 83103. Erel O. Burman B. (1995). Interrelatedness of marital relations and parent-child relations: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 118 108132. 10.1037/0033-2909.118.1.108 Favez N. Frascarolo F. Fivaz-Depeursinge E. (2006). Family alliance stability and change from pregnancy to toddlerhood and marital correlates. Swiss J. Psychol. 65 213220. 10.1024/1421-0185.65.4.213 Favez N. Frascarolo F. Lavancy Scaiola C. Corboz-warnery A. (2013). Prenatal representations of family in parents and coparental interactions as predictors of triadic interactions during infancy. Infant Ment. Health J. 34 2536. 10.1002/imhj.21372 Favez N. Lopes F. Bernard M. Frascarolo F. Lavanchy Scaiola C. Corboz-Warnery A. (2012). The development of family alliance from pregnancy to toddlerhood and child outcomes at 5 years. Fam. Process 51 542556. 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2012.01419.x Feijò L. Hernandez-Reif M. Field T. Burns W. Valley-Gray S. Simco E. (2006). Mothers’ depressed mood and anxiety levels are reduced after massaging their preterm infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 29 476480. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.02.003 Feiring C. Fox N. A. Jaskir J. Lewis M. (1987). The relation between social support, infant risk status and mother–infant interaction. Dev. Psychol. 23 400. 10.1037/0012-1649.23.3.400 Ferber S. G. Feldman R. Kohelet D. Kuint J. Dollberg S. Arbel E. (2005). Massage therapy facilitates mother–infant interaction in premature infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 28 7481. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2004.07.004 Ferber S. G. Kuint J. Weller A. Feldman R. Dollberg S. Arbel E. (2002). Massage therapy by mothers and trained professional enhances weight gain in preterm infants. Early. Hum. Dev 67 3745. 10.1016/S0378-3782(01)00249-3 Field T. (2002). “Preterm infant massage therapy studies: an American approach,” in Seminars in Neonatology Vol. 7 (Philadelphia, PA: WB Saunders) 487494. 10.1053/siny.2002.0153 Field T. Grizzle N. Scafidi F. Abrams S. Richardson S. (1996). Massage therapy for infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behav. Dev. 19 107112. 10.1016/S0163-6383(96)90048-X Field T. Hernandez-Reif M. Diego M. Feijo L. Vera Y. Gil K. (2004). Massage therapy by parents improves early growth and development. Infant Behav. Dev. 27 435442. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2004.03.004 Field T. Scafidi F. Schanberg S. M. (1987). Massage of preterm newborns to improve growth and development. Pediatr. Nurs. 13 385387. Findler L. Taubman-Ben-Ari O. Jacob K. (2008). Internal and external contributors to maternal mental health and marital adaptation one year after birth: comparisons of mothers of pre-term and full-term twins. Women and health 46 3960. 10.1300/J013v46n04_03 Forman D. R. O’hara M. W. Stuart S. Gorman L. L. Larsen K. E. Coy K. C. (2007). Effective treatment for postpartum depression is not sufficient to improve the developing mother–child relationship. Dev. Psychopathol. 19 585602. 10.1017/S0954579407070289 Gavin A. R. Hill K. G. Hawkins J. D. Maas C. (2011). The role of maternal early-life and later-life risk factors on offspring low birth weight: findings from a three-generational study. J. Adolesc. Health 49 166171. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.11.246 Gentili P. Contreras L. Cassaniti M. D’Arista F. (2002). La dyadic adjustment scale. Una misura dell’adattamento di coppia. Minerva Psichiatr. 43 107116. Gjerdingen D. K. Center B. A. (2005). First-time parents’ postpartum changes in employment, childcare, and housework responsibilities. Soc. Sci. Res.34 103116. 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2003.11.005 Glasheen C. Colpe L. Hoffman V. Warren L. K. (2015). Prevalence of serious psychological distress and mental health treatment in a national sample of pregnant and postpartum women. Matern. Child Health J. 19 204216. 10.1007/s10995-014-1511-2 Glazier R. H. Elgar F. J. Goel V. Holzapfel S. (2004). Stress, social support, and emotional distress in a community sample of pregnant women. J. Psychosom. Obstet. Gynecol. 25 247255. 10.1080/01674820400024406 Goodman S. H. (2007). Depression in mothers. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 3 107135. 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091401 Goodman S. H. Rouse M. H. Connell A. M. Broth M. R. Hall C. M. Heyward D. (2011). Maternal depression and child psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 14 127. 10.1007/s10567-010-0080-1 Grych J. H. (2002). “Marital relationships and parenting,” in Handbook of Parenting Vol. 4 ed. Bornstein M. H. (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum) 231251. Harlow H. F. Zimmermann R. R. (1958). The development of affectional responses in infant monkeys. Proc. Am. Philos. Soc. 102 501509. Hay D. F. Kumar R. (1995). Interpreting the effects of mothers’ postnatal depression on children’s intelligence: a critique and re-analysis. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 25 165181. 10.1007/BF02251301 Hays L. J. (2014). Impact Upon Emotional Availability: Infant GERD and Infant Massage Therapy. Ph.D. thesis, University of Colorado Denver, CO. Hernandez-Reif M. Diego M. Field T. (2007). Preterm infants show reduced stress behaviors and activity after 5 days of massage therapy. Infant Behav. Dev. 30 557561. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.04.002 Herwig J. E. Wirtz M. Bengel J. (2004). Depression, partnership, social support, and parenting: interaction of maternal factors with behavioral problems of the child. J. Affect. Disord. 80 199208. 10.1016/S0165-0327(03)00112-5 Hobbs D. (1965). Parenthood as crisis: a third study. J. Marriage Fam. 27 367372. 10.2307/350281 Holmes E. K. Sasaki T. Hazen N. L. (2013). Smooth versus rocky transitions to parenthood: family systems in developmental context. Fam. Relat. 62 824837. 10.1111/fare.12041 Horwitz S. M. Briggs-Gowan M. J. Storfer-Isser A. Carter A. S. (2007). Prevalence, correlates, and persistence of maternal depression. J. Women’s Health 16 678691. 10.1089/jwh.2006.0185 Ingersoll-Dayton B. Morgan D. Antonucci T. (1997). The effects of positive and negative social exchanges on aging adults. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 52 S190S199. 10.1093/geronb/52B.4.S190 Jackson J. B. Miller R. B. Oka M. Henry R. G. (2014). Gender differences in marital satisfaction: a meta-analysis. J. Marriage Fam. 76 105129. 10.1111/jomf.12077 Jean A. D. L. Stack D. M. (2009). Functions of maternal touch and infants’ affect during face-to-face interactions: new directions for the still-face. Infant Behav. Dev. 32 123128. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.09.008 Jean A. D. L. Stack D. M. Girouard N. Fogel A. (2004). “Maternal touching during interactions: the influence of infant age and social context,” in Proceedings of the International Conference of Infant Studies Chicago, IL. Jennings K. D. Stagg V. Connors R. E. (1991). Social networks and mothers’ interactions with their preschool children. Child Dev 62 966978. 10.2307/1131146 Jinon S. (1996). “The effect of infant massage on growth of the preterm infant,” in Increasing Safe and Successful Pregnancy eds Yarbes Almirante C. De Luma M. (Amsterdam: Elsevier Science) 265269. Johnson V. K. (2001). Marital interaction, family organization, and differences in parenting behavior: explaining variations across family interaction contexts. Fam. Process 40 333342. 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2001.4030100333.x Kaplan B. H. Cassel J. C. Gore S. (1977). Social support and health. Med. Care 15 4758. 10.1097/00005650-197705001-00006 Katz L. Gottman J. M. (1996). “Spillover effects of marital conflict: in search of parenting and coparenting mechanisms,” in Understanding how Family-Leve Dynamics Affect Children’s Development: Studies of Two-Parents Families eds McHale J. Cowan P. A. (San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass) 5776. Ke G. Ling X. Li L. (2001). Clinical survey of improved massage method developing infants’ growth and development. China Nurse 36 278280. Kim T. I. Shin Y. H. White-Traut A. C. (2003). Multisensory intervention improves physical growth and illness rates in Korean orphaned newborn infants. Res. Nurs. Health 26 424433. 10.1002/nur.10105 Kohn J. L. Rholes W. Simpson J. A. Martin A. Tran S. Wilson C. L. (2012). Changes in marital satisfaction across the transition to parenthood: the role of adult attachment orientations. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 38 15061522. 10.1177/0146167212454548 Koniak-Griffin D. Ludington-Hoe S. M. (1988). Developmental and temperament outcomes of sensory stimulation in healthy infants. Nurs. Res. 37 7076. 10.1097/00006199-198803000-00002 Kuhn C. Schandberg S. Field T. Symansky R. Zimmerman E. Scafidi F. (1991). Tactile kinesthetiv stimulation effects on sympathetic and adrenocortical function in preterm infants. J. Pediatr. 119 434440. 10.1016/S0022-3476(05)82059-1 Lawrence E. Rothman A. D. Cobb R. J. Rothman M. T. Bradbury T. N. (2008). Marital satisfaction across the transition to parenthood. J. Fam. Psychol. 22 4150. 10.1037/0893-3200.22.1.41 Lee H. (2006). The effects of infant massage on weight, height, and mother-infant interaction. J. Korean Acad. Nurs. 36 13311339. Lindsey E. W. Caldera Y. M. (2006). Mother-father-child triadic interaction and mother-child dyadic interaction: gender differences within and between contexts. Sex Roles 55 511521. 10.1007/s11199-006-9106-z Liu C. (2005). The effect of touching on healthy infants’weight. J. Huaihai Med. 23 137. Lu J. Li J. Wu L. (2005). A study of the effect of touch on health in infants. Pract. Clin. Med. 6 119121. McHale J. P. Kuersten-Hogan R. Lauretti A. Rasmussen J. L. (2000). Parental reports of coparenting and observed coparenting behavior during the toddler period. J. Fam. Psychol. 14 220234. 10.1037/0893-3200.14.2.220 Melson G. F. Ladd G. W. Hsu H. C. (1993). Maternal support networks, maternal cognitions, and young children’s social, and cognitive development. Child Dev. 64 14011417. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb02960.x Mitchell R. E. Trickett E. J. (1980). Task force report: social networks as mediators of social support. Community Ment. Health J. 16 2744. 10.1007/BF00780665 Montagu A. (1971). Touching: The Human Significance of the Skin. Oxford: Columbia Univ Press. Moszkowski R. J. Stack D. M. (2007). Infant touching behaviour during mother-infant face-to-face interactions. Infant Child Dev. 16 307319. 10.1002/icd.510 Moyer Mileur L. Luetkemeier M. Boomer L. Chan G. M. (1995). Effect of physical activity on bone mineralization in premature infants. J. Pediatr. 127 620625. 10.1016/S0022-3476(95)70127-3 Murray L. Fearon P. Cooper P. (2015). “Postnatal depression, mother–infant interactions, and child development. prospects for screening and treatment,” in Identifying Perinatal Depression and Anxiety: Evidence-based Practice in Screening, Psychosocial Assessment and Management eds Milgrom J. Gemmill A. W. (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons) 139164. Na Z. Xie H. Y. Huang J. H. (2005). The effect of infant massage on growth. New J. Tradit. Chin. Med. 37 6970. Nelson C. Bosquet M. (2000). “Neurobiology of fetal and infant development: implications for infant mental health,” in Handbook of Infant Mental Health 2nd Edn ed. Zeanah C. H. Jr. (New York, NY: Guilford Press). O’Higgins M. Roberts I. Glover V. (2008). Postnatal depression and mother and infant outcomes after infant massage. J. Affect. Disord. 109 189192. 10.1016/j.jad.2007.10.027 Onozawa K. Glover V. Adams D. Modi N. Kumar R. C. (2001). Infant massage improves mother–infant interaction for mothers with postnatal depression. J. Affect. Disord. 63 201207. 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00198-1 Oswalt K. L. Biasini F. J. Wilson L. L. Mrug S. (2009). Outcomes of a massage intervention on teen mothers: s pilot study. Pediatr. Nurs. 35 284317. Pinelli J. (2000). Effects of family coping and resources on family adjustment and parental stress in the acute phase of the NICU experience. Neonatal Netw. 19 2737. 10.1891/0730-0832.19.6.27 Prezza M. Principato M. C. (2002). “La rete sociale e il sostegno sociale,” in Conoscere la Comunità eds Prezza M. Santinello M. (Bologna: Il Mulino) 193234. Relier J. P. (1994). Amarlo Prima che Nasca. Il Legame Madre-Figlio Prima Della Nascita. Firenze: Le Lettere. Robertson J. Bowlby J. (1952). Responses of young children to separation from their mothers. Courrier Centre Int. Enfance 2 131142. Robinson J. Little C. Biringen Z. (1993). Emotional communication in mother-toddler relationships: evidence for early gender differentiation. Merrill Palmer Q. 39 496517. Rogosch F. A. Mowbray C. T. Bogat G. A. (1992). Determinants of parenting attitudes in mothers with severe psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 4 469487. 10.1017/S0954579400000900 Rowe J. Jones L. (2010). Discharge and beyond. A longitudinal study comparing stress and coping in parents of preterm infants. J. Neonatal Nurs. 16 258266. 10.1016/j.jnn.2010.07.018 Sander L. (1977). “The regulation of exchange in the infant-caretaker system and some aspect of the context-content realationship,” in Interaction, Conversation, and the Development of Language eds Lewis M. Rosemblum L. (New York, NY: Wiley). Sarno I. Preti E. Prunas A. Madeddu F. (2011). SCL-90-R. Symptom Checklist-90-R. Adattamento italiano. Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali. Saurel-Cubizolles M. J. Romito P. Lelong N. (2007). Women’s psychological health according to their maternal status: a study in France. J. Psychosom. Obstet. Gynecol. 28 243249. 10.1080/01674820701350351 Scafidi F. Field T. (1996). Massage therapy improves behaviour in neonates born to HIV positive mothers. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 21 889898. 10.1093/jpepsy/21.6.889 Scafidi F. Field T. Schanberg S. Bauer C. Tucci K. Roberts J. (1990). Massage stimulates growth in preterm infants: a replication. Infant Behav. Dev. 13 167188. 10.1016/0163-6383(90)90029-8 Scafidi F. Field T. M. Schanberg S. M. Bauer C. R. Vega-Lahr N. Garcia R. (1986). Effects of tactile/kinesthetic stimulation on the clinical course and sleep/wake behavior of preterm neonates. Infant Behav. Dev. 9 91105. 10.1016/0163-6383(86)90041-X Shapiro A. F. Gottman J. M. Carrere S. (2000). The baby and the marriage: identifying factors that buffer against decline in marital satisfaction after the first baby arrives. J. Fam. Psychol. 14 59. 10.1037/0893-3200.14.1.59 Singer L. T. Fulton S. Davillier M. Koshy D. Salvator A. Baley J. E. (2003). Effects of infant risk status and maternal psychological distress on maternal-infant interactions during the first year of life. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 24 233241. 10.1097/00004703-200308000-00003 Spanier G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: new scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. J. Marriage Fam. 38 1528. 10.2307/350547 Stack D. M. (2004). “Touching during mother-infant interactions,” Touch and Massage in Early Child Development ed. Field T. M. (New York: Johnson and Johnson Pediatric Institute) 49-81. Stack D. M. Muir D. W. (1990). Tactile stimulation as a component of social interchange: new interpretations for the still-face effect. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 8 131145. 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1990.tb00828.x Stack D. M. Muir D. W. (1992). Adult tactile stimulation during face-to-face interactions modulates five-month-olds’ affect and attention. Child Dev. 63 15091525. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01711.x Stemp P. S. Turner R. J. Noh S. (1986). Psychological distress in the postpartum period: the significance of social support. J. Marriage Fam. 48 271277. 10.2307/352394 Stork H. (1986). Enfances Indiennes. Étude de Psychologie Transculturelle et Comparée du Jeun Enfant. Paris: Paidos-Le Centurion. Stroud C. B. Meyers K. M. Wilson S. Durbin E. (2015). Marital quality spillover and young children’ìs adjustment: evidence for dyadic and triadic parenting as mechanisms. J. Clin. Chind Adolesc. Psychol. 44 800813. 10.1080/15374416.2014.900720 Sutter-Dallay A. L. (2006). Impact des Symptômes Dépressifs Maternels Postnataux Précoces sur le Développement Cognitif et Moteur du Nourrisson. Ph.D. thesis, University of Bordeaux Bordeaux 2. Taylor S. E. (2011). “Social support: a review,” in The Handbook of Health Psychology ed. Friedman M. S. (New York, NY: Oxford University Press) 189214. Terry D. J. McHugh T. A. Noller P. (1991). Role dissatisfaction and the decline in marital quality across the transition to parenthood. Aust. J. Psychol. 43 129132. 10.1080/00049539108260136 Tronick E. Z. (1995). “Touch in mother-infant interactions,” in Touch Early Development ed. Field T. M. (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.) 5365. Trillingsgaard T. Baucom K. J. Heyman R. E. (2014). Predictors of change in relationship satisfaction during the transition to parenthood. Fam. Relat. 63 667679. 10.1111/fare.12089 Tronick E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. Am. Psychol. 44 112119. 10.1037/0003-066X.44.2.112 Tronick E. Z. Weinberg M. K. (1997). “Le madri depresse e i loro bambini: l’insuccesso nella formazione di stati di coscienza diadici,” in Depressione del Post-Partum e Sviluppo del Bambino eds Murray L. Cooper P. J. (Roma: Cic Edizioni Internazionali) 4772. Twenge J. M. Campbell W. K. Foster C. A. (2003). Parenthood and marital satisfaction: a meta-analytic review. J. Marriage Fam. 65 574583. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2003.00574.x Underdown A. (2009). “The power of touch–exploring infant massage,” in Keeping the Baby in Mind. Infant Mental Health Practice eds Barlow J. Svanberg P. O. (London: Routledge) 1728. Wallace P. M. Gotlib I. H. (1990). Marital adjustment during the transition to parenthood: stability and predictors of change. J. Marriage Fam. 52 2129. 10.2307/352834 Weinberg M. K. Tronick E. Z. (1997). Maternal depression and infant maladjustment: a failure of mutual regulation. handb. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 1 177191. Weiss S. J. Wilson P. Hertenstein M. J. Campos R. (2000). The tactile context of a mother’s caregiving: implications for attachment of low birth weight infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 23 91111. 10.1016/s0163-6383(00)00030-8 Wheeden A. Scafidi F. Field T. Ironson G. Valdeon C. Bandstra E. (1993). Massage effects on cocaine exposed preterm neonates. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 14 318322. 10.1097/00004703-199310000-00005 Whisman M. A. (2001). “The association between depression and marital dissatisfaction,” in Marital and Family Processes in Depression: A Scientific Foundation for Clinical Practice ed. Beach S. R. H. (Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association) 324. Zimet G. D. Powell S. S. Farley G. K. Werkman S. Berkoff K. A. (1988). Psychometric characteristics of the multidimensional scale of perceived social support. J. Pers. Assess. 55 610617. 10.1080/00223891.1990.9674095

      This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Code of Ethics approved by the General Assembly of the Italian Association of Psychology held on March 27, 2015 with written informed consent from all subjects.

      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016ichugao.com.cn
      www.rbtaobao.com.cn
      mmshop.net.cn
      www.tflv7.net.cn
      wdmice.com.cn
      oojoo.com.cn
      www.sitaobi.com.cn
      u8cbi.com.cn
      www.sjzylzm.com.cn
      wwrrgo.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p