Edited by:
Reviewed by:
*Correspondence:
This article was submitted to Psychology for Clinical Settings, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
Infant massage programs have proved to be effective in enhancing post-natal development of highly risk infants, such as preterm newborns and drug or HIV exposed children. Less studies have focused on the role of infant massage in supporting the co-construction of early adult–child relationships. In line with this lack of literature, the present paper reports on a pilot study aimed at investigating longitudinally the quality of mother–child interactions, with specific reference to emotional availability (EA), in a group of mother–child pairs involved in infant massage classes. Moreover, associations between mother–child EA, maternal wellbeing, marital adjustment, and social support were also investigated, with the hypothesis to find a link between low maternal distress, high couple satisfaction and high perceived support and interactions of better quality in the dyads. The study involved 20 mothers and their children, aged between 2 and 7 months, who participated to infant massage classes. The assessment took place at three stages: at the beginning of massage course, at the end of it and at 1-month follow-up. At the first stage of assessment self-report questionnaires were administered to examine the presence of maternal psychiatric symptoms (SCL-90-R), perceived social support (MSPSS), and marital adjustment (Dyadic Adjustment Scale); dyadic interactions were observed and rated with the Emotional Availability Scales (
香京julia种子在线播放
The practice of infant massage represents a simple but effective way to enhance and strengthen healthy social and emotional relationships between adults and children in early infancy; this is true both from a relational and a practical point of view. On the one hand, in fact, massaging a baby requires and intensifies a series of multimodal and interactive competencies (such as emotional expression, eye-contact, physical touch, vocal communication, and turn-taking) that usually characterize adult–child daily repertoires; in this sense, it represents a privileged moment to cultivate and strengthen the relationship. On the other hand, instead, it is cost-saving and no contraindications were reported up to now; thus, it can be used frequently and without risk, accompanying and supporting a process existing per se, which is the one of adult–infant bonding.
Together with the practices of breastfeeding, baby carrying, and co-sleeping, infant massage is part of a wider “caretaking-package” which involves a set of behaviors necessary to satisfy the child’s needs for contact, holding, communication, and feeding; these needs are simple and primitive but often unrecognized (
From a neural-developmental point of view, touch is one of the first sensorial systems to be activated in the fetus during pregnancy, becoming, thus, one of the primary means of communication with the surrounding environment (
Infant touching and infant massage are also part of a “high-” or “proximal-contact” model of caretaking (
Research on infant massage as intervention has been applied especially to preterm infants. In this particular population the exposure to infant massage sessions resulted in an improvement in several health indexes, such as weight-gain, increases in length, head circumference, bone density, and body temperature (
Moreover, massage therapy resulted also capable of modifying the distribution of sleep/awake states, favoring longer periods of active alertness and reducing excitability (
As far as it concerns other groups at high-risk, infant massage sessions have worked successfully on HIV- and drug-exposed newborns, leading to fewer medical complications, less irritability and increased weight, and to an improvement in the performance on social and emotional scales (
Although most non-at-risk infants will receive adequate sensitive handling, the administration of infant massage with regular frequency can represent an useful way to support parenting and to promote caregivers’ sensitive touch in early infancy (
Massages can be given to children on a daily basis and they are economically saving when parents are enrolled as therapists (
Promoting infant massage lessons may represent an ideal way to support parenting and to support early emotional and social relationships between adults and children, useful both for high- and low-risk groups (
Parenting encompasses a broad range of nurturing and care-taking actions performed by caregivers toward the child. The actual behaviors that parents provide are among the most salient aspects of parenting, since the most of the experience of infants stems directly from interactions with caregivers occurring daily (
The transition to parenthood is an event of the family life cycle that asks the couple to face potentially stressful changes and challenges (
The transition toward parenthood is a very critical and stressful stage that may lead to serious psychological distress symptoms in pregnant women and women that recently gave birth to their offsprings, ranging from 4.8% (
Depression represents one of the most frequent distress conditions in the context of the transition to motherhood; review and meta-analytic studies have demonstrated that depression is linked to a range of adverse behavioral and emotional outcomes for the child, in terms of psychopathology and negative affects and behaviors, especially for younger children (
In a wider perspective, parenting and child development inextricably take place in the context of social relationships. Social support can be defined as the amount of advice received and personal needs fulfilled through the presence and interaction with significant others, within or outside the family, such as partners, relatives, or friends (
Moreover, according to
Interestingly,
To our knowledge, to date no studies have examined the quality of mother–infant interactions in the context of infant massage through the application of the Emotional Availability Scales (EAS;
The present study adopted a descriptive and correlational design for providing preliminary data on the longitudinal investigation of mother–infant EA during massage classes.
The sample of the study was composed of 20 mother–child pairs selected from a larger group of participants enrolled in an infant massage course. The children’s (10 boys and 10 girls) age ranged from 2 to 7 months (
Socio-demographic information regarding different domains such as level of education and occupation was collected through a self-report questionnaire. Twenty percent of the mothers reported to be an only child, while the others reported one (40%) or more (40%) siblings. Concerning education, the 10% of the group declared to have a middle school certificate, while the others reported to have an high school diploma (35%) or an academic degree (50%). As far as it regards work, 15% of the mothers reported to be unemployed, while the remaining declared to have a job in the working class (10%), as employees (45%) or in other forms (30%).
Massage courses were presented during childbirth classes in different Venetian sanitary districts. Once enrolled the women were contacted after delivery and invited to participate to the course. Participation was free. The dyads were divided randomly in groups of 5, each of these represented a massage class. At the beginning of the course each mother was given a battery of self-report questionnaires to fill in at home, aimed at investigating socio-demographic information, maternal psychological well being, marital relationships and perceived social support (see the section Quality of Mother–Child Interactions during Massage Lessons). The participants were told that they would have been videotaped three times during the cycle of massage classes (during T1, T3, and T4). Informed consent was asked to both parents before videotaping the baby.
The dyads were offered a cycle of four weekly lessons on infant massage. Each encounter lasted about an hour and a half. Massage courses were provided by a trained psychotherapist. During the course each room was warm and quiet in its atmosphere. Cameras were located in different angles of the room at a distance that allowed to capture the most salient aspects of each dyad’s interaction without interfering with the appropriate atmosphere.
Every mother was told to bring a cushion big enough to contain the baby and some natural oil. Activities took place on the floor, sitting in circle on wickers, in front of their babies, holding cushions firm with their legs. This condition allowed the mothers to hold their babies and to enhance visual contact with them. The conductor sit in the center of the circle in order to be visible for all the participants, and reproduced on a doll the various techniques taught during each encounter. In this way, the conductor did not touch the babies but only showed the participants how to massage.
The first lesson (T1) concerned an initial introduction to the massage course and to the research project. After a brief presentation of the equipe, the mothers were given notions in order to experience in the best way possible the encounters: they were invited to pay attention and to follow their children’s needs, to wait for optimal moments before beginning massage sessions, to feed their babies or to let them sleep whether necessary, to calm them down as they were used to. After a brief moment of relaxation, the mothers were invited to undress their infants from waist down, in order to be able to massage their legs and their abdomen. Instructions focused on how to handle the baby and how to touch, on how to pay attention and to become aware of the baby’s signals. The mothers were lead through a group discussion on massage benefits and they were invited to repeat the procedure at home. The first encounter was video-recorded.
During the second lesson (T2) the conductor introduced another sequence of massage procedures, focusing on face, superior arts, and chest. Alternative positions to use massage were presented and parents were positively reinforced during their efforts. Moreover, the mothers were given the possibility to share their feelings about maternity with the group. The conductor stressed the fact that the massage represents a technique with a particular focus on mother–child relationship, a relationship that can be improved dedicating more time to practice at home and, in turn, to the relationship.
The third lesson (T3) concerned the repetition of the entire sequence applied to the frontal body, also adding some suggestions on how to approach the back during massage. Again, some space during this encounter was left to allow the mothers to talk about their experience of the postpartum period. A bibliography was suggested to acquire more knowledge regarding the themes emerged during the cycle of lessons. This encounter was video-recorded.
After a month the group met again for a so-called moment of reinforce and of review of the techniques previously learned. This session was videotaped (T4) and considered as a follow-up.
The first (T1), the third (T3), and the fourth (T4) lessons were videotaped. For every dyad 20 min of mother–child interaction were recorded during each episode. The interactions were coded using the fourth version of the EAS (
Each EA dimension is given a global score on a 7-point scale, with higher ratings referring to more optimal features. Scores between 5 and 7 are considered adequate and index of a healthy relationship. Scores around 4 indicate inconsistency, (i.e., behaviors that are appropriate in some way but that are not fully healthy). Scores of 3 or below indicate less optimal interactions were problematic behaviors might arise (scores of 1 or 2). The coding refers to the global quality of the interaction observed rather than on specific behaviors. To get a more specific profile of the adult–child relationship, the EA assessment system provides the coders also a Clinical-Screener that allows to attribute each member of the dyad to one of four “zones” (according to the scores given to maternal sensitivity and child responsiveness), which represent four possible categories of EA: the Emotionally Available zone, the Complicated zone, the Detached zone and the Problematic zone. Mother–child interactions were coded by two independent raters previously trained on the EA coding system in order to reach satisfactory reliability with the Biringen’s lab. Inter-rater reliability was calculated on the 20% of the videos using ICCs which ranged from 0.80 to 0.95.
At the beginning of the massage course the mothers were given a battery of self-report questionnaires to fill in at home before the second lesson. The instruments aimed to assess socio-demographic information, maternal psychological wellbeing, marital relationships and perceived social support.
The
The
The
Data were analyzed using IBM SPSS statistics vers. 23. Firstly, descriptive statistics (frequencies, mean scores, and percentages) were examined. Secondly, non-parametric tests were applied; more specifically, we adopted the Friedman and Wilcoxon signed-rank and the Spearman’s Rho to test for any associations between the different instruments adopted in the research design.
During preliminary analysis, Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was used to assess the reliability of the instruments. Descriptive statistics (average scores, frequencies, and percentages) were examined.
The application of Cronbach’s alpha coefficient, to all the three periods considered, reported good reliability for EA maternal scales (0.85 ≤ α ≤ 0.89), for the EA child’s scales (0.73 ≤ α ≤ 0.80) and for all the six scales considered globally (0.87 ≤ α ≤ 0.90).
Average scores and standard deviations of the Emotional Availability Scales (EAS) applied during T1, T3, and T4.
Variables | T1 ( |
T3 ( |
T4 ( |
---|---|---|---|
5.15 (1.07) | 6.03 (1.06) | 6.85 (0.38) | |
4.93 (0.98) | 5.76 (1.02) | 6.73 (0.44) | |
4.73 (0.82) | 5.79 (0.75) | 6.54 (0.63) | |
6.05 (0.94) | 6.53 (0.79) | 7.00 (0.00) | |
4.68 (1.05) | 5.58 (0.95) | 6.50 (0.50) | |
4.15 (1.09) | 5.05 (1.04) | 6.00 (0.79) |
Distribution of the dyads on the zones of the emotional availability (EA) clinical screener during T1, T3, and T4.
T1 ( |
T3 ( |
T4 ( |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mother | Child | Mother | Child | Mother | Child | |
7 (35%) | 5 (25%) | 15 (78.95%) | 14 (73.68%) | 13 (100%) | 13 (100%) | |
11 (55%) | 10 (50%) | 4 (21.05%) | 5 (26.32%) | – | – | |
2 (10%) | 4 (20%) | – | – | – | – | |
– | 1 (5%) | – | – | – | – |
As far as it concerns psychological distress, Cronbach’s alpha was applied to the SCL-90-R symptom dimensions and to the global distress indexes. Good internal consistency was reported for obsessive-compulsive (0.82), interpersonal sensitivity (0.72), social phobia (0.72), paranoid ideation (0.78), psychoticism (0.80), and for the total of the items (0.90). Reliability was acceptable for somatization (0.67) and depression (0.70), poor for anxiety (0.56) and unacceptable for hostility (≤0.50), which was excluded from subsequent analysis.
Average scores, standard deviations, and distribution of the mothers in the SCL-90-R.
Norm | Clinical | ||
---|---|---|---|
746.40 (5.49) | 18 (90) | 2 (10) | |
49.40 (9.52) | 15 (75) | 5 (25) | |
47.65 (6.95) | 15 (75) | 5 (25) | |
46.80 (5.19) | 18 (90) | 2 (10) | |
45.85 (4.83) | 18 (90) | 2 (10) | |
45.95 (3.94) | 19 (95) | 1 (5) | |
46.50 (8.15) | 17 (85) | 3 (15) | |
47.85 (7.57) | 17 (85) | 3 (15) | |
46.30 (5.60) | 18 (90) | 2 (10) | |
45.48 (6.57) | 19 (95) | 1 (5) | |
48.90 (6.69) | 15 (75) | 5 (25) |
Regarding couple adjustment, Cronbach’s alpha coefficient reported good internal consistency for the DAS total score (0.77) and for the subscales concerning Dyadic Consensus (0.69) and Dyadic Satisfaction (0.68). Reliability was unacceptable as far as it concerns Dyadic Cohesion and Affective Expression (α ≤ 0.50). Thus, these subscales were excluded from subsequent analysis. Normative cut-offs were computed from average scores and standard deviations reported in the article of the Italian validation of the DAS (
Average scores, standard deviations, and distribution of the mothers’ scores in the DAS.
<Norm | ≥Norm | ||
---|---|---|---|
54.39 (4.79) | – | 18 (100) | |
42.44 (4.33) | – | 18 (100) | |
123.72 (9.47) | – | 18 (100) |
The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient reported very good reliability for all the scales of the MSPSS, considering both the different subscales and the whole scale (0.76 ≤ α ≤ 0.97).
Average scores, standard deviations, and distribution of the mothers in the MSPSS.
≥Norm | <Norm | ||
---|---|---|---|
5.43 (0.65) | 19 (95) | 1 (5) | |
5.16 (1.18) | 16 (80) | 4 (20) | |
4.46 (1.06) | 17 (85) | 3 (15) | |
5.03 (0.81) | 18 (90) | 2 (10) |
As it is possible to see from the data above, from a descriptive point of view at the beginning of the massage course all the mothers seemed to be able to rely on satisfactory couple adjustment, showing scores within the normative range in all the DAS scales. A similar consideration could be made for social support; although few subjects reported MSPSS scores below average, the majority of the participants seemed to experience a sufficient amount of support provided by family, friends and a significant other. As far as it concerns psychological wellbeing, although the majority of the subjects reported scores that fell into the SCL-90-R normative range, some mothers reported the presence of significant psychological distress as well. It is noteworthy, however, that rarely this perception reached the clinical cut-off. Finally, considering mother–child interactions, it is possible to see how, during T1, most of the mother–child dyads fell in the complicated zone of EA, indicating the presence of an emotional connection but the existence of difficulties as well. The complicated zone, and the zones below were progressively less represented during the ongoing of massage classes. During the follow up (T4), all the dyads that completed the program fell in the emotional available zone.
To assess change vs. stability of EA during the infant-massage course the Friedman test was applied. This non-parametric statistical test can be considered a valid alternative of the parametric repeated measures ANOVA. The results reported a statistically significant increase in maternal sensitivity (
To test for associations between quality of mother–child interactions and maternal psychological distress, the Spearman’s Rho coefficient was applied to the SCL-90-R reliable scores (obsessive-compulsive, interpersonal sensitivity, social phobia, paranoid ideation, psychoticism, somatization, depression, anxiety, GSI, PST, and PSDI) and to the scores obtained through the EAS during T1, T3, and T4.
Associations between EA and psychological distress.
T1 | -0.113 | 0.168 | 0.014 | -0.198 | -0.367 | -0.197 | -0.129 | -0.134 | -0.037 | -0.029 | -0.111 | |
T3 | -0.123 | 0.227 | -0.313 | 0.102 | -0.201 | -0.378 | -0.108 | -0.452 | 0.026 | -0.118 | 0.127 | |
T4 | -0.342 | -0.628ˆ* | -0.401 | -0.516 | -0.468 | 0.139 | -0.314 | -0.301 | -0.627ˆ* | -0.514 | -0.628ˆ* | |
T1 | 0.058 | -0.038 | 0.051 | -0.029 | -0.320 | -0.098 | 0.020 | -0.311 | -0.061 | -0.071 | -0.050 | |
T3 | -0.048 | 0.173 | -0.369 | -0.053 | -0.323 | -0.101 | -0.111 | -0.621ˆ** | -0.091 | -0.233 | 0.079 | |
T4 | 0.007 | -0.247 | 0.116 | -0.211 | -0.264 | 0.215 | 0.034 | -0.029 | -0.210 | -0.034 | -0.420 | |
T1 | 0.108 | 0.157 | 0.073 | 0.123 | -0.249 | -0.020 | -0.038 | -0.451ˆ* | 0.102 | -0.030 | 0.179 | |
T3 | -0.222 | 0.053 | -0.371 | -0.240 | -0.506ˆ* | 0.088 | -0.237 | -0.591ˆ** | -0.252 | -0.358 | -0.084 | |
T4 | 0.027 | -0.435 | -0.234 | -0.283 | -0.130 | 0.463 | -0.314 | -0.175 | -0.348 | -0.247 | -0.418 | |
T1 | 0.060 | 0.035 | 0.177 | -0.200 | -0.201 | -0.329 | 0.150 | -0.189 | -0.031 | 0.035 | -0.129 | |
T3 | -0.253 | 0.079 | -0.413 | -0.086 | -0.337 | -0.088 | -0.280 | -0.582ˆ** | -0.220 | -0.392 | 0.047 | |
T4 | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | |
T1 | 0.023 | 0.195 | 0.330 | 0.282 | 0.207 | -0.061 | 0.237 | 0.050 | 0.248 | 0.302 | 0.086 | |
T3 | -0.260 | 0.088 | -0.352 | -0.088 | -0.495ˆ* | -0.281 | -0.201 | -0.599ˆ** | -0.152 | -0.282 | 0.020 | |
T4 | 0.386 | -0.407 | -0.237 | -0.215 | -0.022 | 0.418 | -0.451 | -0.350 | -0.257 | -0.236 | -0.257 | |
T1 | 0.226 | -0.051 | 0.145 | 0.139 | -0.018 | 0.071 | 0.088 | 0.053 | 0.081 | 0.039 | 0.112 | |
T3 | -0.127 | 0.242 | 0.030 | 0.089 | -0.298 | -0.510 | 0.047 | -0.367 | 0.051 | -0.015 | 0.016 | |
T4 | 0.222 | -0.168 | -0.046 | 0.029 | -0.184 | 0.330 | -0.334 | -0.250 | -0.125 | -0.125 | -0.086 |
To test for associations between quality of mother–child interactions and couple adjustment, the Spearman’s Rho coefficient was applied to the DAS reliable scores (Dyadic Consensus – DC, Dyadic Satisfaction – DS, and DAS total scores) and to the scores obtained through the EAS during T1, T3, and T4.
Associations between EA, couple adjustment, and perceived social support.
T1 | -0.578ˆ* | -0.306 | -0.558ˆ* | 0.027 | 0.207 | -0.016 | 0.094 | |
T3 | -0.443 | -0.034 | -0.387 | -0.031 | 0.081 | 0.229 | 0.187 | |
T4 | -0.543 | -0.259 | -0.402 | 0.059 | 0.321 | -0.433 | 0.029 | |
T1 | -0.355 | -0.477ˆ* | -0.449 | -0.177 | 0.098 | -0.183 | -0.116 | |
T3 | -0.539ˆ* | -0.171 | -0.511 | 0.039 | -0.045 | -0.036 | 0.009 | |
T4 | -0.112 | -0.202 | -0.027 | 0.087 | 0.446 | -0.532 | 0.044 | |
T1 | -0.270 | -0.306 | -0.250 | 0.122 | 0.364 | 0.079 | 0.238 | |
T3 | -0.668ˆ** | -0.351 | -0.630 | 0.113 | -0.009 | -0.103 | -0.015 | |
T4 | -0.255 | -0.353 | -0.182 | 0.231 | 0.178 | -0.473 | 0.025 | |
T1 | -0.265 | -0.359 | -0.379 | -0.473ˆ* | -0.214 | -0.382 | -0.460ˆ* | |
T3 | -0.291 | -0.086 | -0.313 | 0.185 | 0.056 | 0.112 | 0.102 | |
T4 | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | |
T1 | -0.211 | -0.275 | -0.255 | -0.309 | -0.054 | 0.092 | -0.042 | |
T3 | -0.378 | -0.099 | -0.374 | 0.088 | 0.049 | 0.086 | 0.049 | |
T4 | 0.107 | -0.302 | 0.022 | 0.110 | -0.066 | -0.325 | -0.097 | |
T1 | -0.354 | -0.636ˆ** | -0.421 | -0.110 | 0.033 | 0.088 | 0.029 | |
T2 | -0.338 | -0.153 | -0.324 | -0.001 | 0.049 | 0.288 | 0.092 | |
T3 | 0.039 | -0.374 | -0.062 | 0.459 | 0.464 | -0.189 | 0.258 |
To test for associations between quality of mother–child interactions and the dimensions of perceived social support the Spearman’s Rho coefficient was applied to the scores of the MSPSS and to the EAS during T1, T3, and T4. As it is possible to observe in
Spearman’s Rho was applied to test for associations between perceived social support, couple adjustment, and psychological distress. No statistically significant associations were found between couple adjustment and perceived social support, neither between couple adjustment and psychological distress. With respect to psychological distress and perceived social support, statistically significant correlations were found between paranoid ideation and the support received by a significant other (
In order to assess whether there were associations between EA improvements among the three different timepoints and the other measures, the variance score of EA between timepoints (i.e., from T1 to T3, from T3 to T4, and from T1 to T4) was calculated subtracting the scores at later stages with scores at previous stages. Subsequently, Spearman’s Rho correlations were run between the variance scores of EA and the other measures (i.e., psychological distress, perceived dyadic satisfaction, and perceived social support). Some associations were detected for SCL-90-R, namely a negative relationship between higher psychological distress at three scales of the SCL-90-R (assessed at time 1) and lower variance scores between T1 and T4. More specifically, a negative relationship was detected between Interpersonal Sensitivity and variance score of Child Responsiveness (
The first aim of the present study was to investigate EA during infant massage classes and to observe if an improvement in mother–child interactions occurred. Up to date, only few studies applied the EAS in the context of infant massage (
As far as it concerns maternal psychological wellbeing, as expected, a higher degree of adult psychopathology resulted associated with less optimal mother–child interactions, supporting the hypothesis that experiencing some kind of psychological distress might affect different domains of life, including the one of everyday interactions with one’s own child (
Our hypotheses were not confirmed as far as it concerns the associations between mother–child interactions, couple adjustment and social support. The lack of associations seems to reflect in part the non-univocal results reported by the literature. In fact, although several studies reported the presence of associations between marital quality and parenting (
The present study shows a series of limitations that might offer useful suggestions for future research. The first limit regards the sample; the small amount of the participants and the absence of a non-treated control group, in fact, prevent us to generalize the obtained results. A larger sample would allow to adopt more sound statistical analysis, while the presence of a control group would allow to compare the development of mother–child interactions between dyads that undergo infant massage courses and dyads without intervention, thus leading to a better explanation of the effective influence of infant massage upon the establishment of early adult–child interactions.
Another limit regards the absence of a baseline assessment of mother–child interactions. Videotaping the dyads during massage lessons might have influenced the nature of mother–child interactions both in positive or in negative. On one side, in fact, the massage context might have acted as a buffering factor, preventing the mothers from enacting dysfunctional behaviors that otherwise could have been adopted; on the other hand, instead, finding themselves in a new situation and being asked to do something new (massaging their babies while being videotaped) might have made interactions more challenging for these women. In this sense, including a baseline assessment in future would favor a better control of the different intervening variables. Moreover, it should be taken into account that our study only included mothers and did not involve fathers. Expanding the research design in this direction in future would lead to two major consequences: first of all, the possibility to support and sustain also paternal functioning during the postpartum period; secondly the opportunity to increase and to deepen the comprehension of family processes.
Finally, some considerations should be dedicated to the clinical implications of our study. These preliminary findings, in fact, seem to suggest the usefulness of infant massage for the strengthening and the enhancement of early healthy adult–child interactions. This cost-saving technique could provide a simple but effective way to favor the construction of early affective bonds; in this way, it could accompany a process existing per se and sustain the dyad during expected developmental challenges, whether necessary. Especially in a delicate interval such as the post-partum period this practice could become extremely important, since it could help the dyad to face the need of mutual adjustment, facilitating regulatory processes and the establishment of sleep-wake cycles. Moreover, a “guided” emotionally intense approach toward the infant could reassure the mothers, who often perceive the newborns as fragile and are afraid to touch them, making them more confident when handling their babies. From this perspective, infant massage constitutes a precious resource in terms of primary prevention, i.e., in terms of those interventions aimed at sustaining and enhancing the existing resources within the family system, since it can be offered as an enriching support also in the absence of adult psychopathology. A replication of these results in larger samples would thus encourage the diffusion of this non-invasive technique in terms both of relational support and enhancement of parenting abilities.
The study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Code of Ethics approved by the General Assembly of the Italian Association of Psychology held on March 27, 2015. Written consent was obtained from the participants.
AS prepared the study design and supervised the research team; GB carried out the massage courses and recruited the sample. SF collected data and prepared data set. AP and MP wrote the introduction section of the manuscript, performed statistical analyses, and prepared tables and figures. AS, MP, and AP wrote the discussions section of the manuscript. All authors reviewed the manuscript.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Code of Ethics approved by the General Assembly of the Italian Association of Psychology held on March 27, 2015 with written informed consent from all subjects.