Front. Psychiatry Frontiers in Psychiatry Front. Psychiatry 1664-0640 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00457 Psychiatry Review Empathic Accuracy in Clinical Populations RumYonat PerryAnat * Psychology Department, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel

Edited by: Frieder Michel Paulus, University of Lübeck, Germany

Reviewed by: William Ickes, University of Texas at Arlington, United States; Katherine Rice Warnell, Texas State University, United States

*Correspondence: Anat Perry, anat.perry@mail.huji.ac.il

This article was submitted to Social Cognition, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychiatry

03 06 2020 2020 11 457 06 11 2019 05 05 2020 Copyright © 2020 Rum and Perry 2020 Rum and Perry

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Empathy, broadly defined as the ability to understand the other and to share others’ emotions, motivates prosocial behavior and underlies successful interpersonal relations. Dysfunctions in this ability may cause fundamental difficulties in social communication. Empathy has been measured in various ways, from self-report questionnaires to laboratory objective performance tests. Empathic accuracy (EA), i.e., the ability to accurately empathize, is measured using more complex and ecological paradigms, such as asking participants to infer filmed interactions, or having people narrate personal emotional stories then assessing the correspondence between the perceiver and the target of empathy as the criteria for empathic ability. This measure is particularly useful in the study of clinical populations, where deconstructing the multifaceted concept of empathy may contribute to a more complete understanding of specific clinical profiles. This paper presents a scoping review of the literature on EA in clinical populations, and on EA and clinical traits and states in nonclinical or high-risk populations. Following an exhaustive literature search, 34 studies were found eligible to be included in this review. The largest category was studies focused on EA in people with schizophrenia (31%; 11 papers), followed by studies focused on EA in autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and autistic traits in a nonclinical population (22%; 8 papers). Studies were also found on EA and depression tendencies, psychopathy, social anxiety, behavior disorders, and personality disorders, and a few other clinical conditions. The included studies varied on research aims, designs, sample sizes, and male:female ratios. The overall synthesized results suggest that EA is reduced in schizophrenia and ASD. In other clinical populations, the number of studies was very limited. We urge researchers to further examine EA in these less-studied populations. The review reveals a general underrepresentation of female participants in studies on EA in clinical populations. We suggest that future research address understudied clinical populations, such as those diagnosed with psychopathy. Subject, target, and situational variables should also be considered, with special attention to gender differences (and similarities), the association between EA abilities and adaptive functioning, and the study of individuals with clinical conditions as targets, not just observers, in EA tasks.

empathic accuracy autism schizophrenia psychopathy depression anxiety behavior disorders personality disorders Azrieli Foundation10.13039/501100005155

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      Every well-adjusted social interaction—for example, between parents and children, between peers or between partners—requires recognition, understanding and sometimes sharing each other’s thoughts, feelings, and emotions. Applying these complex skills, while maintaining a self/other distinction, is termed empathy (13). An evolutionary perspective suggests that the basic need to care for offspring explains why human beings developed empathy (4). Empathy motivates prosocial behavior and interpersonal relations (2). On the other hand, dysfunction or lack of empathic abilities may cause not only misunderstandings and unpleasantness but also fundamental difficulties living in society. The multifaceted concept of empathy can be divided into cognitive empathy (or mentalizing)—the recognition and understanding of others’ mental states—and emotional empathy (or experience sharing)—in which the affective experience is similar to that of the other, or there is an emotional response to the mental state of the other [(57); for a review see: (2, 8)].

      As a sophisticated yet fundamental ability that plays a central role in human relationships, empathy has been extensively researched for decades, and it has been examined specifically in clinical populations in which social dysfunctions are key. For example, autism spectrum disorder (ASD), psychopathy and schizophrenia are clinical conditions that according to several theories are associated with pronounced empathic dysfunction (915). In both ASD and psychopathy, a social deficit is not only a characteristic, it is a diagnostic criterion (16, 17). While some have suggested that both cognitive and emotional domains of empathy are impaired in schizophrenia (18), others claim that schizophrenia and psychopathy are characterized by deficits in emotional empathy but not cognitive empathy (11, 19, 20). Aberrant empathic functioning, specifically impairments in cognitive empathy, was also found in borderline personality disorder [BPD; (21)] and bipolar disorder (22), two conditions associated with interpersonal deficits. However, other findings support a hypothesis according to which individuals with BPD are uncommonly sensitive or “over empathic” to the internal experience of others (2325). Findings from clinical populations are of great value for understanding the multifaceted concept of empathy on the one hand, and specific clinical profiles on the other hand, but these are not always consistent. One possible explanation for the inconsistencies may be the varied operationalizations of empathy in research.

      Researchers in the fields of developmental, social, cognitive, educational, and clinical psychology, as well as cognitive neuroscience, use different methods and instruments to measure empathy in the general population, and in clinical or high-risk populations. In early childhood, empathy is often measured through observations, as a behavioral response to a simulation of others’ distress (2628) or by caregivers’ reports [e.g., (29)]. In older children, empathy is measured using different tasks, including the evoked emotional response in the child (30, 31). In schoolchildren, adolescents, and adults, empathy can be measured using either self-report questionnaires [e.g., IRI, (32); EQ, (33); CEAQ, (34)] or objective performance tests, which compare participants’ output to predefined “correct” responses. These kinds of tasks include emotion recognition tasks in still pictures, and reading a vignette describing a mental state or a social situation. Theory of Mind (ToM) and traditional false beliefs and “Faux-Pas” tasks are also related to some extent to the cognitive component of empathy (3540). Generally, some tasks or questionnaires primarily measure the cognitive empathic component, while others capture more of the emotional output. Objective performance tests offer some integration between the individual’s perspective and the observed behavioral output, but such laboratory tasks usually fail to capture the dynamic nature and complexities involved in social communication, including rapid and nuanced changes in facial expression, intonation and other pragmatic characteristics of the speech, posture and gestures of the target social partner (41). Other limitations in some of the methods mentioned above include the fact that reading and comprehension abilities and executive functions (e.g., in questionnaires, vignettes) may present a potential confound, and the fact that some measures refer to a very narrow aspect of empathy (e.g., emotion recognition from facial expressions). In studying empathy in clinical populations, these limitations need to be considered.

      Empathic accuracy (EA) tasks have tried to offer a more ecological setting to measure empathic abilities. EA is the ability to accurately judge the cognitive and affective mental states of others (42, 43). Accordingly, in the original lab procedure developed by Ickes and his colleagues, a dyad is videotaped while interacting. Then each member of the dyad views the videotape separately and reports his or her own thoughts and feelings during the interaction, as well as inferences regarding the partner’s thoughts and feelings during the interaction. EA is measured by the similarity between the explicit reported mental states of the target and those reported by the perceiver (42, 43). In the current review we refer to this prototype paradigm (and later adaptations and variations of it) as a dyadic interaction paradigm.

      A more recent EA paradigm developed by Zaki and colleagues is based on the perceiver’s interpretation of a target’s videotaped autobiographical emotional story as the stimulus (instead of a dyadic interaction), and the correspondence between the perceiver’s and the target’s ratings of valence (i.e., how positive or negative the target felt while telling the story) instead of the exact mental content. In this paradigm, both the target and the perceiver use a rating dial to continuously rate the valence of the videotaped story, and the perceiver’s EA score results from the correlation between the two continuous ratings (41, 44, 45). This method comes from an earlier attempt by Levenson and Ruef (46) to create a measure of behavioral empathy that relies on rating dials to provide continuous responses to a given videotaped stimulus. Here, we refer to this method as an emotional story inferring paradigm.

      Another EA paradigm, which has been utilized mostly in research on romantic partners, uses experience-sampling diaries [e.g., (47, 48)]. In this approach, participants provide daily reports of their own mental states and their inferences regarding their partner’s perceived mental states over a period of time. Then reports of each participant on his/her partner’s (the target’s) thoughts and feelings are compared to the target’s own reports to arrive at an EA score. In this review, this is referred to as a daily diary paradigm.

      All three prototypes of EA paradigms yielded various studies, and some of them applied specific variations and adaptations to the original developed tests. Common to all is the reliance on the concordance between the perceiver’s (the subject of the EA measure) view of the target and the target’s (the object of the EA measure) own report on their internal states to generate the EA measure. As such, EA measures provide more ecologically valid data on interpersonal perception in comparison to other experimental techniques. Moreover, an fMRI study by Zaki and colleagues suggests that both cognitive and emotional mechanisms contribute to the ability of the perceiver to accurately match her state with the emotions or thoughts experienced by a social target (49). Thus, measuring EA seems to capture a more nuanced measure of empathy and reflect its complexity.

      The main objective of this review is to provide an overview of the existing literature on EA in clinical populations or high-risk subclinical populations, and on clinical states and traits measured in nonclinical samples. To this end, we aim to (1) conduct a systematic search of the published peer-reviewed papers on EA in clinical populations; (2) map the characteristics and range of findings and conclusions in the identified papers; (3) examine reported challenges and limitations of measuring EA in clinical populations; and (4) propose recommendations for future research directions. Within the scope of this review are studies measuring EA conducted on clinical populations, as well as studies focusing on clinical traits in a high-risk or nonclinical population. We considered studies measuring valence (negative-positive) or content (thoughts, feelings), and measuring EA as a primary or secondary aim of the study (for example, studies measuring EA in a clinical sample as part of a battery of tests assessing social cognition). We also considered a variety of paradigms used to assess EA, including the dyadic interaction paradigm, the emotional story inferring paradigm and the daily diary paradigm. Common to all studies was the aim to assess the perceivers’ ability to accurately understand and report on the targets’ affective or mental state when the criteria are the target’s own representations of his or her mental state.

      Methods

      The methodology was based on the framework outlined by Arksey and O’Malley’s (50) review and recommendations made by Levac et al. (51). It consisted of five key phases: (1) identifying the research question; (2) identifying potentially relevant studies; (3) selection of studies; (4) charting the data; and (5) organizing, summarizing, and reporting the findings. The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (52) were used in the current review as a guide, where applicable.

      Research Question

      This review was guided by the following two questions: (1) What are the characteristics of studies measuring EA in clinical populations? and (2) What are the main findings and conclusions in the literature regarding EA in clinical populations? For the purposes of this review, all papers that used the term “empathic accuracy” and referred to a concordance or correlation between two partners (i.e., a target and a perceiver) were included.

      Data Sources and Search Strategy

      The initial search was implemented in July 2019, using PsycNET and PubMed. The search query included the term “empathic accuracy” AND (permutations of) the terms: “autism”; “psychopathy”; “schizophrenia”; “depression”; “dyslexia”; “attention deficit”; “anxiety”; “OCD”; “behavior disorders”; “personality disorders”; “mood disorders”; “affective disorders”; “neurodegenerative disease”; “mental disability”; “learning disability”; “neurodevelopmental disorder”; “clinical population”; “mental disorders.” The reference lists of all potentially relevant papers were screened in a two-phase process: (a) title and abstract screening; and (b) full-text screening. Empathy measures were examined in the selected studies with respect to the extent to which they tapped into EA. A “snowball” technique was also utilized in which citations within papers were searched to look for potentially relevant studies. A follow-up search was conducted on September 24, 2019, to identify any additional relevant papers published after the initial search, resulting in the final list of papers for the review (see Table 1).

      Summary of studies’ characteristics.

      Study Objective/Research Question Design and Participants EA paradigm Main conclusions regarding EA
      Schizophrenia Spectrum and Psychotic Disorders
      van Donkersgoed et al. (53) To assess the moderating role of the target’s gender and expressivity and the valence of the story on EA performance; the correlation between EA and other commonly used empathy measures. Schizophrenia group (n=92, 67 males)Nonclinical control group (n=42, 32 males) matched for age, gender, and education Emotional story Schizophrenia group performed worse than controls in EA.Individuals with schizophrenia benefit less from the emotional expressivity of targets. No correlations were found between EA and questionnaire scores, suggesting a distinction between self-report empathy and actual empathy performance.
      de Jong et al. (54) To investigate which measures of social cognition and metacognition are related to violent history in patients with psychotic disorder; which domains of metacognition were indicative of a violent history in psychosis. Violent psychotic disorder in care at a forensic clinic for a violent crime (n=23)Clinical group 2: nonviolent psychotic disorder (n=27, all males)Nonclinical control group (n=33, all males) Emotional story EA differentiated between the violent and nonviolent psychotic patients, while scores on social cognition (such as ToM) and a metacognition scale did not.EA may offer an important contribution to statistical models of violence risk in psychotic disorder.
      Harenski et al. (55) To explore the hypothesis that lower EA and smaller brain volumes in regions implicated in social cognition are related to past suicide attempts in offenders with a psychotic disorder. Criminals with a psychotic disorder and a history of suicide attempts (n=18, all males)Criminals with a psychotic disorder and no past suicide attempts (n=25, all males)Nonclinical group: criminals with no history of a psychotic disorder (n=59, all males)Nonclinical control group (n=26; all males) Emotional story Criminal offenders with psychotic disorders and suicide attempts had lower EA and smaller temporal pole volumes compared to the other groups.EA and temporal pole volumes were significantly associated with past suicide attempts independent of other risk factors.
      Horan et al. (56) To evaluate correlations of the Questionnaire of Cognitive and Affective Empathy (QCAE) in schizophrenia with EA (and other empathy measures). Schizophrenia group (n=145, 108 males)Nonclinical control group (n=45, 32 males) Emotional story No significant association was found between the QCAE and EA performance in either group, indicating that self-reported beliefs about empathic characteristics are not necessarily correlated with an actual understanding of others’ affective states.
      Davis et al. (57) To assess whether oxytocin (OT) enhances the effectiveness of a social cognitive training.The final four sessions of training focused on improving EA. Individuals with schizophrenia (n=27, all males)were randomly assigned to an OT condition (n=13) or to a placebo condition (n=14).(Double-blind drug administration with before and after treatment comparison) Emotional story Administration of OT before a psychosocial intervention targeting social cognition improved EA and not other measures of social cognition, in individuals with schizophrenia.
      Ripoll et al. (58) To test schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) participants and healthy controls on the EA paradigm and the Reading of the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET). SPD group (n=19, 13 males, 6 females)Nonclinical control group (n=19, 6 males, 13 females) Emotional story SPD individuals demonstrated lower EA than controls during negative-valence videos, associated with lower social support.RMET did not differ between groups, suggesting that EA paradigms may be more effective at capturing interpersonal dysfunction than static image tasks. Schizotypal severity, trait empathy and cognitive dysfunction did not account for the empathic dysfunction.
      Olbert et al. (59) To examine the relationship between EA (and three other social cognitive paradigms adapted from social neuroscience) and functionally meaningful outcomes in schizophrenia (incremental, external validity). Within-subject design on participants with schizophrenia (n=173, 124 males) Emotional story The EA paradigm was found to have the broadest external validity, and it is the most recommended measure from the four paradigms that were evaluated. EA had a significant association with functional outcome measures: Higher EA was associated with greater nonsocial cognitive ability, functional capacity, social skills and community functioning.
      Kern et al. (60) To evaluate psychometric properties of EA (and three other social cognitive paradigms adapted from social neuroscience) to inform possible use in clinical trials that assess treatment-related changes in social cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia group (n=173, 124 males)Nonclinical control group (n=88, 57 males)within subject (test-retest) in the schizophrenia group Emotional story The EA task had the best psychometric properties of the four paradigms checked:The largest between-group difference was seen on EA; of all measures, only a long version of the EA task met acceptable test-retest reliability standards; EA task was the strongest measure in regard to practice effects.
      Harvey et al. (61) To examine the neural correlates of EA and targets’ expressivity in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia group (n=15, 13 males)Nonclinical control group (n=15, 13 males) Emotional story Schizophrenia patients demonstrated impaired EA, failed to benefit from targets’ emotional expressivity (wherein controls did benefit from targets’ expressivity), and demonstrated reduced neural sensitivity to targets’ affective cues.
      Lee et al. (62) To determine the relative extent of impairment in social and nonsocial cognitive domains in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder patients comparedwith healthy controls. Schizophrenia group (n=38, 21 males)Bipolar disorder group (68, 38 males)Nonclinical control group (n=36, 20 males) Emotional story Schizophrenia patients performed significantly worse on EA than bipolar patients and controls, who did not differ from each other.see findings regarding bipolar patients under Bipolar Disorder
      Lee et al. (63) To examine whether schizophrenia patients showed lower EA compared with controls; whether emotional expressivity of a target moderated group differences; whether EA is associated with self-reported trait empathy or clinical characteristics in the schizophrenia sample. Schizophrenia group (n=30, 25 males)Nonclinical control group (n=22, 17 males) Emotional story Schizophrenia patients were impaired in EA relative to controls.Both groups showed better accuracy for positive- vs. negative-valence videos.Both groups showed greater EA for highly expressive targets, but this effect was significantly smaller in schizophrenia patients.EA was not related to the participants’ self-reports or clinical symptoms.
      ASD and Autistic Traits
      Adler et al. (64) To compare levels of empathic embarrassment accuracy among individuals with ASD with those of matched controls. ASD group (n=17, 16 males, high functioning/Asperger’s syndrome)Nonclinical control group matched for age and IQ (n=24, 21 males) A paradigm designed to measure empathic embarrassment accuracy1 The ASD group displayed less empathic embarrassment accuracy compared with the control group.Higher AQ scores predicted low EA in the ASD group (a marginal correlation).
      aan het Rot and Hogenelst (65) To investigate the influence of autistic traits and trait affective empathy on EA. Nonclinical sample (n=100, 50 male and 50 female) Emotional story Perceivers with more autistic traits demonstrated worse EA, particularly when their trait affective empathy was relatively low.Higher perceiver EA was predicted by a higher perceiver affective empathy and the target being female (rather than male), but there was no significant interaction between these two predictors.
      Demurie et al. (66) To investigate and compare the mind-reading abilities of adolescents with ASD, adolescents with ADHD and typically developed (TD) adolescents. ASD group (n=13, 12 males)ADHD group (n=13, 12 males)Nonclinical control group (n=18, 14 males)adolescents Dyadic interactionIn each dyad one of two targets was TD, and the other was ASD or ADHD Adolescents with ASD demonstrated impairment on both EA and a static task.see findings regarding ADHD under ADHD
      Bartz et al. (67) To test whether normal variance in social proficiency moderates the effects of oxytocin (OT) on social-cognitive performance. Nonclinical sample (n=27, all males). Participants were randomly assigned to either an OT condition or a placebo condition, followed by an EA task. Participants returned 3 to 5 weeks later, received the alternate compound, and completed the EA task again. Emotional story Oxytocin selectively improved EA for people with higher (but not lower) autistic traits.
      Ponnet et al. (68) To investigate EA of participants with ASD asked to infer the mental states of targets in a highly structured conversation vs. a less structured/more naturalistic conversation. ASD group (n=22, all males)Nonclinical control group (n=22, all males) matched for chronological age and IQ Dyadic interactionOne interaction was more structured than the other. Differences between ASD and control groups in EA were more pronounced when participants had to infer the thoughts and feelings of other persons in a less structured conversation.
      Ponnet et al. (69) To measure the social functioning of adults with pervasive developmental disorder (PDD) during a conversation with a TD stranger and to explore whether EA of both groups was affected by behavioral characteristics and by the content of the interaction. Part 1: Eleven dyads, each composed of a partner with ASD (n=11, 9 males; PDD) and a TD partner (n=11, 9 males), interacted in a lab task, then performed the EA task on each other within each dyad.PDD participants with the highest scores in the EA task of Roeyers et al. (70) were invited to participate in this study. TD participants were matched based on sex, age, education and main interests.Part 2: TD participants (n=13, 8 males), with the filmed interactions from part 1 as the stimuli for EA measure. Dyadic interactionASD participants take part in the interaction No significant difference was found between controls and PDD participants in EA.No significant associations were found between EA and IQ scores, age or the time needed to complete the task.EA scores of the 11 participants with PDD correlated significantly with their EA scores on the previous study (Roeyers et al., (70); on a video of structured interaction).No significant difference was found among participants in part 2 in EA towards TD or PDD individuals as targets.Being in the interaction yields higher EA scores than just perceiving the interaction: participants in part 1 (PDD and TD) scored higher in EA than participants in part 2 (TD), who inferred EA from an interaction in which they did not previously take part.
      Ponnet et al. (71) To compare individuals with Asperger syndrome and controls’ performance in two static mind-reading tasks and the EA task. ASD group (n=19, 14 males; Asperger’s syndrome)Nonclinical control group (n=19, 14 males) Dyadic interaction The EA task indicated significant between-group differences, whereas no such differences were found on the static mind-reading tasks.EA in both groups depended on the focus of the target’s thoughts and feelings.Participants with ASD needed more time than the controls to complete the EA task.
      Roeyers et al. (70) To compare individuals with PDD with controls on twopreviously used static empathy tests and onan EA task. ASD group (n=24, 22 males; PDD/high-functioning)Nonclinical control group (n=24, 22 males) matched for sex, education, profession or interests Dyadic interaction Participants with PDD demonstrated worse EA in a video presenting a less structured conversation between two stranger targets, whereas no between-group differences were found in a video presenting a more structured conversation.Participants with PDD did not use more time than controls to complete the EA task.EA measure was proven to be a valid alternative to the previously used static tests.
      Depression Measured in a Nonclinical or High-Risk Population
      aan het Rot et al. (72) To examine the impact of light therapy on mood and on cognitive empathy in premenstrual women with complaints indicating a premenstrual disorder. A nonclinical sample (n=48, all females) divided into two treatment groups (light therapy/sham session; participant-blind between-groups design) Emotional story There were no significant effects of light therapy on EA.Participants obtained higher EA scores when watching positive clips compared to negative clips.
      Hogenelst et al. (73) To investigate the effect of acute tryptophan depletion (ATD), which reduces brain serotonin, on social functioning, EA, and oxytocin levels. High risk for MDD group (n=20, 10 males)Nonclinical matched control groupA randomized, double-blind, crossover design (2 treatment conditions) with between-group comparison Emotional story EA remains unaffected by acute reductions in brain serotonin, even though brain oxytocin levels may be reduced.
      Gadassi et al. (74) To examine associations between EA and depression as a possible mechanism underlying gender differences in the association between interpersonal difficulties and depression in an intimate relationship. Nonclinical sample of romantic couples (51 dyads; measurement of subclinical depression traits in couples) Dyadic interactionand Diary Depressive symptoms were associated with lower EA among females and may have a stronger impact on interpersonal perception in intimate relationships among females than among males.When a female is depressed, both her own and her partner’s EA levels are lower. When males are depressed, neither their own nor their partner’s levels of EA are lower.Depressive symptoms predicted lower EA regarding negative moods and feelings, but not regarding positive ones.
      Papp et al. (75) To examine affectivity in marital interaction: to test partners’ EA and assumed similarity in marital conflict interactions and whether they are moderated by spouses’ levels of depressive symptoms; to examine whether spouses’ ratings of their partner’s specific emotions depend on how they felt themselves in the same conflict interaction. Nonclinical sample of romantic couples (267 dyads; measurement of subclinical depression traits in couples) Dyadic interactionThe interaction was focused on a topic of conflict Females with higher levels of depressive symptoms demonstrated higher EA (and lower assumed similarity) compared to females with lower levels of symptoms.In rating negative emotions, spousal depressive symptoms weakened females’ abilities to rate their partners’ emotions; in rating positivity, higher females’ depressive symptoms strengthened their ratings of their partner’s emotions.Females’ depressive symptoms were associated with lower EA ratings by their partners (for anger, but not for sadness); males’ depressive symptoms were associated with lower EA in rating their partner’s anger.Males’ EA in rating their partner’s sadness was higher when their partner had a higher level of depressive symptoms.Partners of spouses with elevated depressive symptoms demonstrated particular difficulty in assessing partner anger in marital conflict.
      Thomas et al. (76) To examine the correlates of online EA in a sample of married couples in the context of problem-solving discussions, considering depression, relationship length and educational attainment. Nonclinical sample of romantic couples (74 dyads; measurement of subclinical depression traits in married couples) Dyadic interactionThe interaction was focused on a topic of conflict EA was not significantly correlated with depression in either males or females.
      SAD and Trait/State Social Anxiety
      Morrison et al. (77) To compare cognitive empathy and affective empathy in individuals with SAD to that of matched controls;to assess empathy with an adapted version of the EA task, with an additional behavioral index of affective empathy—by examining the degree of congruency between the target’s self-rating of emotion and the participant’s self-rating of his/her own emotions. SAD group (n=32, 18 males)Nonclinical matched control group (n=32, 18 males) Emotional story No between-group differences were found in EA, indicating intact cognitive empathy in SAD.For positively valenced (but not for negatively valenced) clips, individuals with SAD exhibited significantly lower empathic congruence (affective empathy) than controls, indicating that affective empathy may be impaired in SAD.
      Auyeung and Alden (78) To examine whether individual differences in social anxiety moderate EA. A nonclinical sample (n=121, 95 females) measured to assess social interaction anxiety in to conditions: experimental condition (a manipulation designed to increase state anxiety) and a control condition Emotional storySpecifically, targets narrated experiences when they felt: (1) socially excluded (2) socially included Social anxiety was associated with greater EA for others’ social pain, but only when participants experienced social threat: Individuals with lower levels of social anxiety were less accurate in judging others’ negative emotions following a social threat.
      Simpson et al. (79) To test how people with more anxious-ambivalent attachment orientations react when their relationships are threatened by alternative dating partners. Nonclinical sample of romantic couples (82 dyads; measurement of subclinical anxiety traits) Dyadic interaction Highly anxious-ambivalent individuals demonstrated higher EA (than those rated lower on anxiety) in a relationship-threatening situation (watching their partners rating opposite-sex optional dating partners), greater distress, and less confidence in their partners and relationships.The more anxious-ambivalent females reported a slight decrease in the perceived closeness of their relationships. More anxious-ambivalent males’ relationships were more likely to have ended by follow-up.
      Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD)
      Miano et al. (80) To investigate whether BPD patients show motivated inaccuracy by measuring their EA during a relationship-threatening conversation with their own romantic partner. Dyadic analysis of BPD couples (30 couples; the female partner diagnosed with BPD) vs. a nonclinical control group of couples (34 couples) Dyadic interactionSpecifically focused on: (1) a personally threatening topic (2) a relationship-threatening topic Reduced EA when facing a relationship-threatening situation was found in couples in the nonclinical control group, while females with BPD did not show this pattern of motivated inaccuracy and instead increased their EA, a finding that supports the concept of borderline empathy.Male partners of BPD females did not have a different EA pattern than control males.Neutral and personally threatening contexts did not significantly affect EA between BPD and control females.
      Flury et al. (81) To explore the phenomenon of borderline empathy (elevated empathy among individuals with BPD) with the use of EA. A nonclinical sample (n=76, 46 females), composed of high vs. low risk for BPD, assigned to dyads each composed of a high-risk for BPD partner and low-risk for BPD partner. Dyadic interactionIn each dyad one “borderline” (high-risk) and one “nonborderline” (low-risk) The empathic advantage displayed by high BPD individuals may not reflect greater ability, but result from the comparison to the ratings of their partner, who had difficulty inferring emotions of the BPD partners.
      Conduct Disorder and Callous-Unemotional Traits
      Martin-Key et al. (82) To assess EA, emotion recognition and affective empathy in male adolescents with Conduct Disorder (CD) and higher versus lower levels of callous-unemotional (CU) traits. Clinical group: CD (n=37, all males)Nonclinical control group (n=40, all males)adolescents Emotional story Adolescents with CD did not differ in EA from TD adolescents but displayed significant impairments in emotion recognition and affective empathy (measured by asking participants to report whether they experienced the same emotion as the target).No difference in EA was found between high and low CU traits subgroups.
      De Ridder et al. (83) To assess everyday EA in institutionalized adolescents with high and low CU traits, and how EA is related to adolescents’ own behavior, and own affective and relational experience. A sample of institutionalized adolescents (n=71, 45 males) divided into high CU traits vs. low CU traits *adolescents A procedure similar to the diary paradigm2 High CU adolescents unexpectedly did not differ from low CU adolescents in EA (specifically inferring anger and distress in staff members) and notably overestimated the general intensity of both anger and distress, and in particular, inferred more anger when they (the adolescent themselves) were misbehaving.
      Psychopathy
      Brook and Kosson (84) To examine relationships between psychopathy and cognitive empathy.To design an improved EA task, with multiple targets, and a standardized forced-choice response format. A sample of incarcerated offenders (n=103, all males) Emotional story Inverse association between psychopathy and EA was found, as well as robust group differences between psychopathic and nonpsychopathic inmates, findings that corroborate the deficient empathy hypothesis.
      Bipolar Disorder
      Lee et al. (62) See the same study in the category: Schizophrenia spectrum and Psychotic disorders. Bipolar groups did not differ from the control group on EA but outperformed the schizophrenia group.Bipolar patients performed significantly better on social relative to nonsocial cognitive domains, whereas schizophrenia patients showed the opposite pattern.
      Risk for Hypomania
      Devlin et al. (85) To utilize a naturalistic, dynamic social stimulus (EA paradigm) in order to investigate the relationship between hypomania risk and empathy. Nonclinical sample (n=121, 69 females), divided into high vs. low risk for hypomania Emotional story Risk for hypomania was associated with elevated EA of increases in positive emotion for targets describing positive events; however, it was also associated with overestimating global positive emotion for targets describing negative events.
      ADHD
      Demurie et al. (66) See the same study in the category: ASD and autistic traits in a subclinical population. ADHD did not significantly differ in EA from either the control group or ASD group; thus, it was determined to be an intermediate group between the clinical and nonclinical groups. Thoughts and feelings of target persons with ADHD seemed to be less easy to read than the thoughts and feelings of TD targets.
      Neurodegenerative Disease
      Brown et al. (86) To investigate whether deficits in EA in patients with neurodegenerative disease are associated with greater depression in their caregivers. Two independent cross-sectional samples (n=172, n=63) of patients with a variety of neurodegenerative diseases and their caregivers (usually spouses) vs. a nonclinical control group of healthy couples. Dyadic interaction Lower EA in patients was associated with higher depression in their caregivers.In study 1, this relationship was found using EA (after controlling for patient cognitive and functional symptoms) and was not found when using other more traditional tasks. In study 2, the relationship was found after accounting for caregiver characteristics that have previously been associated with caregiver depression.

      1Participants watch films in which protagonists performed embarrassing actions and are asked to rate how embarrassed they feel (empathic embarrassment-EE) and how embarrassed they think the protagonist feels. The participant’s ratings are compared with the protagonist’s own ratings to produce a measure of empathic embarrassment accuracy.

      2Adolescents reported the intensity of anger and distress they perceived in staff members; staff members reported their own levels of anger and distress after each period of at least 1 hour spent with the adolescent.

      Eligibility Criteria

      Peer-reviewed papers written in English were eligible for inclusion if they explicitly aimed to measure “empathic accuracy” (EA) in a clinical population. Studies published in any year were considered. Papers that referred to empathy in clinical populations without measuring EA and review papers were excluded from the analysis, but their reference list was reviewed to identify additional relevant papers. Papers aiming to measure the relation between EA and clinical traits in nonclinical populations were also included in the current review. Exclusion criteria included: papers in languages other than English; nonpeer-reviewed papers (such as theses or dissertations); and papers aiming to measure EA in the general/nonclinical/typically developing population.

      Data Characterization and Analysis

      All papers deemed relevant after the title and abstract screening were procured for subsequent review of the full text. Studies were excluded at this phase if they were found not to meet the eligibility criteria. The following characteristics of each full-text article were then extracted: objectives; participants (clinical population, N, age, gender); definition of EA; EA paradigm used; main findings and main conclusions regarding EA. All references, abstracts and data characteristics were imported into Microsoft Excel. Descriptive statistics were calculated to summarize data characteristics when applicable. The main findings and conclusions of all reviewed papers were discussed in light of the known data characteristics, limitations, and strengths of the included studies.

      Results Search and Selection of Papers

      The original search conducted in July 2019 yielded 17 potentially relevant citations for EA and “autism” (using “ASD” as a search word instead of “autism” yielded no additional papers). For EA and “schizophrenia,” 24 potentially relevant citations were found (using “schizophrenic” as a search word instead of schizophrenia yielded no additional papers). For EA and “psychopathy,” four potentially relevant citations were found. For EA and “depression,” 26 potentially relevant citations were found (using “depressive” as a search word instead of “depression” yielded one additional potentially relevant paper). For EA and “attention deficit,” five potentially relevant citations were found (using “ADHD” as a search word instead of “attention deficit” yielded no additional papers). For EA and “anxiety,” 22 potentially relevant citations were found. For EA and “behavior disorders,” 16 potentially relevant citations were found (using “conduct disorder” or “disruptive behavior disorders” as a search word instead of “behavior disorders” yielded one additional potentially relevant paper). For EA and “personality disorders,” five potentially relevant citations were found (using “borderline disorder” as a search word instead of “personality disorders” yielded no additional papers). For EA and “neurodegenerative,” one potentially relevant citation was found (using “degenerative” or “Alzheimer’s disease” or “Alzheimer” or “dementia” as a search word instead of “neurodegenerative” yielded no additional papers). One potentially relevant paper was found for EA and “learning disabilities” (using “learning disability” as a search word instead of “learning disabilities” yielded no additional papers). No potentially relevant papers were found for EA and “dyslexia” or “dyslexic”, for EA and “OCD,” for EA and “mood disorders,” or for EA and “epilepsy.” No potentially relevant papers were found for EA and “mental disabilities” or “mental disability,” or for EA and “clinical populations.” For EA and “mental disorders,” five potentially relevant citations were found in the search.

      Thus, the initial list consisted of 128 references. After the first phase of relevance screening, 70 citations were considered to potentially meet the eligibility criteria based on title and abstract, and the full-text articles were reviewed. In the second phase of reviewing full texts, 34 papers were excluded. Among the excluded papers, two mentioned measuring EA, but no results regarding EA were reported, and two papers were not available. During the full-text screening, the “snowball” search technique resulted in two additional eligible papers. The updated search in September 2019 produced two more potentially relevant citations, one of which was found to be eligible and was included. During the full-text screening phase, two studies were excluded, as the current inclusion criteria referred to EA as a measure comparing the subject’s perception to the target’s own perceptions: one study (87) that used the term “empathic accuracy” to refer to the “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test [RMET; (36)], and one (88) that referred to EA as the correlation between a perceiver’s rating and a panel of judges’ ratings of the emotions of the same target (and not the concordance between the perceiver’s and the target’s rating). Thus, the final list of papers selected for inclusion in the current review consists of 34 peer-reviewed papers. Figure 1 presents the search flow diagram.

      Studies search flow diagram.

      Characteristics of Included Papers EA Definition

      An explicit definition or description of what the authors mean by “empathic accuracy” was reported in all but four of the papers. Most definitions/paradigms centered on the ability to accurately judge the valence and/or content of emotions or thoughts experienced by another person, mostly citing Ickes et al. (43), Ickes (42), and Zaki et al. (44). However, there was some divergence in how authors characterized EA. Some studies referred to EA as a measure of cognitive empathy (53, 65, 73, 77, 83, 84). In contrast, some other authors mentioned that because EA is the ability to correctly infer the emotional state of a target, it has a relatively affective character (54, 75). Harvey et al. (61) claim that EA is not solely a measure of mental-state attribution (associated with cognitive empathy) or of experience sharing (associated with affective empathy), but that it is the product of these two processes [this definition was also used by Martin-Key et al. (82)].

      Clinical Populations

      Included papers referred to EA in the following categories of clinical populations and traits in high-risk, subclinical or nonclinical populations: schizophrenia spectrum and psychotic disorders (31%; 11 papers); ASD and autistic traits in a nonclinical population (22%; eight papers); depression measured in a nonclinical or high-risk population (14%; five papers); social anxiety disorder (SAD), social anxiety, and trait/state anxiety in a nonclinical population (8%; three papers); BPD (5%; two papers); conduct disorder and callous-unemotional traits (5%; two papers); and one paper in each of the following categories: psychopathy; hypomania; attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD); bipolar disorder and neurodegenerative disease. Two papers were assigned to two categories, as they compared two clinical samples in the study [an ASD group was compared to an ADHD group in Demurie et al. (66); a bipolar disorder group was compared to a schizophrenia group in Lee et al. (62)]. In most of the papers, learning about the nature of EA in a clinical population was the primary aim; thus, a between-group design was assigned, comparing the clinical group to a matched control group. In some of the studies, this was a secondary aim, as when EA was part of a battery of tests to assess social cognition (62), or when the primary aim was evaluating interventions (57, 67, 72, 73) or evaluating the psychometric properties of an EA paradigm (59, 60).

      Clinical Sample Sizes

      Of the final list of eligible papers, 23 (67%) reported studies done directly on participants from a clinical population (i.e., participants have a diagnosis of one of the above-mentioned conditions), while the rest referred to clinical traits in healthy, nonclinical or high-risk populations. Of the 23 studies that included participants with a clinical diagnosis, the largest sample size was n = 173 [(59, 60)]; schizophrenia spectrum and psychotic disorders category), and the smallest sample size was n = 11 [(69)]; ASD category), with 48% (11/23) of the studies based on n < 30. In the studies with nonclinical or high-risk populations, samples were usually larger, with all studies but one ((67); n = 27) based on n > 30, and 6 of them with sample size of n > 100 (see Table 1).

      Male : Female Ratio

      In six studies where EA was measured on clinical samples, there was no representation of females (0 female participants; see Table 1). In one study (BPD category), there was no male representation. In the rest of the reviewed studies on clinical populations, the male to female ratio was in favor of male participants and ranged from 1.2:1 to 16:1. Aggregating the number of all participants diagnosed with a disorder from one of the above categories across studies reveals a male-to-female ratio of 2.9:1, with 887 male and 306 female participants. In the studies on nonclinical or high-risk populations, in one study [(67); ASD and autistic traits category] all participants were males; in one study [(72)]; Depression measured in a nonclinical or high-risk population category) all participant were females; in six studies the number of male and female participants was even; and in the remaining two studies more females than males participated. When aggregating numbers of all participants in the 10 nonclinical studies, the male:female ratio was 1.16:1. The male:female ratio also differed among categories of clinical condition. As can be seen in Figure 2, while studies on psychopathy, ADHD, conduct disorder and callous-unemotional traits, schizophrenia spectrum and psychotic disorders, and ASD and autistic traits relied more on male participants, studies in the categories of depression (depressive traits in a nonclinical or high-risk population), SAD, social anxiety and trait/state anxiety, BPD and risk for Hypomania relied more on female participants.

      Distribution of studies by clinical population category. Percentages refer to the percentage of papers on that population out of all papers in the current review, and colors refer to the male:female ratio. In blue, categories with overall more males than females among all participants (in all studies together). In yellow, categories with overall more females than males among all participants. The darker the color, the more pronounced the underrepresentation for females, with darker blue = 0 females, lighter blue = a ratio of more than 3:1, lightest blue = a ratio of more than 2:1, and gray = a ratio of less than 2:1. Specific male:female ratios for each category are reported under “Specific Results per Clinical Population”.

      EA Paradigm Used

      Most of the studies (22 papers) were based on the emotional story inferring paradigm (or similar); about a third (10 papers) were based on the dyadic interaction paradigm; one study relied on the diary procedure; and one study utilized a similar procedure to that of the daily diary, though slightly modified.

      Limitations mentioned by researchers were mainly a small sample size, underrepresentation of females in the sample, comorbidity with other conditions, use of medications, and a lack of ethnic diversity among targets.

      Publishing Year

      Although we did not limit the search years, all included papers were published between 1997 and September 2019, with 82% (28/34) published after 2010.

      Specific Results per Clinical Population

      Table 1 presents the characteristics, main objective, findings, and conclusion of each of the studies included in this review.

      In the following section, we review the main findings from the papers included, organized by clinical populations or clinical traits. Categories of clinical populations/traits are presented according to the number of relevant studies found, from the categories with a larger number of studies to those with the fewest. Two exceptions are categories that include papers referring to two different clinical conditions in the same comparative study. In these cases, the category of the clinical condition with the smaller number of studies will follow the category with the larger number. These cases will be explicitly noted when presenting the new category.

      Schizophrenia Spectrum and Psychotic Disorders

      Thirty-one percent (eleven papers) of the studies included in the current search focused on EA in the context of schizophrenia and psychotic disorders. In five studies (53, 56, 58, 61, 63), a group of participants with schizophrenia was compared with a nonclinical control group. In Lee et al.’s study (62), a schizophrenia group was compared to both a nonclinical control group and a group of participants diagnosed with bipolar disorder (see below). In de Jong et al.’s study (54), a group of violent participants with a psychotic disorder (a primary diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder) was compared to a nonviolent psychotic disorder group and to a nonclinical control group. One study (60), which aimed to evaluate psychometric properties of EA and other paradigms to inform possible use in clinical trials, used both a between-group design (schizophrenia group in comparison to nonclinical control group) and a within-subject (test-retest) design in the schizophrenia group. Olbert et al. (59) applied a within-subject design in order to examine the relationship between EA (and other social-cognitive paradigms adapted from social neuroscience) and functionally meaningful outcomes in schizophrenia. Davis et al. (57) assessed whether oxytocin would enhance the effectiveness of a psychosocial intervention—applied both before and after treatment with a double-blind drug administration design. Harenski et al. (55) compared criminal offenders with psychotic disorders to criminal offenders with no history of psychotic disorders and to a nonclinical nonoffenders control group. Within the first group, psychotic offenders with a history of suicide attempts were compared to psychotic offenders without such a history. In all studies but one, the EA tests were based on Zaki et al. (44), where EA assessment is based on the valence rating of a target’s emotional states while s/he tells an autobiographical emotional story. In Harenski et al. (55), participants watched video clips in which people described autobiographical events, and participants indicated the content of emotions the people most likely experienced during the event, and additionally ranked the emotions (84).

      The number of participants with schizophrenia ranged from 15 (61) to 173 [(59, 60)]—two studies based on the same sample), with the control groups usually similar to or smaller than the schizophrenia group. In three studies (54, 55, 57) all participants were males. In all the other studies there were more male participants than female participants, with a male:female ratio ranging from 1.2:1 (62) to 6.5:1 (61).

      A synthesis of findings and conclusions from all studies together indicates reduced overall EA abilities in people with schizophrenia spectrum disorders in comparison to nonclinical controls. This is a robust finding that holds cross-culturally (53, 54, 56, 6163). The EA impairment in schizophrenia is not easily explained by attention or motor deficits (61), and no correlation was found between EA performance and schizophrenia symptoms (63). Also, no significant effects of gender or gender by diagnosis were found on EA (58). However, the valence of the content that targets convey and participants need to infer may be an important variable that moderates results: In one study (58) it was found that the EA impairment in schizophrenia is specific to negative content, while in positive content participants with schizophrenia scored similarly to controls. Interestingly, this difficulty understanding others’ negative affect was associated with lower indices of social support. Harvey et al. (61) found that participants with schizophrenia were more impaired than controls in EA in the context of negative videos compared with positive videos. In Lee et al. (63), both groups showed better accuracy for positive valence; however, participants in the schizophrenia group demonstrated impairment in the positive as well as in the negative valence stimuli.

      Findings also indicate that EA is a sensitive measure that captured a group difference between individuals with schizophrenia and controls, even where other tasks (e.g. RMET) did not (58). EA differentiated not only between people with schizophrenia and healthy individuals, but also within a group of patients with a psychotic disorder, EA differentiated between those with and without a violent history (54), and between psychotic offenders with and without past suicide attempts, and nonpsychotic offenders and nonclinical controls (55), where lower EA was associated with a greater likelihood of a past suicide attempt, beyond other risk factors such as depression and substance use. EA was found to be a sensitive differentiating measure in such cases even when other measures (such as ToM, “understanding the other’s mind”) were not (54). In a research project evaluating the psychometric properties of four different social-cognitive paradigms adapted from social neuroscience (basic biological motion, emotion in biological motion, self-referential memory and EA) that were administered to participants with schizophrenia, EA had the broadest external validity (59). The other examined paradigms all had limitations for use in clinical trials, at least without further adaptation (60).

      Similar to findings from the general population, associations between EA measures and self-report empathy measures from questionnaires in participants with schizophrenia were found to be weak (53, 56, 63). This might suggest a discrepancy between subjectively experienced empathy and actual empathy performance in a dynamic, interpersonal task. Another possible explanation is that the EA measure captures a certain aspect of empathy, while questionnaires [in these cases: IRI; (32); QCAE, (89)] capture a different aspect. This lack of a significant correlation between EA tasks and empathy self-report questionnaires, alongside the fact that other tasks designed to measure empathy did not always differentiate between participants with schizophrenia and controls (54, 58) may also indicate that at least some aspects of empathy are intact in schizophrenia. It seems that people with schizophrenia do not respond to others’ greater emotional expressivity as much as healthy individuals do (53, 61, 63). Level of expressivity of the targets in all these studies was based on their score on the Berkeley Expressivity Questionnaire [BEQ; (90)], a self-report questionnaire that assesses tendencies to experience and express strong emotions in general (example items: “Whenever I feel positive emotions, people can easily see exactly what I am feeling”; “I sometimes cry during sad movies”; “I’ve learned it is better to suppress my anger than to show it”; “I am an emotionally expressive person”. Van Donkersgoed et al. (53) found that with less expressive targets, participants with schizophrenia and controls had similarly low EA scores, but with more expressive targets, the control group performed better on EA than the patients with schizophrenia. On the neural level, it was found that expressivity elicited activity in specific regions more powerfully in controls than in participants with schizophrenia (61). Lee et al. (63) found that although both schizophrenia and control groups showed greater EA for more expressive targets, this effect was significantly smaller in schizophrenia participants. What seems to improve EA performance in schizophrenia is oxytocin: Participants assigned to oxytocin demonstrated significantly greater improvements than placebo on the measure of EA [but not on other social-cognitive measures; (57)].

      Lastly, two studies utilized fMRI (61) and structural MRI (55) scans. Supporting the idea that both mental-state attribution and experience-sharing processes contribute to EA, Harvey et al. (55) found that in healthy controls, EA was associated with increased activity in brain regions typically linked to cognitive effort (i.e., lateral PFC), visual attention (i.e., parietal and occipital cortices), socioemotional processes, including mental-state attribution (i.e., mPFC, precuneus, posterior cingulate), experience sharing (i.e., inferior frontal, inferior parietal), and social context processing (i.e., parahippocampal gyrus). However, in participants with schizophrenia, the pattern of accuracy-related brain activity was relatively sparse (61). Harenski et al. (55) found that offenders with psychotic disorders and suicide attempts demonstrated lower EA and had smaller temporal pole volumes relative to controls, to nonpsychotic offenders and to psychotic offenders without past suicide attempts (this association was significant independent of other risk variables).

      Bipolar Disorder

      One of the studies described in the schizophrenia category (62) was a comparative study aiming to determine the relative extent of impairment in social (and nonsocial) cognitive domains in individuals with bipolar disorder compared with schizophrenia patients. EA was thus a part of a battery measuring social cognition within these two groups and in a nonclinical control group. Participants in the bipolar group did not differ from comparison participants on EA, nor in each of the other social-cognitive tasks, whereas schizophrenia patients showed impaired social-cognitive performance compared with both bipolar patients and the control group. Bipolar disorder was found in this study to be associated with less impairment on social relative to nonsocial-cognitive performance, whereas schizophrenia was associated with more impairment on social relative to nonsocial-cognitive performance.

      ASD and Autistic Traits

      Twenty-two percent (eight papers) of the studies that were found in the current search focused on EA in the context of ASD or autistic traits. In five of them (64, 6871), a group of participants with ASD was compared with a nonclinical control group. In one study (66), a group of participants with ASD was compared to both a nonclinical control group and a group of participants diagnosed with ADHD. In two studies (65, 67), autistic traits were assessed in nonclinical samples. In five studies (66, 6871), participants were mostly high-functioning individuals (sometimes defined as Asperger’s syndrome, or PDD). EA measurement was based on a dyadic interaction paradigm, with perceivers asked to infer the content of the targets’ mental states (43, 91). In Demurie et al. (66), one of the targets featured in each filmed interaction was diagnosed with ADHD while the other was a typically developed participant. In Ponnet et al. (69), all members of dyads who participated as targets also participated later as participants for measuring EA; each dyad included a participant with ASD and a typically developed participant. Two studies (65, 67) relied on Zaki et al. (44), where EA assessment is based on the valence rating of a target’s emotional states while s/he tells an autobiographical emotional story. One study focused on empathic embarrassment accuracy among individuals with ASD in comparison to the control group, using a similar paradigm (64). One study (67) aimed to test whether variance in social proficiency moderates the effects of oxytocin on social-cognitive performance, applying a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled design: Participants completed a questionnaire measuring autistic traits [AQ; (92)] and then self-administered intranasal oxytocin or a matching placebo before completing an EA task. EA scores were then compared between the experimental and the control (placebo) group.

      The number of participants with ASD ranged from 11 participants (69) to 24 participants (70), with the control groups usually the same size, or slightly larger. Females were generally underrepresented in all six studies with participants with ASD: The number of female participants with ASD ranged from 0 (68) to 5 (71), resulting in an overall male-to-female ratio of around 10:1. In the two studies that were based on a nonclinical sample (65, 67), sample sizes were larger (n = 100; n = 27). In aan het Rot and Hogenelst’s study (65), the male-to-female ratio was 1:1, and in the Bartz et al. study (67), all participants were males.

      The synthesis of the findings and conclusions from all the studies together shows that individuals with ASD exhibit a deficit in EA abilities (64, 66, 6871). More pronounced autistic traits in typically developed individuals were also associated with poorer EA abilities (65, 67). However, this may be true only for individuals who have more autistic traits as well as less trait affective empathy (5). Additionally, this association was found to be moderated by the hormone oxytocin: Bartz et al. (67) showed that oxytocin selectively improved EA for people with more pronounced autistic traits. In this study, participants with less pronounced autistic traits performed better on the EA task in the placebo condition and maintained this performance level in the oxytocin condition, whereas participants with more pronounced autistic traits performed worse in the placebo condition but significantly better in the oxytocin condition, such that in the oxytocin condition, the performance of participants with more and less pronounced autistic traits did not differ. Roeyers et al. (70) found that participants with ASD did not use more time than the control group to complete the EA assessment, while in Ponnet et al. (71), participants with ASD needed more time than the controls to carry out the EA task.

      Importantly, the measurement of EA in a naturalistic, ecological paradigm captured the difference between the ASD group and a control group when static mind-reading tasks did not (71). Ponnet et al. (68) found that when participants were presented with two filmed interactions, one more structured than the other, participants with ASD demonstrated better EA abilities on the more structured video than on the less structured one, while no such difference was found in the control group. Thus, the findings from both studies (68, 71) emphasize the role of structure in bringing out empathic abilities of individuals with ASD, indicating that they perform better in more structured settings, tasks or situations. Ponnet et al. (69) found that when participants with ASD who had to infer the thoughts and feelings of a target in a videotaped interaction also took part in these prerecorded interactions, they did not differ from a nonclinical control group in their EA scores. The researchers concluded that being in the interaction yields higher EA scores than perceiving a social interaction without participating in it (69). In terms of people with ASD, this may be a result of the opportunity to review a social situation that was previously experienced, hence reflecting practice and learning. It is also possible that the interactive experience itself enhanced EA due to attention, motivation or even bio-behavioral factors, such as oxytocinergic influences. Roeyers et al. (70) noted that although impairments in EA were observed among people with ASD, the underlying mechanisms accounting for this remain unexplained. They added that as the advanced EA measure proved to be a valid alternative for the static tests, they believe that future work incorporating the EA paradigm could expand the research on deficient mind reading in ASD.

      Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder

      In one of the studies reviewed above (66), adolescents with ASD (n = 13) were compared to adolescents with ADHD (n = 13) and to a nonclinical control group (n = 18) on EA performance, in a dyadic interaction paradigm. In each dyad, one of two interacting targets was a typically developing adolescent, and the other was diagnosed with ADHD. Thus, participants with ADHD were examined in this study not only as subjects but also as the targets for EA (participants with ASD were examined only as subjects/perceivers). The study results demonstrate the impairment in EA abilities of adolescents with ASD. Participants with ADHD performed as an intermediate category between the ASD and the control group in EA abilities: Their scores did not differ significantly from those of the control group nor from those of the ASD individuals. As targets, participants with ADHD were less accurately understood than the typically developing participants, and their thoughts and feelings seemed to be less easy to read.

      Depression Measured in a Nonclinical or High-Risk Population

      In 14% of the papers (five papers), the relationship between EA and depressive traits or states in a nonclinical or high-risk population was examined. Two studies (72, 73) used an EA test based on the valence rating of targets narrating autobiographical stories (44, 65). One of them (73) aimed to examine the effects of reduced brain serotonin on EA, oxytocin and mood in never-depressed individuals with low vs. high risk for major depressive disorder. This study utilized a double-blind cross-over design, with an order of treatment randomized by gender and group (high vs. low risk, 10 males and 10 females in each group, and two treatment conditions). The other study (72) aimed to examine the impact of light therapy on mood and on cognitive empathy in premenstrual women with symptoms indicating a premenstrual disorder (PMS). The sample was characterized by mild depression [assessed using the Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptoms; (93)]. This study utilized a participant-blind between-groups (two treatment groups) design and included 48 females. In both studies, participants’ EA performance was not affected by intervention. aan het Rot et al. (72) found that the therapy improved mood (only in women not using hormonal contraceptives), but found no differential effects of light therapy on EA, even when potential moderators such as valence (positive or negative) of the stimuli, the target’s emotional expressivity, PMS severity, participants’ depression and contraceptive use were taken into account. Similarly, Hogenelst et al. (73) found that the procedure used to model reduced serotonin (acute tryptophan depletion; ATD) did not significantly alter EA in the high-risk group, nor in the control group. In both studies, participants obtained higher EA scores when watching positive stimuli compared to negative stimuli, but without moderating the overall results. To sum, in both studies EA and depression were measured in the context of an intervention aimed to target depression (light therapy, ATD), and in both EA was not affected by the intervention.

      The other three studies all used samples of romantic couples [51 couples in Gadassi et al. (74)]; 267 couples in Papp et al. (75); 74 couples in Thomas et al. (76)). These studies measured both EA and depressive symptoms and utilized a dyadic interaction paradigm (Actor–Partner Interdependence Model). All three studies measured EA using a lab procedure where couples are videotaped while interacting (discussing a given topic or an issue of conflict); then they separately review the recording, write the content of their own experienced mental states during the interaction and infer their partner’s mental states. One study (74) additionally utilized the daily diary procedure, measuring both content and valence of the partner’s thoughts and feelings. Thomas et al. (76) examined the predictors of EA and assumed similarity (judgments of how closely linked partner emotions are) in a sample of married couples, in the context of problem-solving discussions, considering depression. They found no association between depression and EA. It is interesting to note, however, that lower levels of depression tended to produce higher levels of assumed similarity. Based on the procedure applied by Thomas et al. (74), Papp et al. (75) tested partners’ EA and assumed similarity in marital conflict interactions, and whether they are moderated by spouses’ levels of depressive symptoms. They found that higher levels of depressive symptoms were associated with reduced EA for negative emotions (among both males and females) and, surprisingly, with increased EA for positive emotions among females. Gadassi et al. (74) aimed to examine gender differences in the association between depressive symptoms and interpersonal perception. In the lab measures, they found that females’ (but not males’) higher levels of depressive symptoms were associated with lower EA. In the daily diary procedure, females’ depressive symptoms were specifically associated with lower levels of EA for negative (but not for positive) feelings, and with lower levels of their partner’s EA for the females’ negative feelings. Males’ depressive symptoms were again unrelated to levels of EA. They concluded that when a woman is depressed, first her own EA is lowered, and second, her partner’s EA when trying to infer her emotional state is also lowered. This pattern was valence-specific and gender-specific. Taken together, findings from these three studies present some inconsistencies regarding the association between EA and depressive symptoms and indicate that the mechanism underlying this potential association may be modified both by valence and by gender.

      SAD and Trait/State Social Anxiety

      Three studies examined associations between EA and social anxiety. One study (77) compared 32 participants with a SAD to a nonclinical matched control group. These researchers aimed to compare cognitive empathy and affective empathy in individuals with SAD to that of nonanxious controls. They used an adapted version of an emotional story inferring paradigm (44), adding to the procedure a measure of “empathic congruence.” According to Morrison et al. (77), while perceiver inference of the target’s emotional valence provides a measure of cognitive empathy, a measure of emotional empathy can be gained by examining the degree of congruence between the target’s self-rating of emotion and the participant’s self-rating of emotion. They found that individuals with SAD did not differ from controls in continuously rating how negative or positive they thought the targets felt (i.e., in EA, cognitive empathy). However, they did differ from controls in their empathic congruence (rating how they themselves felt): For positively valenced (but not for negatively valenced) clips, individuals with SAD exhibited significantly lower empathic congruence.

      In the remaining two studies (78, 79), social anxiety was measured in a nonclinical population. Auyeung and Alden (78) examined whether individual differences in social anxiety moderated EA. They randomly assigned 121 participants to an experimental condition designed to increase state anxiety via social threat or to a control condition; they then asked the participants to observe videos of target individuals discussing either a socially painful or a nonpainful event. Both targets and participants rated the negative emotions that the targets were feeling while discussing the event. The researchers found that social anxiety was associated with higher EA for others’ negative social emotions (social pain), but only when participants experienced social threat (under the social-threat condition). Simpson et al. (79) tested how people with more anxious-ambivalent attachment orientations [a measure of anxiety in the context of relationships; (94, 95)] react when potential alternative dating partners threaten their relationship. Eighty-two dating couples inferred their partner’s mental states from a videotaped interaction in which they each rated pictures of opposite-sex individuals for attractiveness. EA was operationalized in this study as the degree to which one participant’s inference about the content of each of his or her partner’s thoughts and feelings matched the partner’s actual thoughts and feelings (by independent coders). Highly anxious participants demonstrated higher EA in this relationship-threatening situation. These more anxious participants also showed greater relational instability when they more accurately read their partners’ thoughts and feelings, and their relationships were more likely to have ended 4 months later, measured in a follow-up screening. According to Simpson et al. (79), their findings demonstrate that in relationship-threatening situations, anxious-ambivalent individuals appear to be particularly vulnerable to the negative implications of their partner’s thoughts and feelings.

      Borderline Personality Disorder

      Two studies (80, 81) measured EA in the context of BPD. Both utilized a dyadic interaction paradigm. Miano et al. (80) measured EA in 30 romantic couples, with a female partner diagnosed with BPD, in comparison to a control nonclinical group of 37 couples. They aimed to investigate whether females with BPD show inaccuracy during a relationship-threatening conversation with their partner (the authors note that motivated inaccuracy is a protective mechanism for couples in healthy relationships during some relationship-threatening situations). Their findings indicate that when facing a relationship-threatening situation, couples in the control group demonstrated inaccuracy, i.e., reduced EA. In contrast, females with BPD tended to increase their EA compared with females in the control group, in a relationship-threatening context. Male partners of BPD females did not differ from males in the control group in the EA pattern.

      Flury et al. (81) aimed to explore the “borderline empathy phenomenon,” i.e., the claim, suggested by clinical psychologists, that patients with BPD are unusually accurate at “reading” other people (2325, 96, 97). The authors used an assessment of EA. They recruited 30 males and 46 females from a larger sample of participants who completed the Borderline Syndrome Index [BSI; (98)], and scored in the upper and lower quartiles, to create a group of individuals at high risk for BPD and a low-risk group. Participants were then assigned to same-sex dyads, each composed of one “borderline” (high-risk) and one “nonborderline” (low-risk) participant, and EA was measured within this dyadic interaction paradigm. Researchers found that the high-risk BPD dyad members displayed better EA than the low-risk BPD dyad members, which seemed to support the borderline empathy phenomenon. However, further analyses [with the Actor–Partner Interdependence Model, APIM; (99101)] revealed that between those at high risk versus those at low risk, these effects were not a consequence of greater abilities on the part of the BPD participants, but poorer abilities on the part of their partners, meaning that for high-BPD members, EA was harder to predict and more difficult to infer by their partners. The authors emphasize the importance of considering the fact that “high BPD individuals do not have greater empathic ability; they are simply harder to ‘read’.” [(81), p.326]

      Conduct Disorder and Callous-Unemotional Traits

      Two studies evaluated EA abilities in the context of conduct disorder in adolescents. In one study, Martin-Key et al. (82) compared male adolescents with Conduct Disorder (CD) and higher versus lower levels of callous-unemotional (CU) traits (n = 37) and a nonclinical control group of male adolescents (n = 40), using an emotional story inferring paradigm. This study employed a modified version of the EA task developed by Zaki et al. (49) in order to draw three measures: the participants’ ability to track changes in the intensity of the target’s emotion, i.e., EA; their ability to recognize the specific emotion displayed by the target after watching the full video clip, i.e., emotion recognition; and the participants’ reported experience of the same emotion as the target, i.e., emotional empathy. They found that relative to controls, participants with CD showed deficits in emotion recognition and emotional empathy (deficits were particularly evident for sadness, fear and disgust), but not in EA. Comparison between the subgroups of high versus low CU traits did not yield any significant differences in EA either.

      In the second study, De Ridder et al. (83) assessed EA of male institutionalized adolescents toward staff members, over eight days, in 71 participants with high and low CU traits. Their findings indicate that adolescents with high CU traits perform in the normal range for anger recognition, and they are as accurate as low CU in inferring distress among staff members. The adolescents with high CU traits overestimated the intensity of both anger and distress, in particular during their own misbehavior. The authors suggest that this may reflect overrelying on cognitive empathy ability, instead of their impaired emotional empathy abilities. Thus, the two studies, conducted using two different methods, in two different settings, imply that in the context of conduct disorder, EA as a measure of cognitive EA is intact (when the participant is asked to track the intensity of the target’s emotion). However, accuracy in emotion recognition is impaired, as is the ability to accurately share the affective experience of the target, as a measure of emotional empathy.

      Psychopathy

      Surprisingly, only one paper (84) was found in the search to study EA in psychopathy. This study aimed to examine the relationship between psychopathy and cognitive empathy, with a procedure similar to that of the emotional story inferring, using standardized forced-choice response format for both the videotaped targets and the perceivers (and not a continuous rating scale). Findings revealed an inverse association between psychopathy and EA scores, as well as robust group differences between psychopathic and nonpsychopathic male inmates.

      Risk for Hypomania

      One study (85) measured the association between EA and high risk for hypomania. The study included 121 participants (57% females) and utilized an emotional story inferring paradigm. The researchers examined how the risk for hypomania contributes to the emotional experiences upon encountering another person’s emotions and EA of that target’s emotions. The risk for hypomania [assessed by The Hypomanic Personality Scale; (102)] was found to be associated with heightened moment-by-moment detection of positive emotions for targets describing positive events, and with overestimating global positive emotion for targets describing negative events. Hypomania risk was also significantly associated with a higher positive emotional experience after viewing a high-intensity negative emotional story video, but not after viewing a low-intensity negative video or high/low-intensity positive video.

      Neurodegenerative Disease

      Lastly, one paper (86) investigated the association between EA in patients with neurodegenerative disease and their caregivers’ depressive symptoms. Across two independent studies (n = 172, n = 63), lower EA in neurodegenerative patients was found to be associated with greater depressive symptoms in their caregivers (who were mainly partners). This association was found when accuracy was measured via caregiver report or with a dynamic tracking task. Patients’ ability to recognize specific emotions portrayed in photographs or films was not found to be associated with caregivers’ depressive symptoms.

      Discussion

      The current review aimed to scope the existing literature on EA in clinical populations. An exhaustive systematic search yielded 34 peer-reviewed papers aiming to measure EA in a clinical population or to assess links between EA and clinical trait or state in a nonclinical or a high-risk population. Overall, the review indicates a growing interest in the EA measure, a dynamic ecological measure that enables greater sensitivity in detecting between-group differences, and more nuanced characterization of empathic functioning.

      An Overview and a Different View of the Main Findings

      While ASD and psychopathy are considered to be the two main conditions traditionally associated with empathic dysfunction (11), surprisingly, only one study was found to focus on EA in psychopathy, and two more on conduct disorder in adolescents. The category with the most studies found is schizophrenia (with 31% of the studies). Some of the studies assessed EA in people with a diagnosed clinical condition, while others assessed clinical states or traits in nonclinical or high-risk populations. EA was measured in individuals from clinical groups for various purposes: looking for between-group differences, evaluating interventions and assessing measurements or tools. Accordingly, various designs were used: clinical condition group versus nonclinical control group, randomized or test-retest designs and dyadic designs. Studies also varied in sample sizes and male:female ratios, which will be further discussed.

      Almost all studies utilized the emotional story inferring paradigm (or similar), or a dyadic interaction paradigm. These are difficult to compare as they were never used in the same study and were usually used in different contexts or with different populations. For example, the category with the largest number of studies, schizophrenia and psychotic disorders, consists only of studies based on the emotional story inferring paradigm, while all studies focusing on romantic partners used dyadic interactions. This may reflect the tendency of different research groups to use different research paradigms. While there does not seem to be an advantage of one EA paradigm over the other, each has its advantages and limitations. The dyadic paradigm better simulates real-life face-to-face interactions, and it can be used with actual partners expressing emotions from their actual lives together; however, each interaction will end up very different and thus can be difficult to compare. Moreover, this paradigm requires a more demanding coding and scoring process, and it relies on the judgment of raters in assessing the similarity between the target and the perceiver. The emotional story inferring paradigm, on the other hand, is simpler and easier to facilitate as a lab procedure, with the main advantage being the use of the same stimuli for all participants. This can enable a clear separation between the effects of target and perceiver characteristics (as all perceivers see the exact same targets), but it is by nature less ecological. The diary procedure is the most ecological in the sense of having a longer temporal window in which one can examine EA; however, it is suitable mainly for couples, it is the hardest to manipulate and control, and it relies heavily on the participants’ cooperation in their natural environment. Thus, the review does not provide general support for the use of a specific paradigm over the others, but it suggests that scholars should consider the characteristics of each paradigm in light of the research question, the clinical population and the available resources.

      Importantly, EA served as a sensitive measure that detected between-group differences even when other paradigms such as emotion detection from still pictures or ToM measures did not (71) in ASD; [(54, 58) in schizophrenia], suggesting that EA, as a complex ecological paradigm, better captures nuanced deficits. However, it is also possible that EA traces a more specific aspect of empathy impairment in these populations that is not captured by the other tasks. A third potential explanation might be that EA tasks are more difficult for these populations due to attention, executive functions, or motor requirements. These observations should be taken into account when planning future studies with clinical populations, and tasks should be made simpler when possible.

      An interesting modification was added to some of the reviewed studies, namely, asking participants to report not only on the target’s assumed experience, but also on their own. The authors could then assess not only how accurate participants were in identifying the emotional state of the target, but also how much they themselves shared the target’s affective experience. This addition to the EA paradigm seems to be especially valuable in clinical populations, where deconstructing the multifaceted concept of empathy could contribute to a better understanding of unique clinical profiles. For example, Martin-Key et al. (82) found no impairment in the classic EA measure in study participants with conduct disorder, but found a difference in levels of shared experience, which they referred to as emotional empathy. Similarly, Morrison et al. (77), who studied individuals with SAD, measured both EA and empathic congruence, comparing the subjects’ continuous rating of their own emotion to the targets’ ratings. They, too, found no impairment in EA but did find significantly lower empathic congruence in a group of individuals with SAD compared to a nonclinical control group.

      Overall, reduced EA was found in schizophrenia, ASD, and psychopathy when compared with nonclinical control groups, and also when compared to individuals with bipolar disorder (in schizophrenia) or ADHD (in ASD). In the context of depression, lower EA was found in the context of negative emotional content (for both males and females), and in higher levels of depressive symptoms in females, but not in males (74). However, other findings indicated no correlation between depression and EA (76)—and even, in certain contexts, an association between higher depressive symptoms and higher EA (75). Negative emotional content conveyed by the target was also associated with lower EA in schizophrenia (58, 61), and this pattern may be true for healthy individuals as well (63). Thus, it may be concluded that negative emotional content is harder to infer accurately, and that gender and clinical condition are among the variables that moderate this specific difficulty. Other variables found to be associated with lower EA were reduced social support (58), the subjects’ history of violence (54), smaller temporal pole volumes and past suicide attempts (55). Note, however, that all the above were inferred from the schizophrenia cohorts and may not apply to the general population. Importantly, reduced EA was also evident when participants were asked to infer the thoughts and feelings of targets with ADHD (66) or with BPD (81), meaning that individuals with these clinical conditions were “harder to read.” This pattern is demonstrated specifically for depressive tendencies of females within marital relationships, where depression in females was found to be associated with reduced EA in both the females and their partners (74). Interestingly, this was not the case for targets with ASD, who did not differ from TD targets in their “readability” [i.e., did not yield lower EA scores in perceivers; (69)].

      A clinical condition that has been hypothesized to be associated with enhanced EA is BPD (2325, 96, 97). Miano et al. (80) found support for this hypothesis only in females in a romantic relationship setting, and specifically in a relationship-threatening situation. However, Flury et al. (81) remind us that when comparing between a clinical and a nonclinical group within a dyadic setting, EA scores are relative and are not independent. If one group gets a higher score than the other, it may imply better EA, or it may hint at more difficulty inferring from the clinical group as targets. Indeed, after utilizing an Actor–Partner Interdependence Model, that was the authors’ conclusion.

      A clinical population that does seem to exhibit enhanced EA is SAD, specifically under the experience of social threat (78) or when in a relationship-threatening situation (79). These findings imply better performance under social threat, which may be explained by higher arousal, greater attention or higher motivation in such situations. It is important to note that higher EA may not always be an advantage. For example, Miano et al. (80) claim that the pattern of empathic inaccuracy that they found among participants with a low risk for BPD is an adaptive skill in a relationship-threatening situation.

      EA performance was improved by oxytocin in schizophrenia (57) and in people with more pronounced autistic traits (67). Feldman et al. (103) found that face-to-face synchronized parent-child interaction had the effect of normalizing oxytocin level in children with ASD, and keeping it high during social contact. This role of social interaction in elevating oxytocin levels in individuals with ASD, alongside the findings on the association between oxytocin and improved EA performance, may relate to the next variable that was found to be associated with better EA functioning in ASD: participation.

      In ASD, it was found that participating in the same dyadic interaction that they later had to rate contributed to better EA, compared to inferring from passive observation (69). This could indicate an advantage for the participation itself (over observation), an advantage for learning and rehearsing, or both. Higher arousal, immediate feedback, attention and motivation may also explain this effect. Another variable that was found to be associated with improvement in EA abilities among participants with ASD was the extent to which the situation was more or less structured, i.e., how clear and predictable the social interaction was. ASD participants specifically benefited from a structured versus unstructured situation (68). Thus, in order to better characterize EA in ASD, it is desirable to simulate the complex, dynamic and unstructured daily social interactions, while in planning intervention programs it may be of great value to take into account the potential importance of participation and the role of a structured social situation in encouraging the EA abilities of people with ASD.

      Given the dyadic nature of empathy, both the target and the perceiver contribute to EA. The perceiver’s ability to accurately infer the target’s thoughts and feelings depends not only on his/her states and traits but also on the various characteristics of the target, such as expressivity and motivation. Though some studies referred to such “target effects” on EA [e.g., (53, 63, 66, 75)], much of the reviewed literature emphasized the perceiver’s side. Specifically, many studies were designed to investigate whether a clinical condition affects the perceiver’s EA performance [e.g., (5456, 58, 64, 65)]. Another interesting question that is highly relevant on both a social and a clinical level is how a clinical condition of a target affects the way perceivers understand the target’s emotional state. Moreover, a target–perceiver interaction effect must also be considered. Such questions relate to the growing literature on “the double empathy problem,” which stresses that it’s not only autistic people who struggle with empathy—neuro-typical people also struggle to understand the minds of autistic individuals and empathize with them (104, 105). Such ideas challenge the traditional framing of autism as entailing empathic dysfunction. EA measures can be helpful in investigating this dyadic nature of empathy, as they rely on both the target and the perceiver’s reports. Such directions are suggested by the findings of Flury et al. (81) on BPD patients’ “readability,” and of Demurie et in the context of ADHD, and may be of great value in the study of other clinical conditions as well.

      We suggest that these variables, discussed in the context of either impairing or enhancing EA, can be further classified as subject variables (e.g., the clinical condition, clinical profile, biological characteristics, previous experiences, participation, social support), target variables (such as expressivity, content conveyed, clinical condition and specific profile), and situational variables (e.g., structured vs. unstructured, threatening, familiar). Within this framework, wherein the subject, the target or the situation can influence EA results, it may be valuable to consider additional variables in future research on empathy in clinical populations. One such variable that was barely directly addressed in the reviewed papers, yet was very pronounced in the process of synthesizing the findings, is gender.

      The current review reveals a general underrepresentation of female participants in studies on clinical populations, and a slight underrepresentation for males in studies aiming to evaluate clinical traits or states in nonclinical or high-risk samples. This finding may reflect either a trend in research questions and aims, recruitment challenges (sometimes due to male:female ratio in a specific condition) or both. One consequence of this trend is that while in nonclinical studies gender differences can be (and sometimes are) examined, in studies based on clinical samples, the associations between EA, gender and clinical condition are hardly addressed. For example, without considering the male:female ratio of participants in each study, one might conclude that EA is impaired in ASD, schizophrenia, psychopathy and conduct disorder, and that EA is intact in bipolar disorder, enhanced to some extent in borderline disorder, and that in SAD the dysfunction is due to a lack of protective inaccuracy. But a closer look at the gender of participants in each category reveals that while studies in ASD, schizophrenia, psychopathy and conduct disorder were done mostly on male participants, research on BPD and SAD relied more on female participants. To date, in most studies on clinical populations, the sample size is not large enough to address this question, with the recruitment of clinical participants and specifically females constituting one of the main challenges limiting the studies, as researchers themselves often note (57, 61, 64, 67).

      We believe that findings regarding gender, clinical phenotype and EA interactions may have important clinical implications. For example, Gadassi et al. (74) state that according to their findings, when females are depressed, their romantic relationship suffers doubly: first, because their own EA is lower, and second, because their partner’s EA is also lower. In contrast, when males are depressed, neither their own nor their partner’s levels of EA change. In the field of autism research, for example, there is a growing understanding that the male:female ratio might be different than previously assumed (106108). New research indicates that females with autism are underdiagnosed and understudied, due to lack of knowledge on the ASD female phenotype, and perhaps to the “camouflage effect” [an hypothesis that females with ASD are better at camouflaging their social deficits; (109)]. Along these lines, we encourage future studies to take gender into account, and call for a deeper investigation of a potential clinical profile, EA and gender interaction.

      Lastly, we want to draw attention to a group of studies focusing on EA in the context of violent or aggressive behavior in intimate relationships. These studies did not appear in our systematic search based on the chosen search-words but were brought to our attention by a reviewer, and we agree that they are of clear relevance to this review, as aggressive behavior may relate to various clinical conditions (16). For example, Schweinle et al. (110) investigated whether husbands’ wife-directed aggression is related to unusual accuracy (hypersensitivity), or to a bias to infer criticism or rejection inappropriately when they infer women’s thoughts and feelings. They used a procedure similar to the dyadic interaction paradigm to assess EA: videotapes depicting female clients participating in a simulated individual psychotherapy session with the same male therapist, focusing on intimate relationships. Each client watched her filmed therapy session and wrote down her thoughts and feelings through the session [originally developed by Marangoni et al. (91)]. The study’s participants (all males) were asked to infer the client’s thoughts and feelings while watching the videos. Then independent raters rated the similarity between the client’s self-report and the participant’s inferences of her thoughts and feelings. The results revealed that the greater the husbands’ bias to overattribute criticism and rejection to the thoughts and feelings of women they had never met, the more they reported behaving in a verbally aggressive way toward their own wives. The men’s overattribution bias, i.e., inaccurately inferring that women’s thoughts and feelings are critical or rejecting of their male partners, was related not only to aggression against their wives but to the men’s insecure attachment style [see also Clements et al. (111)]. An interesting future research direction may be to study both the violent individuals’ EA and their clinical profiles. Such an investigation can also examine whether the association between the aggression and the EA profile is unique to the intimate relationship context, or if it reflects a more pervasive personality characteristic.

      Strengths and Limitations of the Current Review, and Suggestions for Future Research

      To ensure a broad search of the literature, the search strategy included PsycNET and PubMed, as well as the snowball technique (also using Google Scholar search engine), and an updated search was performed in September 2019. This review may not have identified all published papers on EA in clinical populations despite attempts to be as comprehensive as possible. Thus, the main limitation of this study is the possibility that the review may have missed some relevant papers, as the search included many words and terms, and it was spread over many clinical populations and research fields. We did not review unpublished studies such as dissertations, which may have contributed additional knowledge. Exclusion of the gray literature from the search and exclusion of studies published in a language other than English has probably left some valuable information outside the scope of this review.

      As our aim was to present an overview of the existing literature on EA in clinical populations, we included all eligible peer-reviewed studies, regardless of methodological quality. Future research should address the methodological issues and aim for a meta-analysis of suitable and well-designed studies. This may be of great value in light of the small sample sizes typical of studies on clinical populations.

      It seems that the study of EA in clinical populations could benefit from a modified measure that can capture both EA and empathic congruence (77), or accuracy in sharing the affective experience (82). On the other hand, it is reasonable to assume that clinical populations are even more susceptible than nonclinical populations to the length of a task/fatigue effects, and both those considerations must be taken into account when planning a study evaluating EA in a clinical sample.

      Research on EA in clinical populations has added to the accumulating knowledge on the price one pays for not accurately understanding others’ affective and mental states. Another interesting and potentially important question refers to the experience of the targets when they are not being understood. We have learned that low EA is associated with depression in the partners of the clinical patients with low EA (74, 75, 102). Thus, people who are not accurately understood on a daily basis suffer from the other, less-studied side of the EA model. Therefore, future studies could benefit from not only examining EA in a relevant clinical population, but exploring the effects of EA difficulties on spouses, family members and other social partners as well. It would also be relevant to examine individuals with clinical conditions not just as subjects or perceivers but also as targets of EA, i.e., to study not only how accurate individuals with social deficits are at understanding the other and sharing their emotions, but how accurately they are being understood by others, and possible associations with well-being.

      To the best of our knowledge, this is the first review of the existing literature on EA in clinical conditions, states and traits. It reveals a growing interest in using these measures to deepen our understanding of clinical profiles, and it indicates that EA assessments have the potential to capture unique and subtle characteristics of empathic function and dysfunction. It also points to the paucity of existing studies on EA in the context of most clinical conditions. Due to the variance between and within clinical populations, and the variety of research aims, designs and methods across existing studies, it is difficult to draw robust meta-analytic conclusions regarding the nature of EA in clinical populations. A promising future research direction would be to integrate the cumulative knowledge on EA in the general (nonclinical) population with emerging data from clinical populations. For example, in the studies reviewed here, anxiety was found to be associated with enhanced EA in a relationship-threatening situation ((79); see also 93 for similar results with BPD). Ickes & Simpson (112) refer to motivational inaccuracy as protective in intimate relationship under certain circumstances of threat to the relationship, and it seems that anxiety and BPD are associated (perhaps only in females) with not applying this protective behavior. An alternative explanation is that enhanced EA is associated with enhanced alertness, sensitivity or arousal, which may characterize BPD patients as well as anxious individuals and individuals under threat [support for such an interpretation also can be found in Auyeung and Alden (78), and Devlin et al. (85)]. This interpretation may account for Ponnet et al.’s finding in which participants with ASD did not differ from a control group in their EA scores after participating in an interaction with the target (69). It may be that real face-to-face interactions cause increased alertness and arousal, and these facilitated EA. More research is needed in order to disentangle the role of personality traits and emotional states in EA in both clinical and nonclinical populations.

      In summary, EA is an important measure, paradigm and concept in empathy research in the context of clinical populations. Though some limitations to the use of specific tools for measuring EA in clinical populations need to be considered, it seems that EA paradigms are promising for measuring outcomes and discriminating clinical from nonclinical populations, and subgroups within clinical conditions, even when other paradigms fail to do so. It may be that with further advances in research, EA paradigms could be used as a screening tool, and maybe even in training and practicing empathic abilities. In future research on EA in clinical populations, we suggest addressing understudied populations, such as psychopathy. Subject, target and situational variables should be considered, with special attention to gender differences (and similarities), the association between EA abilities and adaptive functioning, and the study of individuals with clinical conditions as targets of EA. These avenues of investigation may promote a better understanding of the nature of EA, of specific clinical profiles and of social attitudes toward people with clinical conditions.

      Author Contributions

      Both YR and AP equally contributed to the review.

      Funding

      This work was supported by an Azrieli Fellowship from the Azrieli Foundation to AP.

      Conflict of Interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      References Decety J Bartal IB Uzefovsky F Knafo-Noam A . Empathy as a driver of prosocial behaviour: highly conserved neurobehavioural mechanisms across species. Philos Trans R Soc B: Biol Sci (2016) 371(1686):20150077. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0077 Uzefovsky F Knafo-Noam A . Empathy development throughout the life span. In: Social Cognition. New York: Routledge (2016). p. 89115. Walter H . Social cognitive neuroscience of empathy: concepts, circuits, and genes. Emotion Rev (2012) 4(1):917. doi: 10.1177/1754073911421379 Decety J Norman GJ Berntson GG Cacioppo JT . A neurobehavioral evolutionary perspective on the mechanisms underlying empathy. Prog Neurobiol (2012) 98(1):3848. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2012.05.001 Davis MH Luce C Kraus SJ . The heritability of characteristics associated with dispositional empathy. J Personal (1994) 62(3):369–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1994.tb00302.x Decety J . The neuroevolution of empathy. Ann New York Acad Sci (2011) 1231(1):3545. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06027.x Eisenberg N Fabes RA Carlo G Speer AL Switzer G Karbon M . The relations of empathy-related emotions and maternal practices to children′ s comforting behavior. J Exp Child Psychol (1993) 55(2):131–50. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1993.1007 Abramson L Uzefovsky F Toccaceli V Knafo-Noam A . The genetic and environmental origins of emotional and cognitive empathy: Review and meta-analyses of twin studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2020). doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.03.023 Baron-Cohen S . Empathizing, systemizing, and the extreme male brain theory of autism. In: Progress in brain research vol. 186. Oxford, UK: Elsevier (2010) p. 167–75. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53630-3.00011-7 Bird G Viding E . The self to other model of empathy: providing a new framework for understanding empathy impairments in psychopathy, autism, and alexithymia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2014) 47:520–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.021 Blair RJ . Responding to the emotions of others: Dissociating forms of empathy through the study of typical and psychiatric populations. Consciousness Cogn (2005) 14(4):698718. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2005.06.004 Blair RJ . Neuroimaging of psychopathy and antisocial behavior: a targeted review. Curr Psychiatry Rep (2010) 12(1):7682. doi: 10.1007/s11920-009-0086-x Brüne M . Emotion recognition,’theory of mind,’and social behavior in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res (2005) 133(2-3):135–47. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.10.007 Horan WP Green MF . Treatment of social cognition in schizophrenia: Current status and future directions. Schizophr Res (2019) 203:311. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.07.013 Tandon R Gaebel W Barch DM Bustillo J Gur RE Heckers S . Definition and description of schizophrenia in the DSM-5. Schizophr Res (2013) 150(1):310. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.05.028 DSM-5 American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing (2013). Hare RD . The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R). Toronto: Multi-Health Systems (2003). Derntl B Finkelmeyer A Toygar TK Hülsmann A Schneider F Falkenberg DI . Generalized deficit in all core components of empathy in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2009) 108(1-3):197206. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.11.009 Jones AP Happé FG Gilbert F Burnett S Viding E . Feeling, caring, knowing: different types of empathy deficit in boys with psychopathic tendencies and autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2010) 51(11):1188–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02280.x Shamay-Tsoory SG Shur S Barcai-Goodman L Medlovich S Harari H Levkovitz Y . Dissociation of cognitive from affective components of theory of mind in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res (2007) 149(1-3):1123. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.10.018 Harari H Shamay-Tsoory SG Ravid M Levkovitz Y . Double dissociation between cognitive and affective empathy in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res (2010) 175(3):277–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.03.002 Shamay-Tsoory S Harari H Szepsenwol O Levkovitz Y . Neuropsychological evidence of impaired cognitive empathy in euthymic bipolar disorder. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci (2009) 21(1):5967. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2009.21.1.59 Frank H Hoffman N . Borderline empathy: An empirical investigation. Compr Psychiatry (1986) 27(4):387–95. doi: 10.1016/0010-440X(86)90015-5 Guttman HA Laporte L . Empathy in families of women with borderline personality disorder, anorexia nervosa, and a control group. Family Process (2000) 39(3):345–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2000.39306.x Ladisich W Feil WB . Empathy in psychiatric patients. Br J Med Psychol (1988) 61(2):155–62. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1988.tb02774.x Knafo A Zahn-Waxler C Davidov M Van Hulle C Robinson JL Rhee SH . Empathy in early childhood: genetic, environmental, and affective contributions. Ann New York Acad Sci (2009) 1167(1):103–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04540.x Van Hulle C Zahn-Waxler C Robinson JL Rhee SH Hastings PD Knafo A . Autonomic correlates of children’s concern and disregard for others. Soc Neurosci (2013) 8(4):275–90. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2013.791342 Zahn-Waxler C Robinson JL Emde RN . The development of empathy in twins. Dev Psychol (1992) 28(6):1038. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.6.1038 Mikolajewski AJ Chavarria J Moltisanti A Hart SA Taylor J . Examining the factor structure and etiology of prosociality. psychol Assessment (2014) 26(4):1259. doi: 10.1037/a0037132 Knafo A Steinberg T Goldner I . Children’s low affective perspective-taking ability is associated with low self-initiated pro-sociality. Emotion (2011) 11(1):194. doi: 10.1037/a0021240 Nowicki S Duke MP . Individual differences in the nonverbal communication of affect: The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy Scale. J Nonverbal Behav (1994) 18(1):935. doi: 10.1007/BF02169077 Davis MH . Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. J Pers Soc Psychol (1983) 44(1):113. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113 Lawrence EJ Shaw P Baker D Baron-Cohen S David AS . Measuring empathy: reliability and validity of the Empathy Quotient. psychol Med (2004) 34(5):911–20. doi: 10.1017/S0033291703001624 Funk J Fox C Chan M Curtiss K . The development of the Children’s Empathic Attitudes Questionnaire using classical and Rasch analyses. J Appl Dev Psychol (2008) 29(3):187–96. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2008.02.005 Baron-Cohen S O’riordan M Stone V Jones R Plaisted K . Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord (1999) 29(5):407–18. doi: 10.1023/A:1023035012436 Baron-Cohen S Wheelwright S Hill J Raste Y Plumb I . The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl (2001) 42(2):241–51. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00715 Corcoran R Cahill C Frith CD . The appreciation of visual jokes in people with schizophrenia: a study of ‘mentalizing’ability. Schizophr Res (1997) 24(3):319–27. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(96)00117-X Happé FG . Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition (1993) 48(2):101–19. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(93)90026-R Mo S Su Y Chan RC Liu J . Comprehension of metaphor and irony in schizophrenia during remission: the role of theory of mind and IQ. Psychiatry Res (2008) 157(1-3):21–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.04.002 Wimmer H Perner J . Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition (1983) 13(1):103–28. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5 Zaki J Ochsner K . The need for a cognitive neuroscience of naturalistic social cognition. Ann New York Acad Sci (2009) 1167:16. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04601.x Ickes W . Empathic accuracy. J Personal (1993) 61(4):587610. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1993.tb00783.x Ickes W Stinson L Bissonnette V Garcia S . Naturalistic social cognition: Empathic accuracy in mixed-sex dyads. J Pers Soc Psychol (1990) 59(4):730. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.59.4.730 Zaki J Bolger N Ochsner K . It takes two: The interpersonal nature of empathic accuracy. psychol Sci (2008) 19(4):399404. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02099.x Zaki J Ochsner KN . The neuroscience of empathy: progress, pitfalls and promise. Nat Neurosci (2012) 15(5):675–80. doi: 10.1038/nn.3085 Levenson RW Ruef AM . Empathy: A physiological substrate. J Pers Soc Psychol (1992) 63(2):234–46. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.2.234 Howland M Rafaeli E . Bringing everyday mind reading into everyday life: Assessing empathic accuracy with daily diary data. J Personality (2010) 78(5):1437–68. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00657.x Sened H Yovel I Bar-Kalifa E Gadassi R Rafaeli E . Now you have my attention: Empathic accuracy pathways in couples and the role of conflict. Emotion (2017) 17(1):155. doi: 10.1037/emo0000220 Zaki J Weber J Bolger N Ochsner K . The neural bases of empathic accuracy. Proc Natl Acad Sci (2009) 106(27):11382–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0902666106 Arksey H O’Malley L . Scoping studies: towards a methodological framework. Int J Soc Res Methodol (2005) 8(1):1932. doi: 10.1080/1364557032000119616 Levac D Colquhoun H O’Brien KK . Scoping studies: advancing the methodology. Implementation Sci (2010) 5(1):69. doi: 10.1186/1748-5908-5-69 Liberati A Altman DG Tetzlaff J Mulrow C Gøtzsche PC Ioannidis JP . The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration. Ann Internal Med (2009) 151(4):W65. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2009.06.006 Van Donkersgoed RJ De Jong S Aan het Rot M Wunderink L Lysaker PH Hasson-Ohayon I . Measuring empathy in schizophrenia: The Empathic Accuracy Task and its correlation with other empathy measures. Schizophr Res (2019) 208:153–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.03.024 de Jong S van Donkersgoed R Renard S Carter S Bokern H Lysaker P . Social-cognitive risk factors for violence in psychosis: A discriminant function analysis. Psychiatry Res (2018) 265:93–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.04.048 Harenski CL Brook M Kosson DS Bustillo JR Harenski KA Caldwell MF . Socio-neuro risk factors for suicidal behavior in criminal offenders with psychotic disorders. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci (2017) 12(1):7080. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw164 Horan WP Reise SP Kern RS Lee J Penn DL Green MF . Structure and correlates of self-reported empathy in schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res (2015) 66:60–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.04.016 Davis MC Green MF Lee J Horan WP Senturk D Clarke AD . Oxytocin-augmented social cognitive skills training in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology (2014) 39(9):2070–7. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.68 Ripoll LH Zaki J Perez-Rodriguez MM Snyder R Strike KS Boussi A . Empathic accuracy and cognition in schizotypal personality disorder. Psychiatry Res (2013) 210(1):232–41. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.05.025 Olbert CM Penn DL Kern RS Lee J Horan WP Reise SP . Adapting social neuroscience measures for schizophrenia clinical trials, part 3: fathoming external validity. Schizophr Bull (2013) 39(6):1211–8. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt130 Kern RS Penn DL Lee J Horan WP Reise SP Ochsner KN . Adapting social neuroscience measures for schizophrenia clinical trials, Part 2: trolling the depths of psychometric properties. Schizophr Bull (2013) 39(6):1201–10. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt127 Harvey PO Zaki J Lee J Ochsner K Green MF . Neural substrates of empathic accuracy in people with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull (2013) 39(3):617–28. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs042 Lee J Altshuler L Glahn DC Miklowitz DJ Ochsner K Green MF . Social and nonsocial cognition in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: relative levels of impairment. Am J Psychiatry (2013) 170(3):334–41. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12040490 Lee J Zaki J Harvey PO Ochsner K Green MF . Schizophrenia patients are impaired in empathic accuracy. psychol Med (2011) 41(11):2297–304. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711000614 Adler N Dvash J Shamay-Tsoory SG . Empathic embarrassment accuracy in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res (2015) 8(3):241–9. doi: 10.1002/aur.1439 aan het Rot M Hogenelst K . The influence of affective empathy and autism spectrum traits on empathic accuracy. PloS One (2014) 9(6):e98436. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098436 Demurie E De Corel M Roeyers H . Empathic accuracy in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Res Autism Spectr Disord (2011) 5(1):126–34. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2010.03.002 Bartz JA Zaki J Bolger N Hollander E Ludwig NN Kolevzon A . Oxytocin selectively improves empathic accuracy. psychol Sci (2010) 21(10):1426–8. doi: 10.1177/0956797610383439 Ponnet K Buysse A Roeyers H De Clercq A . Mind-reading in young adults with ASD: Does structure matter? J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38(5):905–18. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0462-5 Ponnet K Buysse A Roeyers H De Corte K . Empathic accuracy in adults with a pervasive developmental disorder during an unstructured conversation with a typically developing stranger. J Autism Dev Disord (2005) 35(5):585600. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0003-z Roeyers H Buysse A Ponnet K Pichal B . Advancing advanced mind-reading tests: Empathic accuracy in adults with a pervasive developmental disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl (2001) 42(2):271–8. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00718 Ponnet KS Roeyers H Buysse A De Clercq A Van Der Heyden E . Advanced mind-reading in adults with Asperger syndrome. Autism (2004) 8(3):249–66. doi: 10.1177/1362361304045214 aan het Rot M Miloserdov K Buijze AL Meesters Y Gordijn MC . Premenstrual mood and empathy after a single light therapy session. Psychiatry Res (2017) 256:212–8. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.06.052 Hogenelst K Schoevers RA Kema IP Sweep FC aan het Rot M . Empathic accuracy and oxytocin after tryptophan depletion in adults at risk for depression. Psychopharmacology (2016) 233(1):111–20. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4093-9 Gadassi R Mor N Rafaeli E . Depression and empathic accuracy in couples: An interpersonal model of gender differences in depression. psychol Sci (2011) 22(8):1033–41. doi: 10.1177/0956797611414728 Papp LM Kouros CD Cummings EM . Emotions in marital conflict interactions: Empathic accuracy, assumed similarity, and the moderating context of depressive symptoms. J Soc Pers Relationships (2010) 27(3):367–87. doi: 10.1177/0265407509348810 Thomas G Fletcher GJ Lange C . On-line empathic accuracy in marital interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol (1997) 72(4):839. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.72.4.839 Morrison AS Mateen MA Brozovich FA Zaki J Goldin PR Heimberg RG . Empathy for positive and negative emotions in social anxiety disorder. Behav Res Ther (2016) 87:232–42. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.10.005 Auyeung KW Alden LE . Social anxiety and empathy for social pain. Cogn Ther Res (2016) 40(1):3845. doi: 10.1007/s10608-015-9718-0 Simpson JA Ickes W Grich J . When accuracy hurts: Reactions of anxious–ambivalent dating partners to a relationship-threatening situation. J Pers Soc Psychol (1999) 76(5):754. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.5.754 Miano A Dziobek I Roepke S . Understanding interpersonal dysfunction in borderline personality disorder: A naturalistic dyadic study reveals absence of relationship-protective empathic inaccuracy. Clin psychol Sci (2017) 5(2):355–66. doi: 10.1177/2167702616683505 Flury JM Ickes W Schweinle W . The borderline empathy effect: Do high BPD individuals have greater empathic ability? Or are they just more difficult to “read”? J Res Personality (2008) 42(2):312–32. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2007.05.008 Martin-Key N Brown T Fairchild G . Empathic accuracy in male adolescents with conduct disorder and higher versus lower levels of callous-unemotional traits. J Abnormal Child Psychol (2017) 45(7):1385–97. doi: 10.1007/s10802-016-0243-8 De Ridder J Pihet S Suter M Caldara R . Empathy in institutionalized adolescents with callous-unemotional traits: An ecological momentary assessment study of emotion recognition. Crim Justice Behav (2016) 43(5):653–69. doi: 10.1177/0093854815618431 Brook M Kosson DS . Impaired cognitive empathy in criminal psychopathy: Evidence from a laboratory measure of empathic accuracy. J Abnormal Psychol (2013) 122(1):156. doi: 10.1037/a0030261 Devlin HC Zaki J Ong DC Gruber J . Tracking the emotional highs but missing the lows: Hypomania risk is associated with positively biased empathic inference. Cogn Ther Res (2016) 40(1):72–9. doi: 10.1007/s10608-015-9720-6 Brown CL Lwi SJ Goodkind MS Rankin KP Merrilees J Miller BL . Empathic accuracy deficits in patients with neurodegenerative disease: association with caregiver depression. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry (2018) 26(4):484–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2017.10.012 Doyle-Thomas KA Lee W Foster NE Tryfon A Ouimet T Hyde KL . Atypical functional brain connectivity during rest in autism spectrum disorders. Ann Neurol (2015) 77(5):866–76. doi: 10.1002/ana.24391 Wells JL Brown CL Hua AY Soyster PD Chen KH Dokuru DR . Neurodegenerative Disease Caregivers’5-HTTLPR Genotype Moderates the Effect of Patients’ Empathic Accuracy Deficits on Caregivers’ Well-Being. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry (2019) 27(10):1046–56. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2019.04.009 Reniers RL Corcoran R Drake R Shryane NM Völlm BA . The QCAE: A questionnaire of cognitive and affective empathy. J Pers Assessment (2011) 93(1):8495. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2010.528484 Gross JJ John OP Richards J . Berkeley expressivity questionnaire. Lewiston, NY: Edwin Mellen Press (1995). Marangoni C Garcia S Ickes W Teng G . Empathic accuracy in a clinically relevant setting. J Pers Soc Psychol (1995) 68(5):854. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.68.5.854 Baron-Cohen S Wheelwright S Skinner R Martin J Clubley E . The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, malesand females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord (2001) 31(1):517. doi: 10.1023/A:1005653411471 Rush AJ Trivedi MH Ibrahim HM Carmody TJ Arnow B Klein DN . The 16-Item Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (QIDS), clinician rating (QIDS-C), and self-report (QIDS-SR): a psychometric evaluation in patients with chronic major depression. Biol Psychiatry (2003) 54(5):573–83. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01866-8 Brennan KA Clark CL Shaver PR . Self-report measurement of adult attachment: An integrative overview. In: Simpson JA Rholes WS , editors. Attachment theory and close relationships. New York: Guilford Press (1998). Griffin DW Bartholomew K . Models of the self and other: Fundamental dimensions underlying measures of adult attachment. J Pers Soc Psychol (1994) 67(3):430. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.430 Carter L Rinsley DB . Vicissitudes of’ empathy’in a borderline adolescent. Int Rev Psycho-Analysis (1977) 4:317–26. Krohn A . Borderline “empathy” and differentiation of object representations: a contribution to the psychology of object relations. Int J Psychoanal Psychother (1974) 3(2):142. Conte HR Plutchik R Karasu TB Jerrett I . A self-report borderline scale: Discriminative validity and preliminary norms. J Nervous Ment Dis (1980) 168(7):428–35. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198007000-00007 Campbell L Kashy DA . Estimating actor, partner, and interaction effects for dyadic data using PROC MIXED and HLM: A user–friendly guide. Pers Relationships (2002) 9(3):327–42. doi: 10.1111/1475-6811.00023 Kashy DA Kenny DA . The analysis of data from dyads and groups. In: Reis HT Judd CM , editors. Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press (2000) p. 451–77. Kenny DA . Models of non-independence in dyadic research. J Soc Pers Relationships (1996) 13(2):279–94. doi: 10.1177/0265407596132007 Eckblad M Chapman LJ . Development and validation of a scale for hypomanic personality. J Abnormal Psychol (1986) 95(3):214. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.95.3.214 Feldman R Golan O Hirschler-Guttenberg Y Ostfeld-Etzion S Zagoory-Sharon O . Parent–child interaction and oxytocin production in pre-schoolers with autism spectrum disorder. Br J Psychiatry (2014) 205(2):107–12. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.137513 Milton DE . On the ontological status of autism: the ‘double empathy problem’. Disability Soc (2012) 27(6):883–7. doi: 10.1080/09687599.2012.710008 Milton DEM Heasman B Sheppard E . Double Empathy. In: Volkmar F , editors Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders. New York, NY: Springer (2018). doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-6435-8_102273-1 Gould J Ashton-Smith J . Missed diagnosis or misdiagnosis? Girls and women on the autism spectrum. Good Autism Pract (GAP) (2011) 12(1):3441. Kirkovski M Enticott PG Fitzgerald PB . A review of the role of female gender in autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43(11):2584–603. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1811-1 Van Wijngaarden-Cremers PJ van Eeten E Groen WB Van Deurzen PA Oosterling IJ Van der Gaag RJ . Gender and age differences in the core triad of impairments in autism spectrum disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Autism Dev Disord Abingdon, Oxon: Routledge (2014) 44(3):627–35. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1913-9 Carpenter B Happé F Egerton J eds. Girls and autism: educational, family and personal perspectives. Abingdon, Oxon: Routledge. (2019). Feb 7. Schweinle WE Ickes W Bernstein IH . Emphatic inaccuracy in husband to wife aggression: The overattribution bias. Pers Relationships (2002) 9(2):141–58. doi: 10.1111/1475-6811.00009 Clements K Holtzworth-Munroe A Schweinle W Ickes W . Empathic accuracy of intimate partners in violent versus nonviolent relationships. Pers Relationships (2007) 14(3):369–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2007.00161.x Ickes W Simpson JA . Motivational aspects of empathic accuracy. In: Fletcher GJO Clark MS , editors, Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Interpersonal processes. Malden, MA: Blackwell (2003) pp. 229–49. doi: 10.1002/9780470998557.ch9
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016hanabi8.com.cn
      www.hxchain.com.cn
      itbcze.com.cn
      lbirti.com.cn
      l37.com.cn
      www.fwdbcb.com.cn
      www.plobms.com.cn
      www.uigsnj.com.cn
      www.sxzesf.com.cn
      szwallet.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p