Front. Psychiatry Frontiers in Psychiatry Front. Psychiatry 1664-0640 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00301 Psychiatry Review Two-Person Approaches to Studying Social Interaction in Psychiatry: Uses and Clinical Relevance Pan Yafeng 1 2 Cheng Xiaojun 1 * 1 School of Psychology, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China 2 Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden

Edited by: Leonhard Schilbach, Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich, Germany

Reviewed by: Wolfgang Tschacher, University of Bern, Switzerland; Sebastian Walther, University of Bern, Switzerland; Cade McCall, University of York, United Kingdom

*Correspondence: Xiaojun Cheng, chengxiaojun@szu.edu.cn

This article was submitted to Social Cognition, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychiatry

24 04 2020 2020 11 301 15 01 2020 26 03 2020 Copyright © 2020 Pan and Cheng 2020 Pan and Cheng

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Social interaction is ubiquitous in human society. The two-person approach—a new, powerful tool to study information exchange and social behaviors—aims to characterize the behavioral dynamics and neural mechanisms of real-time social interactions. In this review, we discuss the benefits of two-person approaches compared to those for conventional, single-person approaches. We describe measures and paradigms that model social interaction in three dimensions (3-D), including eye-to-eye, body-to-body, and brain-to-brain relationships. We then discuss how these two-person measures and paradigms are used in psychiatric conditions (e.g., autism, mood disorders, schizophrenia, borderline personality disorder, and psychotherapy). Furthermore, the advantages of a two-person approach (e.g., dual brain stimulation, multi-person neurofeedback) in clinical interventions are described. Finally, we discuss the methodological and translational challenges surrounding the application of two-person approaches in psychiatry, as well as prospects for future two-/multi-person studies. We conclude that two-person approaches serve as useful additions to the range of behavioral and neuroscientific methods available to assess social interaction in psychiatric settings, for both diagnostic techniques and complementary interventions.

two-person approach social interaction psychiatry application intervention 31800951 2018A030310431 2017KQNCX172 National Natural Science Foundation of China10.13039/501100001809 Natural Science Foundation of Guangdong Province10.13039/501100003453 Foundation for Distinguished Young Talents in Higher Education of Guangdong10.13039/501100012243

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      The scientific study of the neurophysiological mechanisms that underlie social processes is undergoing a major paradigm shift: moving from the examination of single brains to the simultaneous acquisition of data from multiple brains and their interaction [see, e.g., our recent contributions: (16); see also (79) for reviews]. Within this fast-emerging area of research, we focused on recent advances that examined the relationship between eye gaze/body movement/brain activity recorded from interacting dyads under psychiatric settings. Thus, we attempted to characterize social interaction in psychiatry within a two-person framework (911).

      In the present review, we will first briefly introduce the two-person approach and its benefits compared to those of a single-person approach, as well as potential interpersonal paradigms/markers derived from this approach. We will then focus on two-person studies in two aspects: first, the applications of the two-person approach in multiple cases of psychiatric conditions (e.g., autism, mood disorders, schizophrenia, borderline personality disorder, and psychotherapy), and second, the potential benefits of the two-person approach in psychiatric interventions (e.g., behavioral intervention, dual brain stimulation, multi-person neurofeedback). Finally, we will discuss challenges and future prospects of the applications of the two-person approach.

      What Is the Two-Person Approach?

      Human society is organized socially. Despite the interactive nature of human social behaviors, conventional neuroscientific studies investigating social cognitive processes have typically been restricted to isolated individual behaviors, leaving the dynamic (neural) interactions between individuals incompletely understood. Methodological advances allow researchers to address this issue by developing a novel technique termed “hyperscanning” or “hypermethod” [e.g., using electroencephalography (EEG) (12), functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) (13), or functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) (14)]. This technique first highlights the simultaneous consideration of two individuals in an interacting dyad.

      In 2013, Schilbach and colleagues further advanced this field and formally proposed the theoretical framework of “second-person neuroscience” (11) or “two-person neuroscience” (15). Accordingly, recent years have seen fruitful empirical evidence of this two-person approach [for a review, see (9)]. The two-person approach has a basic assumption: behavioral and neural mechanisms supporting social cognition within the context of a real-time reciprocal social interaction are distinguishable from those within the context of social observation (without interaction). To further define two-person studies properly, two criteria were proposed: (i) social interactions should occur in real time and be reciprocal, and (ii) social interactions elicit psychological engagement (feeling of involvement with one another) between interacting partners. Studies having one of the two criteria could be seen as two-person studies.

      Note that two-person approaches do not necessarily mean that investigations should be conducted only with two interacting individuals; one can also develop variants by monitoring multiple persons because social interaction could also take place in multi-person situations (16).

      Is “Two” Indeed Better Than “One”?

      As described in the previous section, human social behaviors have an interactive nature. To characterize the dynamic social interaction between individuals, it is imperative to adopt the two-person approach. However, one important question should be addressed first:is “two” indeed better than “one”? Several neuroimaging studies have attempted to address this issue (6, 1719). Using fNIRS-based hyperscanning and machine learning approaches, Pan et al. found that two-brain measures served as a better neural-classification feature than single-brain measures (6). Specifically, machine learning techniques were reported to be more successful when decoding instructional approaches from instructor-learner brain coupling data than when using a single-brain method. Supporting these findings, previous fMRI studies reported that two-brain measures, such as brain-to-brain similarities, are more sensitive and better suited to track inter-personal influences, such as social network proximity (17); friends showed more similar neural responses to naturalistic movies, and such similar neural responses decreased with increasing social distance between friends. Monitoring and measuring two individuals simultaneously uncovered additional information beyond conventional single-brain approaches (18, 19). For example, compared to the single-brain method, which reflects a mixture of both neuronal components (i.e., stimulus-induced neural processes) and non-neuronal components (e.g., intrinsic neural processes and non-neuronal noise), two-brain measures using fMRI isolated stimulus-related inter-brain correlations (18). We believe that these prior studies are sufficient to imply that “two” performs better than “one” in several aspects during real-time social interaction; however, more research is needed to clarify the assets of the two-person approach compared to those of the single-person approach.

      It is important to note that we do not claim that adopting the single-person approach to investigate the social cognitive process is useless – this contention would discredit various classic and ongoing investigations in this field [e.g., (2022)]. Instead, we propose that using the two-person approach would add additional value to the exploration of dynamic and truly social interaction, thus advancing our understanding of both behavioral and neural mechanisms underlying human social behaviors.

      Modeling Social Interaction in 3-D

      To take advantage of the two-person approach, the field calls for interactive paradigms and interpersonal markers of real-time social interaction (23, 24). Here, we highlight three dimensions (3-D) that characterize the behavioral and neural mechanisms of social interaction. These paradigms/markers allow us to model real-time social interaction in 3-D: eye-to-eye, body-to-body, and brain-to-brain.

      Eye-to-Eye

      Gaze behavior is critical in social interaction and in communication in particular. While many studies have investigated the role of gaze behavior in social observation, research about a person's interactive gaze allowing eye-to-eye contact and face-to-face interaction is still lacking. Pfeiffer et al. reviewed novel approaches to investigate the neural systems that support social gaze behavior, thus requiring active social engagement (25); these novel approaches include interactions with virtual agents (26), live interactions via videos (27), and dual eye-tracking setups (28).

      Regarding dual-eye-tracking setups, in the last decade, we have seen fruitful applications of the eye-to-eye paradigm in investigating neural mechanisms of social interaction. For example, Saito and colleagues initiated a combination of fMRI hyperscanning and dual eye-tracking. With this novel setup, they found that paired subjects showed higher inter-individual neural synchronization in the right inferior frontal gyrus during mutual gaze and joint attention activities than non-paired subjects (28). Using EEG hyperscanning in an eye-to-eye (face-to-face) situation, Lachat et al. found that the joint attention condition—compared to the no-joint attention condition—induced an 11-13 Hz power decrease over left centro-parieto-occipital regions (29). As Lachat et al. suggested, the power decrease might reflect attention mirroring, social coordination, and mutual attentiveness associated with joint attention. Hirsch et al. combined a two-person eye-tracking system and fNIRS recordings. The results revealed that fronto-temporo-parietal neural systems synchronize within and across brains during live eye-to-eye contact, in contrast with the results for an eye-to-picture gaze (30).

      Body-to-Body

      Body movement is an important nonverbal cue and signal for social interaction. The coupling between hand/body movements acts as an index for implicit social interaction (31). For example, Yun et al. found that synchrony of both fingertip movement and neural activity between two individuals in a dyad increased after a cooperation interaction. In more complex social interactions, nonverbal interpersonal coordination (body sway) among people was indicative of leadership in joint music making (32).

      Furthermore, recent advances also reported that body-to-body coupling was associated with many positive outcomes, including prosociality (2), therapeutic alliances (33), mentalizing (34), and closeness (35). Using a combination of a two-person paradigm and fNIRS recordings, Hu and colleagues found that the manipulation of body-to-body synchrony predicted subsequent prosocial behaviors. Brain-to-brain synchronization between the two participants during ongoing movements might be a potential underlying mechanism. At the behavioral level, Ramseyer et al. (33) quantified nonverbal synchrony between the patient's and therapist's movements. They found that nonverbal synchrony reflected the relationship quality; synchrony was associated with symptom reduction. Baimel et al. found that behavioral synchrony between partners fostered mentalizing capacities. Synchrony increased the mental state attribution to interacting participant dyads (34). Another behavioral study explored the influence of motor synchrony on the experience of intimacy (35). Specifically, the authors examined whether body-to-body synchrony between partners instilled a sense of intimacy. The results suggested that synchrony was strongly associated with intimacy and possibly promoted closeness in intimate situations.

      Another subdomain of the body-to-body relationship concerns peripheral physiological signals, including the heart rate, electrodermal activity, and respiration. The relationship between the physiological activity of two or more people is referred to as “interpersonal autonomic physiology” or “physiological synchrony” (36). The concept of physiological synchrony has been incorporated into a wide range of contexts to investigate its relation with a number of social behaviors, including cooperation (37), singing (38), and romantic interaction (39). Specifically, in the field of psychiatry, physiological synchrony serves as a useful tool to track psychotherapy processes (4042). In 2016, Koole and Tschacher reviewed clinical studies on therapeutic alliances and interpersonal synchrony and then integrated both concepts into the interpersonal synchrony model of psychotherapy. In a later empirical study, Tschacher and Meier explored physiological synchrony in naturalistic psychotherapy sessions and found that synchrony correlated with the therapeutic alliance and psychotherapy session reports.

      Brain-to-Brain

      The most recent neuroimaging work in this field shifted the focus on single-brain functioning toward two-brain communication during real-time social interaction. As mentioned above, this shift is boosted by a fast-developing technique: “hyperscanning”. The concept of hyperscanning was first proposed by Montague et al. (14). In their commentary paper, the authors described simultaneous neuroimaging during linked social interactions; specifically, participants could interact with each other while their brain activity was simultaneously recorded. Regarding data analysis, hyperscanning setups enable us to effectively quantify the relationship between two brain activities. A common finding derived from previous hyperscanning studies is that brain activity from two interacting participants in a dyad tends to be “coupled together”, creating a joint networked state. This phenomenon is usually called “brain-to-brain synchrony/synchronization” (16) or “interpersonal brain/neural synchronization” (4, 5, 4345).

      The mechanisms of brain-to-brain synchrony are still debated. Although some researchers see brain-to-brain synchrony, per se, as a mechanism for social behaviors [e.g., (8)], others claim that synchrony is not a mechanism in itself but a measurable reflection of the underlying neural computations that support some psychological processes [e.g., (16)]. Dikker and colleagues proposed “shared attention” as a possible source of brain-to-brain synchrony by successfully demonstrating a positive relationship between alpha band power (a well-characterized index of attention) and synchrony measures. This account is also in line with a series of past studies using mutual gaze tasks (28, 4648). Apart from this account of shared attention, other studies also posited that social signals (such as gaze, gestures, or vocalizations) could promote mutual temporal alignment of the brains involved, leading to a joint networked state to facilitate information transfer (5, 49).

      Note that this review is not intended to be a comprehensive review of two-person approaches, given the excellent reviews previously published on this topic (9, 11, 50). For this reason, we did not go into depth about any techniques but instead refer readers to Table 1 that briefly describes the commonly used measures and other empirical work [e.g., (5860)]. Additionally, compared to emerging two-person neuroscience endeavors (i.e., brain-to-brain), two-person behaviors (i.e., eye-to-eye and body-to-body) have long been monitored to investigate social interaction. With this in mind, we mainly discussed behavioral paradigms that were (potentially) related to two-person neuroscience, as that is where our novelty lies.

      Several commonly used measures in two- or multi-person studies.

      Measure Description Example References
      Motion energy analysis Computing pixel changes across video frames and generate motion energy time series for both participants during interaction (33, 51)
      Windowed cross-correlation Tracking the movements of two variables or sets of data relative to each other (33, 51, 52)
      Phase locking value Measuring the consistency of the phase-difference (3, 53, 54)
      Circular correlation coefficient Measuring the circular covariance of differences between the observed phase and the expected phase (5557)
      Wavelet transform coherence Measuring local correlation between two signals as a function of both frequency and time (1, 46)
      Granger causality analysis Estimating the directional coupling (1, 4, 5, 45)

      Readers should keep in mind that the three aforementioned dimensions (eye-to-eye, body-to-body, and brain-to-brain) are not isolated but are strongly associated with each other. For instance, eye contact synchronization might occur in parallel with brain activity synchronization (28, 30), and body-to-body coupling could be associated with brain-to-brain coupling (2, 31). These novel approaches facilitate the investigation of behavioral dynamics and neural mechanisms of social behaviors (50) and thus yield new insights into the field of social interaction in psychiatry.

      Recent Applications of the Two-Person Approach in Social Interactions Under Psychiatric Settings Autism

      Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by social deficits in communication and inter-personal interactions, as well as non-social deficits in repetitive behavior (61). Social deficits in communication in individuals with ASD are reflected in various aspects, such as joint attention (6264), motor imitation (65), and interpersonal coordination (66, 67).

      Joint attention is a set of nonverbal behaviors, including eye gaze, pointing, and showing, which are used to reference outside objects during a communicative exchange (62). Generally, joint attention occurs within the context of a social interaction, when one person directs another person's attention to an object (initiating joint attention), and the second person's attention follows (responding to joint attention). Previous studies have established impaired joint attention in children with ASD (62) and that the early development of joint attention predicts future language and social cognitive skills in children with ASD (68, 69); therefore, joint attention skills are critical targets of intervention for this population.

      Atypical joint attention behaviors and brain activation patterns have been observed in individuals with ASD when they were required to view images or movies of real or virtual people (63, 70). However, these traditional experimental setups (i.e., viewing images or movies) might not be a promising method to disclose the neural basis of initiating joint attention in individuals with ASD. Concerning this issue, Redcay et al. adopted a dual-video setup that allowed for a face-to-face interaction between the subject and experimenter via video during fMRI data collection. By using the two-person approach, they depicted activation patterns related to initiating joint attention and responding to joint attention in both the ASD group and the normal control group (27). Compared with the normal group, the ASD group showed a reduced brain activation difference between joint attention conditions (including initiating joint attention and responding to joint attention) and solo attention conditions in the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex and right posterior superior temporal sulcus. Distinct regions included the ventromedial prefrontal cortex for responding to joint attention and the intraparietal sulcus and middle frontal gyrus for initiating joint attention (27). The lack of differentiation was further characterized by reduced activation during joint attention conditions and relative hyperactivation during solo attention conditions (71).

      Although Redcay and colleagues applied the two-person approach and adopted a live face-to-face communication task in their study, they used a single fMRI setting and measured a single brain in an isolated manner. To elucidate the neural substrates of direct, real-time interactions between ASD patients and normal subjects, Tanabe et al. (72) conducted the first fMRI-based hyperscanning study in ASD individuals by using the mutual gaze paradigm developed by Saito et al.. They found that compared to the normal–normal pairs, ASD–normal pairs exhibited less accurate gaze direction detection and less prominent inter-brain coherence in the right inferior frontal gyrus during eye contact (72). The findings suggest that the impairment of joint attention in ASD could be related to the difficulty in understanding shared intention through eye contact, which is represented by reduced inter-subject synchronization of cortical regions including the right inferior frontal gyrus.

      Apart from impaired joint attention, most individuals with ASD have deficits in interpersonal motor imitation and coordination (6567). A related study has shown that in individuals with ASD, a higher degree of autistic traits (i.e., higher Autism Spectrum Quotient score) could predict a lower ability to modulate movements to coordinate with normal individuals (i.e., social interactive tasks) but not differences in movement preparation and planning with a non-biological stimulus (i.e., non-social tasks) (66). This finding suggests that the failure of individuals with ASD to coordinate with others was not due to basic motor or executive function difficulties. Furthermore, the performance of individuals with ASD regarding interpersonal motor coordination could possibly depend on the social skill ability of the individuals with whom they are paired (73). Pairs of participants with widely differing Autism Spectrum Quotient scores performed better than pairs with similar Autism Spectrum Quotient scores in the inter-personal rhythmic movement task. Specifically, participants with relatively higher Autism Spectrum Quotient scores tended to precede their partners in the task.

      Recently, Wang and colleagues conducted fNIRS-based hyperscanning studies to further explore brain-to-brain coupling during interpersonal coordination tasks between children with ASD and normal partners (i.e., parents in the study). The results showed that compared to solo and non-interactive behaviors, coordinating interactions with their parents could elicit increased inter-personal neural synchronization in the frontal cortex of children with ASD (67). Neural synchronization was further found to be modulated by the children's autism symptoms and covaried with their cooperation task performance. That is, children with severe autism symptoms showed worse behavioral performance and less neural synchronization with their parents during coordination than children with less severe symptoms.

      Mood Disorders

      Patients suffering from mood disorders, e.g., major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder, showed atypical interpersonal communication according to their mood state (61). Previous studies have employed fMRI to explore cognitive and emotional dysfunctions and found altered activation of the amygdala, as well as the frontal, cingulate, and temporal cortices, in patients with major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder during various cognitive tasks (74). However, brain activation during conversation has not yet been investigated in patients with mood disorders due to methodological difficulties.

      In 2014, Takei and colleagues conducted a fNIRS study in which major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder patients performed a face-to-face conversation with an interviewer who was selected from the hospital staff and had not been previously acquainted with the participants (i.e., a two-person situation with a focus on the patient's brain). In their study, patients' frontal and temporal lobe activation levels were measured during the conversation condition (including speech and listening phases) and control condition (including syllables and silent phases). The results showed less activation in the left dorsolateral prefrontal and left frontopolar cortices in major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder patients than in normal individuals, as well as a rapid decrease in bilateral frontopolar activation in major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder patients. Particularly, in patients with major depressive disorder, the average amount of signal change over time in the frontopolar cortex was positively correlated with their Global Assessment of Functioning scores; in patients with bipolar disorder, the average brain activation during conversation was negatively correlated with the age of onset in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and both middle temporal lobes (75). These findings suggest that both continuous activation and rapid change may reflect the pathophysiological characteristics of major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder.

      Schizophrenia

      Schizophrenia is marked by poor social-role performance and social-functioning deficits that are well reflected in interpersonal communication (61). Such social deficits could be captured by body-to-body dynamics. For example, Kupper and colleagues found that a low level of nonverbal synchrony was associated with negative symptoms, low social competence, impaired social functioning, and low self-reported competence. Negative symptoms were more prominent when patients reduced their imitation of the movements of the interactant; in turn, positive symptoms were more prominent when interactants reduced their imitation of patients' movements (51).

      The social deficits shown by patients with schizophrenia could be associated with reduced volume and/or reduced gray matter activation in specific brain regions, such as the temporal lobe, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and cingulate cortex (76, 77). Takei et al. used fNIRS to investigate frontal and temporal lobe activation in patients with schizophrenia during the conversation (i.e., a two-person situation with a focus on the patient's brain). The results showed that patients with schizophrenia, compared to normal controls, were characterized by decreased activation in the bilateral temporal lobes and right inferior frontal gyrus during the conversation task (78). The decreased activation in the related brain regions negatively correlated with disorganization and negative symptoms suggested that the disorganization and negative symptoms observed in patients with schizophrenia in clinical situations are related to dysfunction of the left temporal lobe and right inferior frontal gyrus. In addition, frontal lobe dysfunction was also reported to be linked to difficulties in gesture planning and execution (79), which might explain the poor social functioning in schizophrenia patients.

      Borderline Personality Disorder

      Borderline personality disorder is characterized by repeated interpersonal conflict and unstable relationships (61). Bilek and colleagues recently explored the neurobiological mechanism of social interactive deficits in borderline personality disorder. In their study, current borderline personality disorder patients and remitted borderline personality disorder patients were recruited to perform a joint attention task with normal participants. Compared with the normal-normal pairs, normal-current borderline personality disorder pairs showed reduced interpersonal brain connectivity. Remarkably, for remitted patients, interpersonal brain connectivity was restored. These findings emerged only in the study of information flow between dyads and were not associated with any between-group differences in individual brain structure or function, indicating the necessity of two-person approaches. Cross-brain measurements, therefore, deliver state-associated biomarkers that may help to guide diagnostic and therapeutic procedures in the future (80).

      Psychotherapy

      As described above, individuals with psychiatric disorders have social deficits that reflect verbal or nonverbal coordination with others to some extent. It is worth noting that the synchrony, especially nonverbal synchrony, between patients and therapists is also highlighted during psychotherapy.

      Ramseyer et al. (33) found higher nonverbal coordination in genuine interactions (i.e., real pairs of patients and therapists) in contrast with pseudo-interactions (i.e., random pairs of patients and therapists). More importantly, nonverbal coordination was associated with patients experiencing high quality relationships and high self-efficacy (33). Other studies showed that nonverbal synchrony between patients and therapists could be modulated by therapeutic approaches (81) and varied by disorder (82).

      Recently, researchers attempted to disclose the neural mechanisms underlying behavioral synchrony during psychotherapy. In a preliminary study, typical students were recruited as clients, and they were required to have interactions with professional counsellors. By using the fNIRS-based hyperscanning technique, researchers recorded the brain activation of both clients and counsellors in the frontal cortex and right temporoparietal junction during the psychological counselling phase and the chatting phase (83). Better working alliances and increased interpersonal brain synchrony in the right temporoparietal junction between clients and counselors were observed during psychological counseling (versus chatting). Such inter-personal brain synchrony was correlated with the bond of the working alliance. This study refines the neural explanation of behavioral synchrony during psychotherapy.

      Briefly, interpersonal body and brain synchrony could play important roles in the processes of psychotherapy. The lack of coordination during psychotherapy may be a risk factor for the condition's recurrence (84). The findings provide insights for psychological interventions for psychiatric disorders.

      Interpersonal Body and Brain Coupling Offer Insights for Psychological Interventions for Psychiatric Disorders Manipulation of Inter-personal Body Synchrony

      Given that synchrony has also been associated with the outcome of psychotherapy, “moving together” could possibly be an efficient means to improve psychiatric patients' social dysfunction. Notably, the idea of using imitation and synchronization in clinical interventions to target social functions has a long tradition in dance/movement therapy in general and in working with children with ASD in particular (8587). Some studies have provided empirical evidence for this notion. For example, a seven-week intervention study focusing on movement mirroring showed that young adults with ASD reported improved well-being, body awareness, self–other distinction, and social skills after the intervention (88). Moreover, patients treated with an interpersonal movement imitation and synchronization intervention showed a significantly larger improvement in emotional inference than those treated with a control movement intervention that focused on individual motor coordination (89).

      Interventions targeting social synchronous behavior on social functions can even positively affect two-year-old toddlers with ASD (90). In the study by Landa et al., toddlers with ASD were randomized to either a classroom-based inter-personal synchrony intervention (including imitation, joint attention, and affect sharing) or a non-inter-personal synchrony intervention. It was found that after approximately 200 hours of interpersonal synchrony interventions (versus non-interpersonal synchrony interventions), toddlers showed enhanced socially engaged imitations paired with eye contact with the examiner and demonstrated a trend toward higher levels of nonverbal cognition during posttest assessments (90). The study provided evidence for plasticity in these developmental systems in toddlers with ASD.

      Notably, the interventions could initially be conducted via a computer-mediated interference. It was reported that individuals with high-functioning autism showed a reduced sensitivity to the other person's responsiveness to one's own behavior when they were required to have real-time sensorimotor interaction with normal individuals; however, they performed equally well as controls under the highly simplified, computer-mediated, embodied form of social interaction. This finding supports the increasing use of virtual reality interfaces to help people with ASD better compensate for their social disabilities (91).

      Manipulation of Inter-personal Brain Synchrony

      Related studies have revealed that interpersonal brain synchrony reflects social dysfunctions and intervention effects to some extent (80, 83). Thus, it could be possible to improve the social communication and interpersonal relationships of patients with psychiatric disorders by manipulating interpersonal brain synchrony.

      To date, some studies have applied transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) to specific brain regions of interacting persons to directly examine the relationship between interpersonal brain synchrony and behavioral synchrony (92, 93). For example, Novembre and colleagues induced beta band (20 Hz) oscillations over the left motor cortex in pairs of individuals who both performed a finger-tapping task with the right hand and found that in-phase 20 Hz stimulation enhanced inter-personal movement synchrony compared with anti-phase or sham stimulation, particularly for the initial taps following the preparatory period. However, in the study of Szymanski et al., both the same-phase-same-frequency and the different-phase-different-frequency conditions were associated with greater dyadic drumming asynchrony relative to the sham condition. The inconsistent findings might be related to the different stimulation protocols and experimental paradigms, which need to be verified.

      Apart from dual-brain stimulation, neurofeedback is also a promising approach to manipulating brain activity. There is growing evidence to support the idea that a single participant's brain activity can be self-regulated with neurofeedback, yielding specific behavioral effects [see (94, 95) for a review]. During the past several decades, the technique of neurofeedback has been applied to patients with psychiatric disorders (e.g., major depressive disorder, personality disorder, and schizophrenia) to relieve psychiatric symptoms [see (96) for a review]. Previous studies have revealed that a neurofeedback tool that tracks human interaction at the neural level has potential clinical applications for the diagnosis and treatment of social cognition disorders. For example, persons with autism may respond better to explicit cues via technological interfaces than to human cues (91).

      Challenges Artifacts and Sample Size

      To study the behavioral and neurophysiological mechanisms underlying social interactions, two-person approaches call for highly ecologically valid experimental paradigms. At the methodological level, compared to fMRI and EEG data, fNIRS data offer the advantage of capturing brain activity in realistic social situations while being less affected by motor artifacts (97). Some video-based techniques or peripheral devices restrain participants even less (e.g., motion energy analysis, actigraphy).

      However, high ecological validity comes at the cost of having unavoidable artifacts. The (neural) signal is potentially contaminated by at least two factors. First, motion artifacts are likely to be generated in unconstrained environments (98, 99), affecting the reliability of data. Second, spontaneous systemic effects from neural activity or peripheral physiological fluctuations may affect signal quality (100). These artifacts could be mitigated by several artifact correction methods. However, good post hoc data processing is never better than good data collection. Future studies should simultaneously consider the ecological validity and motion/systemic artifacts when applying two-person approaches.

      A common issue in two-person neuroscientific research relates to the sample size. “Two-person” is a fancy technique, but it also means that a larger sample size would be required. For example, 60 participants indicate a sample size of 60 for single-person studies but only half for two-person studies in many cases (i.e., 60 participants would lead to a sample size of 30 for two-person studies; and an even smaller sample size for multi-person studies). One is encouraged to define the sample size prior to formal experiments by conducting statistical power analyses, e.g., using the G*power toolbox (101). Future studies are needed to consolidate the previous findings from two-person studies by enlarging the sample size to increase statistical power.

      Statistical Methods of Assessing Synchrony

      There are several techniques that estimate the covariance or directional coupling of time series generated from two interacting partners in previous studies. These include the phase locking value [(53); see also (3, 54)], wavelet transform coherence [(102); see also (4, 13)], windowed cross-correlation [(52); see also (33, 51)], and Granger causality analysis [(103); see also (5, 32, 45)].

      However, when evaluating data analyses for two-person data, there is currently no uniform analytical pipeline (i.e., statistical methods for assessing synchrony). Different two-person studies adopt distinct analysis strategies (e.g., with vs. without filtering), making the findings less congruent. In an endeavor to achieve transparency, consistency, and repeatability, future research should reach a consensus on common analysis guidelines. This practice will largely facilitate the replication of findings and their interpretations. Recent advances have seen some efforts in this direction [e.g., (104)].

      Indeed, different methods aim at addressing distinctive research questions. For example, interpersonal phase synchronization between two participants in a dyad could be addressed using phase locking value or wavelet transform coherence methods, whereas the former was commonly used in behavioral and EEG studies and the latter was widely used in fNIRS research. Windowed cross-correlation not only provides information regarding simultaneous synchrony but can also reveal lagged information (i.e., which time series is leading the other). Granger causality also provides suggestions for coupling directionality, in case one is interested in exploring the direction of information flow between individuals. Researchers should utilize suitable statistical methods for assessing synchrony, targeting specific research aims.

      Clinical Translation Application

      Current two-person studies are still facing technical and methodological challenges, making the findings difficult to interpret and controversial for direct clinical translation. In addition, considering the limited sample size in previous two-person studies, it is rather inappropriate to generalize the laboratory findings to real-life psychiatric applications. Notwithstanding, previous studies pave the way for the use of two-person approaches in practical psychiatric settings. For example, it was reported that body-to-body synchrony reflected relationship quality and outcomes in psychotherapy (33). This implies that interpersonal markers could be potential tools to aid diagnostic procedures. Additionally, dual brain stimulation was reported to foster behavioral coordination and improve social interactions (92, 93). This is relevant for complementary treatments for social disorders, such as autism.

      Conclusions and Future Perspectives

      In sum, two-person approaches are promising tools for studying social interaction, particularly in the field of psychiatry. Although still facing several methodological and translational challenges, the two-person approach has its benefits compared to the conventional single-person approach when studying dynamic social interactions. The use of two-person approaches in psychiatry facilitates advancements in our understanding of the mechanism of atypical social interaction in the fields of autism, mood disorders, schizophrenia, borderline personality disorder, and psychotherapy. Two-person approaches also show promise in clinical interventions when combined with brain stimulation and neurofeedback techniques.

      A future direction is to integrate the two-person approaches with computational modeling techniques (105), which may help further empower a better understanding of the computational mechanism of social interaction in psychiatry. Another direction is to manipulate the situations during social interaction using virtual reality (106) and test the effects of social factors, such as interpersonal distances and angles on eye-to-eye, body-to-body, and brain-to-brain communications. Eventually, as psychiatric disorders are strongly influenced by genetic factors (107), future studies are also encouraged to combine two-person approaches and multivariate genetic models.

      Author Contributions

      YP and XC wrote the manuscript.

      Funding

      Support was provided by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31800951), the Natural Science Foundation of Guangdong (2018A030310431), the Foundation for Distinguished Young Talents in Higher Education of Guangdong (2017KQNCX172), and the Natural Science Foundation of SZU (2018053).

      Conflict of Interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      References Cheng X Pan Y Hu Y Hu Y . Coordination Elicits Synchronous Brain Activity Between Co-actors: Frequency Ratio Matters. Front Neurosci (2019) 13:110. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.01071 Hu Y Hu Y Li X Pan Y Cheng X . Brain-to-brain synchronization across two persons predicts mutual prosociality. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci (2017) 12:1835–44. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx118 Hu Y Pan Y Shi X Cai Q Li X Cheng X . Inter-brain synchrony and cooperation context in interactive decision making. Biol Psychol (2018) 133:5462. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.12.005 Pan Y Cheng X Zhang Z Li X Hu Y . Cooperation in lovers: An fNIRS-based hyperscanning study. Hum Brain Mapp (2017) 38:831–41. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23421 Pan Y Novembre G Song B Li X Hu Y . Interpersonal synchronization of inferior frontal cortices tracks social interactive learning of a song. Neuroimage (2018) 183:280–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.005 Pan Y Dikker S Goldstein P Zhu Y Yang C Hu Y . Instructor-learner brain coupling discriminates between instructional approaches and predicts learning. Neuroimage (2020) 211:116657. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116657 Babiloni F Astolfi L . Social neuroscience and hyperscanning techniques: Past, present and future. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2014) 44:7693. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.07.006 Hasson U Ghazanfar AA Galantucci B Garrod S Keysers C . Brain-to-brain coupling: A mechanism for creating and sharing a social world. Trends Cognit Sci (2012) 16:114–21. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.007 Redcay E Schilbach L . Using second-person neuroscience to elucidate the mechanisms of social interaction. Nat Rev Neurosci (2019) 20:495505. doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0179-4 Schilbach L . Eye to eye, face to face and brain to brain: Novel approaches to study the behavioral dynamics and neural mechanisms of social interactions. Curr Opin Behav Sci (2015) 3:130–5. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.03.006 Schilbach L Timmermans B Reddy V Costall A Bente G Schlicht T . Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behav Brain Sci (2013) 36:393414. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12000660 Babiloni F Cincotti F Mattia D Mattiocco M De Fallani FV Tocci A . Hypermethods for EEG hyperscanning. Annu Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol - Proc (2006) 1:3666–9. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2006.260754 Cui X Bryant DM Reiss AL . NIRS-based hyperscanning reveals increased interpersonal coherence in superior frontal cortex during cooperation. Neuroimage (2012) 59:2430–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.003 Montague PR Berns GS Cohen JD McClure SM Pagnoni G Dhamala M . Hyperscanning: Simultaneous fMRI during linked social interactions. Neuroimage (2002) 16:1159–64. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1150 Leong V Schilbach L . The promise of two-person neuroscience for developmental psychiatry: Using interaction-based sociometrics to identify disorders of social interaction. Br J Psychiatry (2019) 215:636–8. doi: 10.1192/bjp.2019.73 Dikker S Wan L Davidesco I Kaggen L Oostrik M McClintock J . Brain-to-Brain Synchrony Tracks Real-World Dynamic Group Interactions in the Classroom. Curr Biol (2017) 27:1375–80. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.002 Parkinson C Kleinbaum AM Wheatley T . Similar neural responses predict friendship. Nat Commun (2018) 9:322. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-02722-7 Simony E Honey CJ Chen J Lositsky O Yeshurun Y Wiesel A . Dynamic reconfiguration of the default mode network during narrative comprehension. Nat Commun (2016) 7:12141. doi: 10.1038/ncomms12141 Balconi M Pezard L Nandrino JL Vanutelli ME . Two is better than one: The effects of strategic cooperation on intra- and inter-brain connectivity by fNIRS. PloS One (2017) 12:e0187652. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0187652 Frith CD . The social brain? Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci (2007) 362:671–8. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.2003 Lieberman MD . Social Cognitive Neuroscience: A Review of Core Processes. Annu Rev Psychol (2007) 58:259–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085654 Lindström B Golkar A Jangard S Tobler PN Olsson A . Social threat learning transfers to decision making in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2019) 116:4732–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1810180116 De Jaegher H Di Paolo E Gallagher S . Can social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends Cognit Sci (2010) 14:441–7. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.009 Konvalinka I Roepstorff A . The two-brain approach: How can mutually interacting brains teach us something about social interaction? Front Hum Neurosci (2012) 6:110. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00215 Pfeiffer UJ Vogeley K Schilbach L . From gaze cueing to dual eye-tracking: Novel approaches to investigate the neural correlates of gaze in social interaction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2013) 37:2516–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.07.017 Wilms M Schilbach L Pfeiffer U Bente G Fink GR Vogeley K . It's in your eyes-using gaze-contingent stimuli to create truly interactive paradigms for social cognitive and affective neuroscience. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci (2010) 5:98107. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq024 Redcay E Kleiner M Saxe R . Look at this: The neural correlates of initiating and responding to bids for joint attention. Front Hum Neurosci (2012) 6:114. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00169 Saito DN Tanabe HC Izuma K Hayashi MJ Morito Y Komeda H . Stay tuned: Inter-individual neural synchronization during mutual gaze and joint attention. Front Integr Neurosci (2010) 4:112. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2010.00127 Lachat F Hugueville L Lemaréchal JD Conty L George N . Oscillatory brain correlates of live joint attention: A dual-EEG study. Front Hum Neurosci (2012) 6:112. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00156 Hirsch J Zhang X Noah JA Ono Y . Frontal temporal and parietal systems synchronize within and across brains during live eye-to-eye contact. Neuroimage (2017) 157:314–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.018 Yun K Watanabe K Shimojo S . Interpersonal body and neural synchronization as a marker of implicit social interaction. Sci Rep (2012) 2:18. doi: 10.1038/srep00959 Chang A Livingstone SR Bosnyak DJ Trainor LJ . Body sway reflects leadership in joint music performance. Proc Natl Acad Sci (2017) 114:E4134–41. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1617657114 Ramseyer F Tschacher W . Nonverbal synchrony in psychotherapy: Coordinated body movement reflects relationship quality and outcome. J Consult Clin Psychol (2011) 79:284–95. doi: 10.1037/a0023419 Baimel A Birch SAJ Norenzayan A . Coordinating bodies and minds: Behavioral synchrony fosters mentalizing. J Exp Soc Psychol (2018) 74:281–90. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.10.008 Sharon-David H Mizrahi M Rinott M Golland Y Birnbaum GE . Being on the same wavelength: Behavioral synchrony between partners and its influence on the experience of intimacy. J Soc Pers Relat (2019) 36:29833008. doi: 10.1177/0265407518809478 Palumbo RV Marraccini ME Weyandt LL Wilder-Smith O McGee HA Liu S . Interpersonal Autonomic Physiology: A Systematic Review of the Literature. Pers Soc Psychol Rev (2017) 21:99141. doi: 10.1177/1088868316628405 Vanutelli ME Gatti L Angioletti L Balconi M . Affective Synchrony and Autonomic Coupling during Cooperation: A Hyperscanning Study. BioMed Res Int (2017) 2017:3104564. doi: 10.1155/2017/3104564 Müller V Delius JAM Lindenberger U . Complex networks emerging during choir singing. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2018) 1431:85101. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13940 Chatel-Goldman J Congedo M Jutten C Schwartz J-L . Touch increases autonomic coupling between romantic partners. Front Behav Neurosci (2014) 8:95. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00095 Kleinbub JR . State of the Art of Interpersonal Physiology in Psychotherapy: A Systematic Review. Front Psychol (2017) 8:2053. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02053 Koole SL Tschacher W . Synchrony in Psychotherapy: A Review and an Integrative Framework for the Therapeutic Alliance. Front Psychol (2016) 7:862. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00862 Tschacher W Meier D . Physiological synchrony in psychotherapy sessions. Psychother Res (2019) 6:116. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2019.1612114 Dai B Chen C Long Y Zheng L Zhao H Bai X . Neural mechanisms for selectively tuning in to the target speaker in a naturalistic noisy situation. Nat Commun (2018) 9:2405. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-04819-z Jiang J Dai B Peng D Zhu C Liu L Lu C . Neural synchronization during face-to-face communication. J Neurosci (2012) 32:16064–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2926-12.2012 Jiang J Chen C Dai B Shi G Ding G Liu L . Leader emergence through interpersonal neural synchronization. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2015) 112:4274–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1422930112 Bilek E Ruf M Schäfer A Akdeniz C Calhoun VD Schmahl C . Information flow between interacting human brains: Identification, validation, and relationship to social expertise. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2015) 112:5207–12. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1421831112 Goelman G Dan R Stößel G Tost H Meyer-Lindenberg A Bilek E . Bidirectional signal exchanges and their mechanisms during joint attention interaction – A hyperscanning fMRI study. Neuroimage (2019) 198:242–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.05.028 Oberwelland E Schilbach L Barisic I Krall SC Vogeley K Fink GR . Look into my eyes: Investigating joint attention using interactive eye-tracking and fMRI in a developmental sample. Neuroimage (2016) 130:248–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.026 Leong V Byrne E Clackson K Georgieva S Lam S Wass S . Speaker gaze increases information coupling between infant and adult brains. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2017) 114:13290–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1702493114 Schilbach L . Towards a second-person neuropsychiatry. Philos T R Soc B (2015) 371:20150081. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0081 Kupper Z Ramseyer F Hoffmann H Tschacher W . Nonverbal Synchrony in Social Interactions of Patients with Schizophrenia Indicates Socio-Communicative Deficits. PloS One (2015) 10:e0145882. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0145882 Boker SM Rotondo JL Xu M King K . Windowed cross-correlation and peak picking for the analysis of variability in the association between behavioral time series. Psychol Methods (2002) 7:338–55. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.3.338 Lachaux JP Rodriguez E Martinerie J Varela FJ . Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Hum Brain Mapp (1999) 8:194208. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4<194::AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-C Dumas G Nadel J Soussignan R Martinerie J Garnero L . Inter-brain synchronization during social interaction. PloS One (2010) 5:e12166. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012166 Goldstein P Weissman-Fogel I Dumas G Shamay-Tsoory SG . Brain-to-brain coupling during handholding is associated with pain reduction. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2018) 115:E2528–37. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1703643115 Jammalamadaka SR SenGupta A . Topics in Circular Statistics. (2001), (River Edge, NJ: World Scientific). doi: 10.1142/9789812779267 Pérez A Dumas G Karadag M Duñabeitia JA . Differential brain-to-brain entrainment while speaking and listening in native and foreign languages. Cortex (2019) 111:303–15. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.11.026 Llobera J Charbonnier C Chagué S Preissmann D Antonietti J-P Ansermet F . The Subjective Sensation of Synchrony: An Experimental Study. PloS One (2016) 11:e0147008. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0147008 McCall C Singer T . Facing Off with Unfair Others: Introducing Proxemic Imaging as an Implicit Measure of Approach and Avoidance during Social Interaction. PloS One (2015) 10:e0117532. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0117532 Nozaradan S Peretz I Keller PE . Individual Differences in Rhythmic Cortical Entrainment Correlate with Predictive Behavior in Sensorimotor Synchronization. Sci Rep (2016) 6:20612. doi: 10.1038/srep20612 American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association (2013). Bruinsma Y Koegel RL Koegel LK . Joint attention and children with autism: A review of the literature. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev (2004) 10:169–75. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20036 Nakano T Kato N Kitazawa S . Lack of eyeblink entrainments in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia (2011) 49:2784–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.06.007 Oberwelland E Schilbach L Barisic I Krall SC Vogeley K Fink GR . Young adolescents with autism show abnormal joint attention network: A gaze contingent fMRI study. NeuroImage Clin (2017) 14:112–21. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.01.006 Koehne S Hatri A Cacioppo JT Dziobek I . Perceived interpersonal synchrony increases empathy: Insights from autism spectrum disorder. Cognition (2016) 146:815. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.007 Curioni A Minio-Paluello I Sacheli LM Candidi M Aglioti SM . Autistic traits affect interpersonal motor coordination by modulating strategic use of role-based behavior. Mol Autism (2017) 8:113. doi: 10.1186/s13229-017-0141-0 Wang Q Han Z Hu X Feng S Wang H Liu T . Autism Symptoms Modulate Interpersonal Neural Synchronization in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder in Cooperative Interactions. Brain Topogr (2019) 33:112–22. doi: 10.1007/s10548-019-00731-x Mundy P Sigman M Kasari C . A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. J Autism Dev Disord (1990) 20:115–28. doi: 10.1007/BF02206861 Charman T . Why is joint attention a pivotal skill in autism? Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci (2003) 358:315–24. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1199 Schilbach L Wilms M Eickhoff SB Romanzetti S Tepest R Bente G . Minds made for sharing: Initiating joint attention recruits reward-related neurocircuitry. J Cognit Neurosci (2010) 22:2702–15. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21401 Redcay E Dodell-Feder D Mavros PL Kleiner M Pearrow MJ Triantafyllou C . Atypical brain activation patterns during a face-to-face joint attention game in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Hum Brain Mapp (2013) 34:2511–23. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22086 Tanabe HC Kosaka H Saito DN Koike T Hayashi MJ Izuma K . Hard to “tune in”: Neural mechanisms of live face-to-face interaction with high-functioning autistic spectrum disorder. Front Hum Neurosci (2012) 6:115. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00268 Mukai K Miura A Kudo K Tsutsui S . The effect of pairing individuals with different social skills on interpersonal motor coordination. Front Psychol (2018) 9:1708. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01708 Savitz J Drevets WC . Bipolar and major depressive disorder: Neuroimaging the developmental-degenerative divide. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2009) 33:699771. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.01.004 Takei Y Suda M Aoyama Y Sakurai N Tagawa M Motegi T . Near-infrared spectroscopic study of frontopolar activation during face-to-face conversation in major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder. J Psychiatr Res (2014) 57:7483. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.06.009 Hooker CI Bruce L Lincoln SH Fisher M Vinogradov S . Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2011) 70:1169–78. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.07.027 Benedetti F Bernasconi A Bosia M Cavallaro R Dallaspezia S Falini A . Functional and structural brain correlates of theory of mind and empathy deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2009) 114:154–60. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.06.021 Takei Y Suda M Aoyama Y Yamaguchi M Sakurai N Narita K . Temporal lobe and inferior frontal gyrus dysfunction in patients with schizophrenia during face-to-face conversation : A near-infrared spectroscopy study. J Psychiatr Res (2013) 47:1581–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.07.029 Stegmayer K Bohlhalter S Vanbellingen T Federspiel A Wiest R Müri RM . Limbic Interference During Social Action Planning in Schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull (2018) 44:359–68. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx059 Bilek E Stößel G Schäfer A Clement L Ruf M Robnik L . State-dependent cross-brain information flow in borderline personality disorder. JAMA Psychiatry (2017) 74:949–57. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.1682 Altmann U Schoenherr D Paulick J Deisenhofer AK Schwartz B Rubel JA . Associations between movement synchrony and outcome in patients with social anxiety disorder: Evidence for treatment specific effects. Psychother Res (2019) 0:117. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2019.1630779 Paulick J Rubel JA Deisenhofer AK Schwartz B Thielemann D Altmann U . Diagnostic Features of Nonverbal Synchrony in Psychotherapy: Comparing Depression and Anxiety. Cognit Ther Res (2018) 42:539–51. doi: 10.1007/s10608-018-9914-9 Zhang Y Meng T Hou Y Pan Y Hu Y . Interpersonal brain synchronization associated with working alliance during psychological counseling. Psychiatry Res - Neuroimaging (2018) 282:103–9. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2018.09.007 Bouhuys AL Sam MM . Lack of coordination of nonverbal behaviour between patients and interviewers as a potential risk factor to depression recurrence: Vulnerability accumulation in depression. J Affect Disord (2000) 57:189200. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(99)00093-2 Behrends A Muller S Dziobek I . Moving in and out of synchrony: A concept for a new intervention fostering empathy through interactional movement and dance. Arts Psychother (2012) 39:107–16. doi: 10.1016/j.aip.2012.02.003 Scharoun SM Reinders NJ Bryden PJ Fletcher PC . Dance/Movement Therapy as an Intervention for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Am J Danc Ther (2014) 36:209–28. doi: 10.1007/s10465-014-9179-0 Samaritter R Payne H . Kinaesthetic intersubjectivity: A dance informed contribution to self-other relatedness and shared experience in non-verbal psychotherapy with an example from autism. Arts Psychother (2013) 40:143–50. doi: 10.1016/j.aip.2012.12.004 Koch SC Mehl L Sobanski E Sieber M Fuchs T . Fixing the mirrors: A feasibility study of the effects of dance movement therapy on young adults with autism spectrum disorder. Autism (2015) 19:338–50. doi: 10.1177/1362361314522353 Koehne S Behrends A Fairhurst MT Dziobek I . Fostering Social Cognition through an Imitation- and Synchronization-Based Dance/Movement Intervention in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Controlled Proof-of-Concept Study. Psychother Psychosom (2016) 85:2735. doi: 10.1159/000441111 Landa RJ Holman KC O'Neill AH Stuart EA . Intervention targeting development of socially synchronous engagement in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder: A randomized controlled trial. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip (2011) 52:1321. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02288.x Zapata-Fonseca L Froese T Schilbach L Vogeley K Timmermans B . Sensitivity to social contingency in adults with high-functioning autism during computer-mediated embodied interaction. Behav Sci (Basel) (2018) 8:22. doi: 10.3390/bs8020022 Novembre G Knoblich G Dunne L Keller PE . Interpersonal synchrony enhanced through 20 Hz phase-coupled dual brain stimulation. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci (2017) 12:662–70. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw172 Szymanski C Müller V Brick TR von Oertzen T Lindenberger U . Hyper-transcranial alternating current stimulation: Experimental manipulation of inter-brain synchrony. Front Hum Neurosci (2017) 11:115. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00539 Caria A Sitaram R Birbaumer N . Real-time fMRI: A tool for local brain regulation. Neuroscientist (2012) 18:487501. doi: 10.1177/1073858411407205 Weiskopf N . Real-time fMRI and its application to neurofeedback. Neuroimage (2012) 62:682–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.009 Fovet T Jardri R Linden D . Current Issues in the Use of fMRI-Based Neurofeedback to Relieve Psychiatric Symptoms. Curr Pharm Des (2015) 21:3384–94. doi: 10.2174/1381612821666150619092540 Pan Y Borragán G Peigneux P . Applications of Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy in Fatigue, Sleep Deprivation, and Social Cognition. Brain Topogr (2019) 32:9981012. doi: 10.1007/s10548-019-00740-w Brigadoi S Ceccherini L Cutini S Scarpa F Scatturin P Selb J . Motion artifacts in functional near-infrared spectroscopy: A comparison of motion correction techniques applied to real cognitive data. Neuroimage (2014) 85:181–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.082 Cui X Baker JM Liu N Reiss AL . Sensitivity of fNIRS measurement to head motion: An applied use of smartphones in the lab. J Neurosci Methods (2015) 245:3743. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.02.006 Zhang X Noah JA Hirsch J . Separation of the global and local components in functional near-infrared spectroscopy signals using principal component spatial filtering. Neurophotonics (2016) 3:015004. doi: 10.1117/1.nph.3.1.015004 Erdfelder E FAul F Buchner A Lang AG . Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods (2009) 41:1149–60. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 Grinsted A Moore JC Jevrejeva S . Application of the cross wavelet transform and wavelet coherence to geophysical time series. Nonlinear Process Geophys (2004) 11:561–6. doi: 10.5194/npg-11-561-2004 Seth AK Barrett AB Barnett L . Granger causality analysis in neuroscience and neuroimaging. J Neurosci (2015) 35:3293–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4399-14.2015 Nastase SA Gazzola V Hasson U Keysers C . Measuring shared responses across subjects using intersubject correlation. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci (2019) 14:669–87. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz037 Bolis D Schilbach L . Beyond one Bayesian brain: Modeling intra-and inter-personal processes during social interaction: Commentary on “mentalizing homeostasis: The social origins of interoceptive inference” by Fotopoulou & Tsakiris. Neuropsychoanalysis (2017) 19:35–8. doi: 10.1080/15294145.2017.1295215 Zhou C Han M Liang Q Hu YF Kuai SG . A social interaction field model accurately identifies static and dynamic social groupings. Nat Hum Behav (2019) 3:847–55. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0618-2 Faraone SV Doyle AE . Genetic influences on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep (2000) 2:143–6. doi: 10.1007/s11920-000-0059-6
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016www.khzrgz.com.cn
      www.jxylw888.org.cn
      jjyygo.com.cn
      itjwph.com.cn
      laowuu.com.cn
      tbchain.com.cn
      www.sxpic.com.cn
      uxbwpb.com.cn
      sudxtn.com.cn
      ouluofen.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p