Front. Neurosci. Frontiers in Neuroscience Front. Neurosci. 1662-453X Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fnins.2022.1041397 Neuroscience Original Research Chronology of auditory processing and related co-activation in the orbitofrontal cortex depends on musical expertise Bücher Steffen 1 Bernhofs Valdis 2 Thieme Andrea 1 Christiner Markus 2 3 * Schneider Peter 1 2 3 4 * 1Section of Biomagnetism Heidelberg, Department of Neurology, Faculty of Medicine Heidelberg, Heidelberg, Germany 2Jāzeps Vītols Latvian Academy of Music, Riga, Latvia 3Centre of Systematic Musicology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria 4Department of Neuroradiology, Medical School Heidelberg, Heidelberg, Germany

Edited by: Mari Tervaniemi, CICERO Learning, Faculty of Educational Sciences, University of Helsinki, Finland

Reviewed by: Bernhard Ross, University of Toronto, Canada; Chan Hee Kim, Seoul National University, Republic of Korea

*Correspondence: Markus Christiner, markus.christiner@uni-graz.at Peter Schneider, schneider@musicandbrain.de

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share senior authorship

This article was submitted to Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience, a section of the journal Frontiers in Neuroscience

04 01 2023 2022 16 1041397 10 09 2022 02 12 2022 Copyright © 2023 Bücher, Bernhofs, Thieme, Christiner and Schneider. 2023 Bücher, Bernhofs, Thieme, Christiner and Schneider

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Introduction

The present study aims to explore the extent to which auditory processing is reflected in the prefrontal cortex.

Methods

Using magnetoencephalography (MEG), we investigated the chronology of primary and secondary auditory responses and associated co-activation in the orbitofrontal cortex in a large cohort of 162 participants of various ages. The sample consisted of 38 primary school children, 39 adolescents, 43 younger, and 42 middle-aged adults and was further divided into musically experienced participants and non-musicians by quantifying musical training and aptitude parameters.

Results

We observed that the co-activation in the orbitofrontal cortex [Brodmann-Area 10 (BA10)] strongly depended on musical expertise but not on age. In the musically experienced groups, a systematic coincidence of peak latencies of the primary auditory P1 response and the co-activated response in the orbitofrontal cortex was observed in childhood at the onset of musical education. In marked contrast, in all non-musicians, the orbitofrontal co-activation occurred 25–40 ms later when compared with the P1 response. Musical practice and musical aptitude contributed equally to the observed activation and co-activation patterns in the auditory and orbitofrontal cortex, confirming the reciprocal, interrelated influence of nature, and nurture in the musical brain.

Discussion

Based on the observed ageindependent differences in the chronology and lateralization of neurological responses, we suggest that orbitofrontal functions may contribute to musical learning at an early age.

auditory evoked fields auditory cortex musical aptitude audiation orbitofrontal cortex BA10 chronology

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      The chronology and lateralization of sound processing contribute significantly to the understanding of the processing of auditory stimuli in the brain. There is ample evidence that the temporal hierarchy and the interactions between the right- and left-sided auditory pathways significantly determine the circuits between the peripheral to the cortical level (Tervaniemi and Hugdahl, 2003; Eggermont and Moore, 2012), pointing out that the left hemisphere is specialized for temporal processing, whereas the right hemisphere subserves processes domiciled in the spatial/spectral domain (Zatorre and Belin, 2001; Poeppel, 2003; Boemio et al., 2005; Schönwiesner et al., 2005). The human auditory cortex is subdivided into three main parts with multiple interconnections: the core (primary auditory cortex), the belt (secondary auditory cortex), and the parabelt region (Hackett et al., 1998; Rauschecker and Scott, 2009), which receive their cortical input from afferent subcortical limbic projections (Kraus and Nicol, 2005; Wong et al., 2007; Kraus and Chandrasekaran, 2010; Kraus and Anderson, 2014; Kraus et al., 2017) and efferent top-down projections from higher cognitive levels and transcallosal connections (Zatorre et al., 2007; Rauschecker and Scott, 2009).

      The musical brain is an excellent model to show the neuroplasticity of auditory processing (Münte et al., 2002; Wan and Schlaug, 2010). Active music making involves numerous neural processes that have a great long-term impact on perception, cognition, behavior, and brain activity from childhood (Hyde et al., 2009; Moreno et al., 2009; Skoe et al., 2015; Slater et al., 2015; Habibi et al., 2018) to adolescence (Tierney et al., 2015) and to adulthood (Pantev et al., 1998; Herdener et al., 2010; Benner et al., 2017; James et al., 2020). Furthermore, valuable insights are gained in understanding how neural processing is related to musical expertise (referring to both musical aptitude and musical training) and outstanding auditory skills (Zatorre et al., 2007; Kraus and Chandrasekaran, 2010; Zatorre and Salimpoor, 2013; Kraus and Anderson, 2014; Wengenroth et al., 2014). The Heschl gyri (HG) in the center of the auditory cortex are found to have on average 130% more gray matter in professional musicians than in non-musicians (Schneider et al., 2002). Musicians also possess enlarged auditory-evoked response patterns (Schneider et al., 2005; Benner et al., 2017). The primary auditory-evoked response pattern (P1-complex) can be localized by magnetoencephalography (MEG) within the central part of the first HG including an early middle latent P30 and a subsequent P50 response pattern occurring 30 and 50 ms after stimulus onset. The subsequent secondary N1 and tertiary P2 responses of the auditory belt and parabelt areas originate more in the surrounding belt areas of the first HG (Schneider et al., 2005). The P1-N1-P2 complex of late auditory-evoked fields is generally related to elementary sound perception, attentional factors, feature recognition, and especially precognitive processes, but rarely to handedness or gender (Schneider et al., 2022). The latter is known to influence the sensitivity and hemispheric dominance of later event-related potential (ERP) components such as N2a, mismatch negativity, or P3 (Patel and Azzam, 2005; Sur and Sinha, 2009).

      In musically experienced subjects, the latency differences of the right- and left-hemispheric P1, N1, and P2 responses are strikingly smaller than in non-musicians (Seither-Preisler et al., 2014; Groß et al., 2022). The medial part of HG is involved in elementary auditory processing, such as frequency, intensity, or tone duration, whereas the lateral HG and the posterior supratemporal gyrus provide complex auditory processing necessary for selective attention, recognition of musical pitch, rhythm and melody (Schneider et al., 2005), and specific auditory and language-related skills (Wengenroth et al., 2014; Benner et al., 2017; Christiner et al., 2022; Groß et al., 2022).

      There is ample evidence that transfer effects of musical experience also reach non-auditory areas, supported by the existence of dual processing streams [“what” and “where” streams (Milner and Goodale, 2008; Rauschecker, 2012; Kandel et al., 2013, p. 704–705)] that both feed into the prefrontal cortex. This has led to the suggestion that orbitofrontal functions in particular may be critically important across the lifespan from childhood to adulthood (Sala and Gobet, 2020; Chaddock-Heyman et al., 2021), especially in response to pleasurable or aesthetic aspects of musical stimulation (Blood and Zatorre, 2001; Nieminen et al., 2011; Särkämö et al., 2013). The rostral prefrontal cortex (BA10) is the largest cytoarchitectonic area in the human prefrontal cortex (Gilbert et al., 2006). When compared with apes, the rostral prefrontal cortex is thought to play an important role in human thinking because of its large evolutionary expansion and large volume difference (Gilbert et al., 2006). Katherine Semendeferi et al. (2011) suggest that the low cell density of the BA10 is the reason for its detectable strong connectivity with other brain regions. A large number of dendrites in the BA10 is an indication of strong networking tendencies (Semendeferi et al., 2001, 2011; Dumontheil, 2014). BA10 is one of the last brain regions to mature during human development. Therefore, several authors suggest that BA10 is the region in the brain, that is, the last to become myelinated (Fuster, 1997; Jacobsen et al., 2006; Burgess et al., 2007), demonstrating higher speed and accuracy of neuronal responses in the prefrontal cortex within the human maturation process (Dumontheil, 2014).

      The rostrolateral prefrontal cortex (BA10) includes specific functions that are also relevant to musical processing, such as the integration of information (Badre and Wagner, 2004; Gilbert et al., 2006; Wolfensteller and von Cramon, 2011; Dumontheil, 2014), memory retrieval (Gilbert et al., 2006), descending processes (Christoff and Gabrieli, 2000; Koechlin et al., 2003; Ramnani and Owen, 2004; Gilbert et al., 2006; Amati and Shallice, 2007; Dumontheil, 2014), bifurcation, branching, and reallocation of attention (Gilbert et al., 2006), multitasking (Roca et al., 2011), flexible motor timing (Remington et al., 2018), prospective memory (Gilbert et al., 2006), and internalization processes (Gilbert et al., 2006). In particular, a rostral-caudal axis of activated brain areas has been described with the increase of complexity and abstraction and the processing of acute, direct sensory stimuli (Christoff and Gabrieli, 2000; Badre, 2008; Dumontheil, 2014).

      Furthermore, the prefrontal cortex is known to distinguish implicit (relatively automatic and possibly unconscious) and explicit (controlled, conscious, and reflected) assessments (Cunningham et al., 2004). Whereas the amygdala is more involved in the unconscious and automatic evaluation, activity in the medial BA10 and ventrolateral (BA47) prefrontal cortex is greater when a task requires explicit evaluation. BA10 and BA47 are most active in rival, good/bad value judgments (Cunningham et al., 2004). fMRI studies have shown that a combination of frontal and temporal regions, in particular the fronto-medial cortex (BA9,10), bilateral prefrontal regions (BA45 and 47), the left temporal pole, and temporo-parietal junctions, is involved in aesthetic judgments (Jacobsen et al., 2006). Functional lateralization of BA10 is observed in tasks that focus on “problem solving” (Christoff et al., 2003), “maintaining intentions over a delay” (Burgess et al., 2003), “coordinating goals and subgoals” (Koechlin et al., 1999; Braver and Bongiolatti, 2002; Ramnani and Owen, 2004), and “basing responses on information recalled from episodic memory” (Koechlin et al., 2003). The medial part of the BA10 is active during emotional contexts or mentalization processes (Gilbert et al., 2006).

      There is converging evidence that musical training benefits prefrontal cortex performance abilities already in childhood. Musical training increases working memory capacity (Roden et al., 2014; Frischen et al., 2021) and accelerates inhibition processes (Moreno et al., 2011, 2014; Bugos and DeMarie, 2017) or executive functions (Schellenberg, 2011; Linnavalli et al., 2018). Likewise, adult musically trained subjects improved executive functions (Brattico et al., 2009; Virtala et al., 2018) and working memory processing (Trainor et al., 1999; Pallesen et al., 2010; Nikjeh and Lister, 2012).

      Using musicality measures, which focus on rhythmic and tonal discrimination tasks, Gordon investigated the influence of formal musical training and dispositional aspects of musical aptitude (Gordon, 1989, 1998, 2011). In contrast to the basic auditory perception, Gordon coins the concept of Audiation, which should be the basis for both, musical aptitude and achievement. The term “audiation” is used to describe a process in which music is comprehended in the mind some time ago and refers to past musical experiences (Gordon, 1997, 1998; Sieroka, 2005). Gordon’s concept of “audiation” requires the conscious handling of what has been learned and experienced, in acquisitional processes, such as reading, performing, writing, or interpreting, which increases with musical experience. Consequently, his notion of abstract sound perception points to higher cognitive processing of music and, in any case, requires working memory capacity as a reservoir of experience to imitate, retain, recall, and anticipate music (Gordon, 1998), as controlled by prefrontal functions. For his musical aptitude tests [Intermediate Measures of Music Audiation (IMMA), Advanced Measures of Music Audiation (AMMA) see section “Materials and methods”], Gordon claimed that the age-matched test scores of musical aptitude represent the innate potential to learn music and remain stable from an age of 9 years (Gordon, 1998; Sieroka, 2005).

      Our goal was to investigate the extent to which primary and secondary auditory processing is reflected in orbitofrontal (prefrontal) co-activations. In addition, we sought to uncover differences regarding age, musical training, and musical aptitude. Finally, we were interested in investigating whether musical experience promotes coherences between auditory-evoked responses and orbitofrontal co-activations in the BA10 area. For this purpose, we recruited 162 participants (musicians and non-musicians) of different ages to measure auditory-evoked fields (AEFs) when listening to instrumental sounds, as well as to assess musical training parameters and aptitude.

      Materials and methods Subjects

      In this study, 162 subjects without any neurological, auditory, or developmental disorders were included in four age groups: 38 primary school children (7–11 years), 39 adolescents (12–17 years), 43 young adults (18–29 years), and 42 middle-aged adults (30–67 years). Furthermore, each of the age groups has been subdivided into musically experienced and musically non-experienced subjects according to the cumulative amount of musical practice, referred to as “musicians” and “non-musicians” in Tables 1, 2.

      Demographic data age groups.

      Age group Sex Handedness Musical status Age Total number of participants
      Male Female Right Left Mus Non Mean SD
      Children 21 17 34 4 18 20 8.2 ± 0.9 38
      Adolescents 20 19 34 5 20 19 14.7 ± 2.0 39
      Young adults 20 23 39 4 19 24 25 ± 4.6 43
      Middle-aged adults 21 21 37 5 23 19 46.3 ± 8.8 42

      Demographic data musicians vs. non-musicians.

      Age group Sex Handedness
      Mus Non Mus Non
      Female Male Female Male Right Left Right Left
      Children 8 10 9 11 16 2 18 2
      Adolescents 11 8 8 12 17 2 17 3
      Young adults 9 10 14 10 18 1 21 3
      Middle-aged adults 10 13 11 8 20 3 17 2

      All subjects participated voluntarily in the study and were informed about the procedure and risks. The subjects were also informed of their right to discontinue the examinations. All subjects gave their written consent to participate in the study (for participants younger than 18 years of age, consent was obtained from a legal guardian).

      Musical training and aptitude

      To assess participants’ musical practice, a cumulative index of musical practice (IMP) was calculated according to the formula I=MPpyphp+jyjhj hours per week × years, combining participants’ data on the number of years of formal music training in school (j) and the amount of time spent practicing in private time (p) (compare Seither-Preisler et al., 2014; Serrallach et al., 2016). The IMP has already been determined in several previous studies for children and adolescents. On this basis, children with high and low musical practice could be distinguished at a value of IMP = 2.5 (Seither-Preisler et al., 2014) and adolescents at a value of IMP = 4.0 (Serrallach et al., 2016). In the present study, the same method was used to set a value of IMP = 50 for young adults and IMP = 100 for middle-aged adults, taking into account the considerably higher cumulative musical training of experienced adults (Benner et al., 2017). In this respect, values above the cutoff value represent musically experienced participants, while values below the cutoff value represent non-musicians.

      To assess musical aptitude in children, adolescents, and adults, we used the IMMA and AMMA tests developed by Gordon (1986, 1989, 1998, 2011). The IMMA/AMMA consists of 30 pairs of fictitious short melodies played on the piano and includes a tonal and a rhythmic subtest (Gordon, 1997). Each presented melody is immediately repeated, with the first melody serving as a reference, while the second melody may be tonal, rhythmic, or not altered at all. The subjects were instructed to compare each pair of melodies and identify the feature of each in a three-way forced choice task. The “raw tonal” and “raw rhythmic” test scores were calculated separately by evaluating the number of correct responses minus the number of incorrect responses plus a standardized base value of 20. The value for random selection is 20, and the highest score achievable is 40 points. In the previous work using the IMMA and AMMA tests, subjects with low musical experience typically scored between 15 and 27 points, whereas subjects with high musical experience scored between 25 and 40 points (Schneider et al., 2002; Seither-Preisler et al., 2014; Wengenroth et al., 2014). Here, we calculated an age-appropriate percentage score based on the raw scores according to Gordon’s reference values (Gordon, 1986, 1989).

      The musicians of all four age groups scored higher in the IMMA/AMMA score (children: M = 70.7 ± 4.2; adolescents: M = 72.3 ± 3.7; young adults: M = 80.4 ± 2.8; middle-aged adults: M = 81.4 ± 3.3) than the non-musicians (children: M = 39.8 ± 5.1; adolescents: M = 47.8 ± 3.2; young adults: M = 37.2 ± 3.6; middle-aged adults: M = 37.6 ± 5.2, Figure 1A). All differences were statistically significant (p ≤ 0.01) (two-tailed).

      Musical aptitude and cumulative amount of musical training (A) group-averaged perceptual score of the IMMA/AMMA-tests, (B) group-averaged IMP values. The bold solid lines (circles) depict the values of the musicians, the dashed lines (triangles) those of the non-musicians. C, children; A, adolescents; Y, young adults; M, middle-aged adults. “Mus” represent the musicians, while “Non” illustrates the non-musicians.

      The musicians of all four age groups also scored higher in the cumulative amount of musical training (children: M = 7.8 ± 1.2; adolescents: M = 30.5 ± 7.7; young adults: M = 147.6 ± 20.3; middle-aged adults: M = 491.1 ± 45.7) than the non-musicians (children: M = 2.4 ± 0.6; adolescents: M = 8.3 ± 1.2; young adults: M = 9.7 ± 3.6; middle-aged adults: M = 27.9 ± 6.1, Figure 1B). All differences were statistically significant (p ≤ 0.01) (two-tailed).

      Auditory stimulation to measure individual auditory-evoked fields

      Auditory-evoked fields were recorded with a Neuromag 122-channel whole-head MEG (Neuromag, Helsinki, Finland) at the Section of Biomagnetism, Department of Neurology in Heidelberg. We used the same acoustic stimulation protocol with seven different sampled instrument sounds (piano, guitar, flute, bass clarinet, trumpet, violin, and percussion) and four artificial simple harmonic complex tones in all subjects because of comparability. This protocol has been applied in the identical form in previous studies with children (Seither-Preisler et al., 2014), adolescents (Wengenroth et al., 2010; Serrallach et al., 2016; Christiner et al., 2022; Groß et al., 2022; Schneider et al., 2022), and adults (Schneider et al., 2005; Wengenroth et al., 2014). One was that our findings can be compared with previous investigations, which represents an important advantage. The other was that we assessed multiple times the nature of the stimulus design. Since the stimuli are comprised of a variety of different timbres and pitches, these sounds do not only occur in the musical context but also partially in everyday sounds. Each sound was presented 200 times in pseudorandomized order as performed in earlier studies (Schneider et al., 2005; Seither-Preisler et al., 2014; Wengenroth et al., 2014; Serrallach et al., 2016). All stimuli had the same length (500 ms). In addition, a fixed, superimposed onset and offset ramp of 10 ms was used for the entire stimuli, on the one hand, to avoid click noises that are audible in the case of short onset times (<5 ms) and, on the other hand, to exclude delayed P1 latencies in response to longer, soft onset ramps. The interstimulus intervals were pseudorandomized in the time range of 300–400 ms to exclude the superposition of external oscillations in the averaged signal. This set of stimuli is known to evoke the primary auditory P1 response occurring about 50–100 ms after tone onset. It is followed by the N1 complex peaking around 95–180 ms and the P2 response peaking around 190–270 ms after tone onset. The stimuli were presented binaurally via 90-cm plastic tubes through foam earpieces placed in the ear canal and connected to small shielded transducers that were fixed in boxes next to the subject’s chair. The intensity of the stimulation was adjusted from the output of the foam pieces to 70 ± 2 dB SPL as determined by a Brüel and Kjaer artificial ear (type 4152) with an additional 2cc coupler as available kindly through our ENT department.

      Acquisition of AEFs

      Prior to measurement, four reference coils were attached to the subject’s head (left and right temples and left and right mastoid) with skin-friendly adhesive tapes. An electronic digitizing pen and a sensor on the forehead were first used to scan three points on the head surface that define the head coordinate system (nasion and right and left preauricular points). In addition, 32 other points on the head surface were digitized. The position coils were also calibrated, and their position relative to the MEG dewar was determined.

      For the 20-min measurement, the subjects were led into the MEG chamber, placed there under the dewar, and asked to adopt a relaxed posture. Subjects were instructed to listen attentively to the binaurally presented sounds in a relaxed state and to leave their eyes open while watching a silent movie to control their vigilance. In the beginning, the head position inside the dewar was determined. To obtain a larger signal-to-noise ratio, the stimuli were presented to subjects in a continuous sequence for 17 min [total N = 1200 acoustic stimuli, therefore leading to a noise reduction of √(1,200) = 34.6]. The use of pseudorandomized interstimulus intervals furthermore minimized the confounding influence of superimposed oscillatory and artifact-related patterns to allow robust source modeling as a basis for additional analysis of the time course, latencies, and amplitudes of auditory-evoked fields. The AEFs were recorded with a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz corresponding to a low-pass filter of 330 Hz [filter range 0.00 (DC)—330 Hz]. Data analysis was conducted with the BESA Research 6.0 software (MEGIS Software GmbH, Gräfelfing, Germany). Prior to averaging, data were inspected to automatically exclude external artifacts using the BESA Research event-related fields (ERF) module. By applying the automatic artifact scan tool, on average 3–7 noisy (bad) channels were excluded, and around 10% of all epochs exceeding a gradient of 600 fT/cm and amplitudes either exceeding 3,000 fT/cm, or falling below 100 fT/cm, were rejected from further analysis. Thereby, a major part of endogenous artifacts, like eye blinks, eye movements, cardiac activity, face movements, and muscle tensions, could be accounted for. A baseline amplitude calculated over the 100-ms interval before the onset of the tones was subtracted from the signals. The responses of each subject were first collapsed into a grand average (about 1,100 artifact-free epochs after the rejection of 10% of artifact-afflicted or noisy epochs) in a 100 ms prestimulus to 400 ms poststimulus time window. Based on a standard single-sphere head model (Hämäläinen and Sarvas, 1987; Sarvas, 1987; Scherg, 1990), spatio-temporal source modeling was performed in normalized coordinates independently of the individual brain anatomy. The primary and secondary source activities of the auditory cortex were fitted using a four-dipole model with two equivalent dipoles in each hemisphere (Scherg, 1990; Wengenroth et al., 2014) and the BA10 activity with an additional seeded dipole in the right and left prefrontal cortex, respectively (Figure 2).

      Source modeling in auditory cortex and frontal lobe. (A) Top view on the left and right auditory cortex including the right (red) and left (blue) Heschl’s gyrus and the localization of the primary auditory evoked and orbitofrontally seeded responses depicted as green and pink dipoles bilaterally, respectively. (B) Source waveforms of the left and right primary activity (green) and the orbitofrontally seeded musical aesthetic activation (pink). (C) Source locations of the four dipoles projected in a spherical head model calculated with BESA software (Scherg, 1990).

      The first pair of equivalent dipoles was freely fitted in the right and left auditory cortex using an individual fitting interval covering the full P1 and following N1 response up to its peak. To increase the stability of the free fit, a regional source was used in both hemispheres (two perpendicular dipoles at the same location). The P1 localized consistently around the posterior border in the center of Heschl’s gyrus bilaterally in all age groups (mean group-averaged x-coordinate R: +47.3 ± 0.5; L: –48.1 ± 0.4 mm; mean group-averaged y-coordinate R: –15.5 ± 0.9; L: –19.7 ± 0.8 mm in Talairach stereotaxic space, Mean/ ± SE), corroborating our earlier findings (Schneider et al., 2005; Seither-Preisler et al., 2014; Serrallach et al., 2016). After localizing the primary source activity, a principal axis transformation was performed with the two components of the regional source to identify the principal component (orientation fit). To model co-activation in the frontal cortex, a second source was seeded into the BA10 area with the normalized coordinates (X = ± 20; Y = 50; Z = 0; Talairach). Finally, we used a standard seeding technique based on individual dipole orientation fits in the time window of primary and secondary auditory processing (50–250 ms), which was feasible in all participants. The orientation was always defined in direction of the vertex. Subsequently, latencies and amplitudes of the primary P1 and secondary P2 responses were derived from the resulting auditory-evoked response complex. In children, the N1 and P2 responses are still weak and not precisely detectable in the average signal due to variability in latency and amplitude. However, in the individual ERFs, both the N1 and P2 peaks could be recognized and evaluated as age-dependent delayed responses, still weak but sufficiently recognizable. In all cases, they could be identified as weak superimposed responses on the negative sustained field following the descending slope of the primary P1 response complex (see Figure 3). It was not the goal of this study to examine co-activations of AEFs at the whole-brain level; instead, we intended to specifically examine the co-activations in the orbitofrontal cortex, as motivated by a pilot fMRI experiment preceding this work (see Supplementary material).

      Auditory evoked fields in response to instrumental and harmonic complex sounds. The superior curve shows the auditory evoked P1-N1-P2 complex followed by a sustained field and the offset about 600 ms after tone onset. The inferior curve depicts the coactivated response of the orbitofrontal cortex, seeded with fixed coordinates in the center of the BA10 region. The subpanels (A–D) show the responses of the musicians, subpanel (E–H) those of the non-musicians [(A,E) children; (B,F) adolescents; (C,G) young adults; (D,H) middle-aged adults]. Arrows indicate the first onset peak of the auditory and orbitofrontal response, respectively. Red curve, right hemisphere; dark blue curve, left hemisphere.

      Statistical analyses

      The statistical analysis was divided into four main parts. In the first step, we performed two-way ANOVAs to look at the main effects and interactions between musical status and the four age groups with regard to P1 and P2 latency and amplitudes variables. In the second step, we performed a series of t-tests for each of the four age groups. The musical status (musicians vs. non-musicians) represents the grouping variable and the MEG variables are the dependent variables. As a follow-up analysis, we ran discriminant analyses where we entered the MEG variables, which were significantly different according to t-test analyses. Performing discriminant analyses has the advantage that no corrections for multiple comparisons have to be applied because it takes relationships among variables into account and provides information about which of the variables discriminate the groups best. This allows for determining the most important variables, which discriminate groups more precisely. In the fourth step, we provided correlations between musical expertise and auditory response patterns with frontal activity.

      Results Temporal dynamics and magnitude of the auditory-evoked responses

      Individual two-way ANOVAs were performed to examine the effect of musical status and age on the dependent variables. The latter were the left and right latencies and amplitudes of the auditory-evoked primary P1 and secondary auditory P2 responses. We reported the main effects and the interactions only when they were statistically significant. The P1 response is visible in both hemispheres in infancy (Figures 3A, E), whereas the P2 response is weak in childhood and matures later during young adulthood (Figures 3C, G). The ANOVA of P1 latency revealed a significant main effect for age bilaterally [right: F(3, 154) = 67.6, p<0.001, partial η2 = 0.57; left: F(3, 154) = 83.7, p <0.001, partial η2 = 0.62] and musical status in the left hemisphere [F(1, 154) = 9.7, p = 0.002, partial η2 = 0.06]. Furthermore, an interaction between “musical status” and “age group” was observed in the left hemisphere [F (3, 154) = 4.2, p = 0.007, partial η2 = 0.75]. Musicians in the children group showed a shorter latency than the non-musicians [children mus: 86.4 ms; children non: 94.5 ms, p = 0.04, r = 0.33; t(36) = –2.1, p = 0.04, Table 3], while latencies were not different between musicians and non-musicians in the adolescents and adults (Figure 4A). The ANOVA of P1 amplitude revealed a significant main effect for age bilaterally [right: F(3, 154) = 18.8, p <0.001, partial η2 = 0.27; left: F(3, 154) = 24.3, p <0.001, partial η2 = 0.32]. An interaction between “musical status” and “age group” was found for the right P1 amplitude [F(3, 154) = 4.1, p = 0.007, partial η2 = 0.07] because the P1 amplitude was smaller in musicians than in non-musicians in children [t(35) = –1.9, p = 0.05], however, larger in young adults [t(41) = 2.5, p = 0.02] and middle-aged adults [t(40) = 2.0, p = 0.05] but differed not significantly in adolescents. The ANOVA of the P2 latency revealed a significant main effect for age bilaterally [right: F(3, 154) = 83.0, p <0.001, partial η2 = 0.62; left: F(3, 154) = 82.7, p <0.001, partial η2 = 0.62, Figure 4B]. An interaction between ‘musical status’ and ‘age group’ was found for the P2 latency bilaterally [right: F(3, 154) = 4.0, p = 0.009, partial η2 = 0.07; left: F(3, 154) = 4.2, p = 0.007, partial η2 = 0.08]. The ANOVA of the P2 amplitude revealed a significant main effect for age bilaterally [right: F(3, 154) = 51.9, p <0.001, partial η2 = 0.50; left: F(3, 154) = 55.0, p <0.001, partial η2 = 0.52]. An interaction between “musical status” and “age group” was found for the P2 amplitude bilaterally [right: F(3, 154) = 9.14, p = 0.001, partial η2 = 0.15; left: F(3, 154) = 10.18, p = 0.001, partial η2 = 0.17] because the P2 amplitude was bilaterally smaller in musicians than in non-musicians in children [right P2: t(36) = –2.0, p = 0.05; left: P2: t(36) = –2.3, p = 0.03], however, 3- to 4-fold larger in young adults [right P2: t(41) = 3.5, p <0.001; left: P2: t(41) = 3.4, p <0.001, Figures 3C, G] and middle-aged adults [right P2: t(40) = 4.2, p < 0.001; left: P2: t(40) = 3.7, p < 0.001, Figures 3D, H] but differed not significantly in adolescents. In musicians, P2 emerges as the most dominant response with a mean amplitude of 28.2 nAm in the right hemisphere and 23.0 nAm in the left hemisphere (means of latencies and amplitudes see Table 3 and Figures 3, 4C). Interestingly, the acoustically evoked responses in adult musicians show a P1-N1-P2 response complex with similar large balanced amplitudes of the individual subcomponents (Figure 3D). We found no significant differences between the amplitudes and latencies of the right and left hemispheres.

      Independent t-test (musicians vs. non-musicians).

      Variables Children Adolescents Young adults Middle-aged adults
      Mus Non Mus Non Mus Non Mus Non
      Mean ± SE Mean ± SE t p r Mean ± SE Mean ± SE t p r Mean ± SE Mean ± SE t p r Mean ± SE Mean ± SE t p r
      P1 latency R 86.3 ± 3.0 90.1 ± 2.2 –1.1 0.27 70.0 ± 2.2 72.8 ± 2.1 –1.0 0.31 66.9 ± 1.3 66.1 ± 1.4 0.4 0.69 62.7 ± 1.2 62. ± 2.1 2.9 0.78
      P1 latency L 86.8 ± 2.9 94.5 ± 2.3 –2.1 0.04 0.33 72.5 ± 3.3 74.3 ± 2.3 –0.7 0.49 65.4 ± 1.5 66.5 ± 1.5 0.5 0.63 62.4 ± 0.9 63.6 ± 1.5 –.71 0.48
      P1 amplitude R 27.0 ± 2.5 35.1 ± 3.1 –1.9 0.05 0.30 16.6 ± 2.5 16.5 ± 1.9 0.01 0.98 20.1 ± 2.2 13.1 ± 1.8 2.5 0.02 0.36 19.3 ± 2.3 12.9 ± 2.2 2.0 0.05 0.30
      P1 amplitude L 32.8 ± 2.7 38.7 ± 2.6 –1.6 0.13 20.9 ± 2.7 20.5 ± 2.0 0.1 0.91 21.9 ± 2.6 15.8 ± 1.9 1.9 0.06 19.4 ± 1.9 15.8 ± 2.3 1.2 0.25
      N1 latency R 214.9 ± 9.2 234.5 ± 9.3 –1.5 0.14 126.3 ± 4.9 122.9 ± 4.9 0.5 0.62 116.2 ± 1.9 113.4 ± 2.4 0.8 0.40 109.9 ± 1.6 106.2 ± 1.8 1.5 0.14
      N1 latency L 213.4 ± 8.5 227.5 ± 7.1 –0.8 0.41 146.1 ± 8.7 134.3 ± 7.0 1.1 0.29 115.7 ± 1.9 107.8 ± 4.9 1.4 0.17 111.5 ± 1.8 108.3 ± 2.1 1.2 0.24
      N1 amplitude R –41.9 ± 5.1 –29.4 ± 3.9 –1.9 0.06 –18.7 ± 3.8 –14.6 ± 4.0 –0.7 0.47 –14.8 ± 4.2 –20.8 ± 4.6 0.9 0.33 –25.1 ± 4.8 –27.7 ± 4.0 0.4 0.67
      N1 amplitude L –37.3 ± 4.2 –29.2 ± 3.7 –1.5 0.16 –17.5 ± 4.7 –8.2 ± 2.9 –1.7 0.10 –7.9 ± 4.8 –14.1 ± 4.1 1.0 0.33 –19.6 ± 3.6 –22.1 ± 3.9 0.5 0.64
      P2 latency R 287.1 ± 13.4 316.7 ± 8.7 –1.9 0.07 205.8 ± 10.4 187.0 ± 8.9 1.4 0.18 197.6 ± 3.6 196.7 ± 6.6 0.1 0.91 206.4 ± 2.5 207.2 ± 3.7 –0.2 0.86
      P2 latency L 294.1 ± 8.8 316.6 ± 9.4 –1.7 0.09 221.7 ± 9.9 202.3 ± 10.6 1.3 0.19 201.6 ± 4.2 196.9 ± 6.0 0.6 0.54 217.5 ± 3.7 215.9 ± 4.5 0.3 0.78
      P2 amplitude R –35.5 ± 4.8 –22.1 ± 4.5 –2.0 0.05 0.32 –5.8 ± 4.3 3.1 ± 3.6 –1.6 0.13 19.8 ± 3.9 4.8 ± 2.1 3.5 0.00 0.47 28.2 ± 4.4 4.3 ± 3.3 4.2 0.00 0.55
      P2 amplitude L –25.7 ± 3.9 –14.3 ± 3.1 –2.3 0.03 0.36 –2.9 ± 3.9 4.11 ± 2.6 –1.5 0.14 23.0 ± 3.8 8.3 ± 2.4 3.4 0.00 0.47 23.0 ± 2.9 7.6 ± 2.8 3.7 0.00 0.50
      BA10 latency R 92.7 ± 6.2 118.6 ± 6.3 –2.9 0.01 0.44 85.8 ± 4.3 117.8 ± 5.5 –4.6 0.00 0.50 70.2 ± 3.7 102.3 ± 3.8 –6.0 0.00 0.68 61.3 ± 2.6 98.4 ± 3.7 –8.5 0.00 0.80
      BA10 latency L 95.2 ± 6.6 117.8 ± 7.5 –2.3 0.03 0.36 84.7 ± 3.6 116.1 ± 5.6 –4.7 0.00 0.61 68.2 ± 3.0 103.4 ± 4.3 –6.4 0.00 0.71 60.8 ± 3.0 98.2 ± 3.8 –7.9 0.00 0.78
      BA10 amplitude R 7.9 ± 0.9 10.5 ± 1.4 –1.6 0.13 6.4 ± 0.7 7.1 ± 0.9 –0.6 0.54 5.3 ± 0.7 6.7 ± 1.0 –1.1 0.29 6.3 ± 0.5 8.3 ± 0.9 –2.0 0.05 0.30
      BA10 amplitude L 9.7 ± 1.1 10.2 ± 1.0 –0.3 0.75 6.5 ± 0.7 6.0 ± 0.7 0.5 0.60 5.7 ± 0.8 6.8 ± 0.8 –1.0 0.35 7.5 ± 0.6 7.6 ± 0.9 –0.1 0.96
      Lat Diff BA10-P1 R 12.3 ± 4.7 28.8 ± 6.3 –2.1 0.05 0.33 19.1 ± 3.6 44.7 ± 5.6 –3.9 0.00 0.54 9.8 ± 2.2 41.6 ± 3.5 –7.3 0.00 0.75 8.9 ± 1.7 41.2 ± 4.8 –6.8 0.00 0.73
      Lat Diff BA10-P1 L 12.1 ± 4.9 32.9 ± 6.1 –2.6 0.01 0.40 13.8 ± 2.9 41.4 ± 5.1 –4.8 0.00 0.62 7.1 ± 1.5 41.7 ± 4.3 –7.0 0.00 0.73 11.1 ± 2.1 38.7 ± 4.3 –6.1 0.00 0.69

      Age related changes of the most important MEG variables. Group averaged values of (A) P1 latency (B) P2 latency (C) P2 amplitude (D) BA10 latency (E) BA10 amplitude (F) BA10-P1 latency difference, averaged over both hemispheres, respectively. Y-axis, latencies in ms and amplitudes in nAm. X-Axis, Subgroups of children (C), adolescents (A), young adults (Y), and middle-aged adults (O). Solid lines (circles), musicians; dashed lines (triangles), non-musicians.

      Discriminant analyses

      We ran separate discriminant analyses for musicians and non-musicians of our four groups (children, adolescents, young adults, and middle-aged adults) to illustrate which of the variables discriminated our musicians from non-musicians best. We used only variables, which differentiated the musicians from the non-musicians at least at a 0.05 level (see Table 3). We used the statistically recommended cutoff value of 0.4 (Warner, 2012) to decide which of the standardized discriminant coefficients was large enough to be significant. Loads of the predictor variables onto the discriminant functions are presented in Table 4.

      Predictor variables of the discriminant analyses.

      Children Adolescents Young adults Middle-aged adults
      Variables r Variables r Variables r Variables r
      BA10 latency R 0.61 Lat Diff BA10-P1 L 0.89 Lat Diff BA10-P1 R 0.80 BA10 latency R 0.89
      Lat Diff BA10-P1 L 0.53 BA10 latency L 0.88 Lat Diff BA10-P1 L 0.77 BA10 latency L 0.83
      BA10 latency L 0.48 BA10 latency R 0.86 BA10 latency L 0.71 Lat Diff BA10-P1 R 0.71
      P1 latency L 0.51 Lat Diff BA10-P1 R 0.73 BA10 latency R 0.66 Lat Diff BA10-P1 L 0.64
      Lat Diff BA10-P1 R 0.42 P2 amplitude R –0.39 P2 amplitude R 0.44
      P2 amplitude L 0.41 P2 amplitude L –0.37 P2 amplitude L –0.39
      P2 amplitude R 0.31 P1 amplitude R –0.28 P1 amplitude R –0.21
      P1 latency R 0.25 BA10 amplitude R 0.21

      Bold values indicate significant discriminant coefficients (values > 0.4).

      The discriminant function significantly separated the musicians and non-musicians in all four age groups [children: canonical R2 = 0.42, Λ = 0.58, χ2(8) = 17.77, p = 0.02; adolescents: canonical R2 = 0.44, Λ = 0.56, χ2(4) = 20.02, p = 0.001; young adults: canonical R2 = 0.67, Λ = 0.33, χ2(7) = 40.79, p<0.001; middle-aged adults: canonical R2 = 0.69, Λ = 0.31, χ2(8) = 42.66, p <0.001].

      Timing of auditory and frontal activities depends on musicianship and age

      Regarding BA10 co-activation in the orbitofrontal cortex, it was found that the latency of BA10 activation was significantly shorter in musicians compared with non-musicians at all ages by about 25–40 ms (Figure 4D). The difference values continued to increase with age [right: children: 25.9 ms (p = 0.01, r = 0.44); adolescents: 32.0 ms (p < 0.001, r = 0.50); young adults: 32.1 ms (p < 0.001, r = 0.68); middle-aged adults: 37.1 ms (p< 0.001, r = 0.80); left: children: 22.6 ms (p = 0.01, r = 0.36); adolescents: 31.4 ms (p < 0.001, r = 0.61); young adults: 35.2 ms (p < 0.001, r = 0.71); middle-aged adults: 37.4 ms (p < 0.001, r = 0.78), see Table 3 and Figures 3, 4F). However, no significant differences were found for BA10 amplitudes Figure 4E. In musicians in all four age groups, BA10 co-activation (lower curve) shows a characteristic highly significant reduced latency difference between its first response peak and the auditory cortex’s evoked primary P1 peak (upper curve). This was evident with three to fivefold shorter BA10-P1 latency differences right side: children: latency difference in musicians 12.3 ms, in non-musicians 28.8 ms (p = 0.05, r = 0.33), adolescents: 19.1 vs. 44.8 ms (p <0.001, r = 0.54); young adults: 9.8 vs. 41.6 ms (p <0.001; r = 0.75); middle-aged adults: 8.9 vs. 41.2 ms (p < 0.001; r = 0.73); left side: children: latency difference in musicians 12.1 ms, in non-musicians 32.9 ms (p = 0.01, r = 0.40), adolescents: 13.8 vs. 41.4 ms (p <0.001, r = 0.62); young adults: 7.1 vs. 41.7 ms (p <0.001, r = 0.73); middle-aged adults: 11.1 vs. 38.7 ms (p <0.001, r = 0.69, see also Table 3). Thus, in non-musicians, the first peak of the BA10 response with positive polarity in direction of the vertex occurs on average 30–45 ms after the auditory-evoked P1 response, that is, more in the time range of the subsequent N1 response.

      Correlations between musical expertise, auditory response patterns, and hemispheres

      Figure 5 shows significant correlations between AMMA/IMMA values and BA10 latency in the whole group of subjects (subpanel a, r = –0.48, p = 2*E-9), but this no longer remains significant when musicians and non-musicians are considered separately (subpanel d, r = –0.13 and –0.14, n.s., respectively). The correlation plots of BA10 latency and IMP show similarly a strong correlation for the whole group (subpanel b, r = –0.52, p = 1.6*E-12) that remains visible if considered only the non-musicians (subpanel f, non: r = –0.53, p = 5*E-7), however, vanishes for the musicians (r = –0.18, n.s.). If the outliers in the non-musician group with high BA10 responses were disregarded, the remaining distribution could be considered normal and the correlation even dropped to an insignificant level. Taking together, no significant correlations were found between the latency of the frontal response and musical expertise if musicians and non-musicians were considered as separate groups. In contrast, the P2 amplitude and BA10 latency (subpanels c and f) exhibit a strong correlation, visible both for the whole group (r = –0.35, p = 7.9*E-6) and also for the musicians and non-musicians separately (mus: r = –0.36, p = 4*E-4; non: r = –0.35, p = 2*E-4).

      Correlational analyses between behavioral and MEG variables. (A,D) Represent BA10 latency and AMMA/IMMA score correlations; (B,E) BA10 latency and IMP cumulative; (C,F) BA10 latency and P2 amplitude. (A–C) Depict the correlations over all subjects, colors indicate here the four age groups. (D–F) Depict the correlations separately for the musicians (circles) and the non-musicians (crosses); n.s., non-significant.

      Discussion

      Comparing auditory processing in musician and non-musician children, adolescents, young adults, and middle-aged adults, we found a systematic reduction in latency differences between the primary auditory response and associated co-activation in the orbitofrontal cortex. However, these patterns were time-shifted in non-musicians and showed 25–40 ms later responses in the frontal lobe than in musicians. The influence of musical expertise from a long-term perspective from childhood to adulthood has been widely demonstrated (Kraus and Chandrasekaran, 2010; Kraus and Anderson, 2014; Skoe et al., 2015). However, the specific role of simultaneous activation patterns of spatially separated brain responses and potential delay differences has not yet been investigated.

      Latency as an indicator of neural efficiency

      There is compelling evidence that higher neuronal efficiency translates into shorter latencies in auditory-evoked responses (Sharma et al., 2005, Seither-Preisler et al., 2014). In our study, the characteristic group differences between the observed latencies in the orbitofrontal lobe in musicians and non-musicians lead to a different type of interaction between the primary and secondary auditory-evoked response patterns and the frontal co-activation involved. In course of their musical expertise, the musicians seem to develop a BA10P1-N1-P2-complex, while the non-musicians develop a P1-BA10N1-P2- pattern, as visible from the measured source waveforms (Figure 3) and the prominent discriminant function values for the latency differences between the orbitofrontal and auditory onset responses (Table 4). In musicians, the BA10 response is already activated at the time of immediate automatic primary activation in childhood, which at that early stage may also connect to emotional and intuitive aspects at a more unconscious level. Regarding our correlational results, a direct influence of both cumulative musical experience and music audiation on the orbitofrontal BA10 activation was observed for the entire groups, in approximately equal proportions, while no significant relationship was found when musicians and non-musicians were considered separately. In contrast, the correlation between BA10 activation and P2 amplitude was strong irrespective of whether the whole sample or subgroups were considered (Figure 5). The acceleration of auditory co-activated prefrontal response patterns in musicians as seen in our data (Figure 4D) confirms previous findings (Dumontheil, 2014). For the first time, we demonstrated that independent of age, the latency of the BA10 response is significantly shorter in musicians than in non-musicians. Discriminant analysis revealed that BA10 was one of the most important variables, which best distinguished the musicians from the non-musicians (Table 4).

      Based on the ANOVA analyses, the following conclusions can be drawn. P1 latency in the left hemisphere seems to be reduced by musical training during early childhood corresponding to the findings of previous studies (Seither-Preisler et al., 2014; Serrallach et al., 2016). We also detected that the amplitude of the right P1 and the left P2 was significantly different for the musicians and non-musicians in the children and adults. However, while in the child group musicians showed lower mean values of the right amplitude compared with the non-musicians, this was the opposite for the adult groups where the mean values were considerably larger for musicians when compared with the non-musicians. This shift could be explained by the fact that P2 develops more through both music making and listening experience and then becomes the strongest component either through long-term training (Benner et al., 2017) or by short-term plasticity (Schneider et al., 2022). Interestingly, the mean values of the musicians and non-musicians for the left P1 latency, the right P1 amplitude, and the right and the left P2 amplitudes did not differ in the adolescent group.

      Intrahemispheric correlation and timing of auditory response patterns

      Previous research already indicated that musical expertise is associated with an increased magnitude of the primary P1 (Schneider et al., 2002) and secondary P2 responses (Benner et al., 2017). In our study, we found a remarkable correlation between P2 amplitude and latency of orbitofrontal activation (Figure 5F). Furthermore, with respect to the latency differences between P1 (elementary auditory processing level) and BA10, we also found strongly reduced values for the musicians (Figure 4F). Significant correlations between AMMA/IMMA values and BA10 latency were found in the whole group of subjects, but this does not remain significant when musicians and non-musicians were considered separately. Since musically active individuals are said to possess stronger multisensory and interhemispheric networks in the brain, one would expect a higher degree of synchronized brain activity per se in the brain of such individuals (Groß et al., 2022). Here, we show that the magnitude of the auditory-evoked P2 response correlates strongly with the timing of orbitofrontal activation. Other recent studies support the finding that the P2 response initiates multisensory integration processes and prepares transfer effects into other domains (Schneider et al., 2022).

      Influence of musical practice and audiation on primary and frontal activities

      To disentangle the complementary influence of musical aptitude and musical training, we performed the musicality measures IMMA/AMMA and collected for each subject a cumulative amount of musical training (IMP). At the level of auditory processing, our data confirmed a remarkable effect of musical training on the magnitude of the late auditory-evoked P2 response, consistent with previous findings (Shahin et al., 2005; Benner et al., 2017). The IMMA/AMMA score also showed a significant effect on P2 amplitude, suggesting that listening to music involves cognitive aspects, such as the understanding of perceived sounds and music (cf. Gordon, 1997, 1998). At the orbitofrontal level, both the cognitive aspects of musical training, listening, and dispositional aspect of musical aptitude (as measured by the IMMA/AMMA score) demonstrated a strong influence only when the whole sample was considered, but this disappeared when musicians and non-musicians were considered separately. It remains an open question as to what the observed greater co-activation of the orbitofrontal cortex in musicians compared with non-musicians might signify. Sensory integration of auditory processes was found to generally include two-stage processes with a first fundamental processing stage of “categorical perception” of auditory processing and a second stage of labeling through sensory integration in the frontal lobe (Elmer et al., 2015). Such hierarchical processes bridging the gap between primary and frontal activities have also been observed for salient auditory skills, such as absolute pitch (Wengenroth et al., 2014). In this sense, it could be suggested that enhanced auditory responses in musicians should also trigger intensive co-activation in non-auditory areas. The experience-dependent temporal changes in connectivity between the orbitofrontal cortex and auditory areas may outline how music training affects a widely distributed network due to more mature myelination and greater attentional focus.

      Everyday auditory processes, such as sound object recognition, identification of pitch direction, and melody recognition, also require both elementary steps of fundamental and spectral pitch perception in the auditory cortex and the detection of tone direction in frontal areas (Johnsrude et al., 2000; Schneider et al., 2005). In neuroesthetics, the prefrontal cortex, and in particular Brodmann area 10 (BA10), is repeatedly mentioned in connection with aesthetic processing (Review: Brown et al., 2011). The prefrontal cortex is further activated in sensory processing with respect to monitoring and in learning processes. Together with the anterior insula, the prefrontal cortex is also known as the “gateway to conscious subjective experience” (Kringelbach, 2005; Brown et al., 2011). Prominent connections between primary or secondary sensory areas and the orbitofrontal cortex are described as a dorsal pathway (where stream) for spatial processing and as a ventral pathway (what stream) for decoding more complex parameters (meaning-making) (Rauschecker, 2012; Kandel et al., 2013). Our data corroborate that these hierarchically organized auditory processing networks (Rauschecker and Scott, 2009, Rauschecker, 2012), which link auditory to prefrontal areas, are more pronounced and efficient in musically experienced subjects (Zatorre et al., 2007; Benner et al., 2022 in submission).

      In summary, we propose that the chronology of auditory processing is crucial to understand the underlying mechanisms of higher cognitive processing of musical sounds. Precisely speaking, musically experienced listeners show largely reduced latency differences and therefore almost simultaneous activations in the auditory and prefrontal cortex. Follow-up studies should investigate in more detail the extent to which co-activation of the prefrontal cortex in musicians might be associated with greater memory performance or even stronger musical-aesthetic sensations.

      Data availability statement

      The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

      Ethics statement

      The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Medical Faculty of Heidelberg S-778/2018. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

      Author contributions

      SB and PS involved the acquisition of data. MC and PS performed the statistical analysis. SB, MC, VB, AT, and PS were responsible for finalizing the work. SB, MC, and PS performed a critical revision of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

      Funding

      PS was funded by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF) and the German Research Foundation (DFG; Heisenberg fellowship program “Sound perception between outstanding musical abilities and auditory dysfunction: The neural basis of individual predisposition, maturation, and learning-induced plasticity in a lifespan perspective”). MC was a recipient of an APART-GSK Fellowship of the Austrian Academy of Sciences at the Centre of Systematic Musicology of the University Graz. Open Access Funding by the University of Graz. The work was supported by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF) as (Grant 01KJ0809/10 and 01KJ1204) (collaborative project “AMseL: Audio and Neuroplasticity of Musical Learning” in cooperation with the University of Graz and the German Research Foundation (DFG), Lead Agency collaborative project “Auditory plasticity of the adult musical brain” in cooperation with the University of Basel and Heisenberg (DFG SCHN 965/4-1) fellowship program “Sound perception between outstanding musical abilities and auditory dysfunction: The neural basis of individual predisposition, maturation, and learning-induced plasticity in a lifespan perspective” (DFG SCHN 965/7-1) by PS in Heidelberg.

      We thank A. Rupp and M. Bendszus for providing the MEG and 3T-MRI in Heidelberg.

      Conflict of interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Publisher’s note

      All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

      Supplementary material

      The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: /articles/10.3389/fnins.2022.1041397/full#supplementary-material

      References Amati D. Shallice T. (2007). On the emergence of modern humans. Cognition 103 358385. 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.04.002 16709406 Badre D. (2008). Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro–caudal organization of the frontal lobes. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12 193200. 10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.004 18403252 Badre D. Wagner A. D. (2004). Selection, integration, and conflict monitoring: Assessing the nature and generality of prefrontal cognitive control mechanisms. Neuron 41 473487. 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00851-1 Benner J. Reinhard J. Christiner M. Wengenroth M. Stippich C. Schneider P. (2022). Temporal hierarchy of cortical responses reflects core-beltparabelt organization of human auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex (under review). Benner J. Wengenroth M. Reinhardt J. Stippich C. Schneider P. Blatow M. (2017). Prevalence and function of Heschl’s gyrus morphotypes in musicians. Brain Struct. Funct. 222 35873603. 10.1007/s00429-017-1419-x 28397108 Blood A. J. Zatorre R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98 1181811823. 10.1073/pnas.191355898 11573015 Boemio A. Fromm S. Braun A. Poeppel D. (2005). Hierarchical and asymmetric temporal sensitivity in human auditory cortices. Nat. Neurosci. 8 389395. 10.1038/nn1409 15723061 Brattico E. Brattico P. Jacobsen T. (2009). The origins of the aesthetic enjoyment of music—A review of the literature. Mus. Sci. 13 1539. 10.1177/1029864909013002031 Braver T. S. Bongiolatti S. R. (2002). The role of frontopolar cortex in subgoal processing during working memory. NeuroImage 15 523536. 10.1006/nimg.2001.1019 11848695 Brown S. Gao X. Tisdelle L. Eickhoff S. B. Liotti M. (2011). Naturalizing aesthetics: Brain areas for aesthetic appraisal across sensory modalities. NeuroImage 58 250258. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.012 21699987 Bugos J. A. DeMarie D. (2017). The effects of a short-term music program on preschool children’s executive functions. Psychol. Mus. 45 855867. 10.1177/0305735617692666 Burgess P. W. Dumontheil I. Gilbert S. J. (2007). The gateway hypothesis of rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11 290298. 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.004 17548231 Burgess P. W. Scott S. K. Frith C. D. (2003). The role of the rostral frontal cortex (area 10) in prospective memory: A lateral versus medial dissociation. Neuropsychologia 39 545555. 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00327-5 Chaddock-Heyman L. Loui P. Weng T. B. Weisshappel R. McAuley E. Kramer A. F. (2021). Musical training and brain volume in older adults. Brain Sci. 11:50. 10.3390/brainsci11010050 33466337 Christiner M. Serrallach B. Benner J. Bernhofs V. Schneider P. Renner J. (2022). Examining individual differences in singing, musical and tone language ability in adolescents and young adults with dyslexia. Brain Sci. 12:744. 10.3390/brainsci12060744 35741629 Christoff K. Gabrieli J. D. (2000). The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: Evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex. Psychobiology 28 168186. 10.3758/BF03331976 Christoff K. Ream J. M. Geddes L. P. T. Gabrieli J. (2003). Evaluating self-generated information: Anterior prefrontal contributions to human cognition. Behav. Neurosci. 117 11611168. 10.1037/0735-7044.117.6.1161 14674837 Cunningham W. A. Raye C. L. Johnson M. K. (2004). Implicit and explicit evaluation: fMRI correlates of valence, emotional intensity, and control in the processing of attitudes. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16 17171729. 10.1162/0898929042947919 15701224 Dumontheil I. (2014). Development ofabstract thinking during childhood and adolescence: The role of rostrolateral prefrontal cortex. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 10 5776. 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.07.009 25173960 Eggermont J. J. Moore J. K. (2012). “Morphological and functional development of the auditory nervous system,” in Human auditory development, eds Werner L. Fay R. R. Popper A. N. (New York, NY: Springer), 61105. 10.1007/978-1-4614-1421-6_3 Elmer S. Rogenmoser L. Kühnis J. Jäncke L. (2015). Bridging the gap between perceptual and cognitive perspectives on absolute pitch. J. Neursci. 35 366371. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3009-14.2015 25568128 Frischen U. Schwarzer G. Degé F. (2021). Music lessons enhance executive functions in 6-to 7-year-old children. Learn. Inst. 74:101442. 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2021.101442 Fuster J. M. (1997). The prefrontal cortex: “Anatomy, physiology, and neuropsychology of the frontal lobe”. (Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott-Raven), 373385. Gilbert S. J. Spengler S. Simons J. S. Steele J. D. Lawrie S. M. Frith C. D. (2006). Functional specialization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10): A meta-analysis. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18 932948. 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.6.932 16839301 Gordon E. (1986). A factor analysis of the musical aptitude profile, the primary measures of music audiation, and the intermediate measures of music audiation. Bull. Counc. Res. Mus. Educ. 1725. Gordon E. (1989). “Audiation, music learning theory, music aptitude and creativity,” in Suncoast music education forum on creativity, ed. Richmond J. W. (Tampa: The University of South Florida), 7581. Gordon E. (1997). Learning sequences in music, 3rd Edn. Chicago, IL: GIA Publications. Gordon E. (1998). Introduction to research and the psychology of music. Chicago, IL: GIA Publications. Gordon E. (2011). Roots of music learning theory and audiation. Chicago, IL: GIA Publications. Groß C. Serrallach B. L. Möhler E. Pousson J. E. Schneider P. Christiner M. (2022). Musical performance in adolescents with ADHD, ADD and dyslexia-behavioral and neurophysiological aspects. Brain Sci. 12:127. 10.3390/brainsci12020127 35203891 Habibi A. Damasio A. Ilari B. Elliott Sachs M. Damasio H. (2018). Music training and child development: A review of recent findings from a longitudinal study. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1423 7381. 10.1111/nyas.13606 29508399 Hackett T. A. Stepniewska I. Kaas J. H. (1998). Thalamocortical connections of the parabelt auditory cortex in macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 400 271286. 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19981019)400:2<271::AID-CNE8>3.0.CO;2-6 Hämäläinen M. S. Sarvas J. (1987). Feasibility of the homogeneous head model in the interpretation of neuromagnetic fields. Phys. Med. Biol. 32:91. 10.1088/0031-9155/32/1/014 Herdener M. Esposito F. di Salle F. Herdener M. Esposito F. di Salle F. (2010). Musical training induces functional plasticity in human hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 30 13771384. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4513-09.2010 20107063 Hyde K. L. Lerch J. Norton A. Forgeard M. Winner E. Evans A. C. (2009). Musical training shapes structural brain development. J. Neurosci. 29 30193025. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5118-08.2009 19279238 Jacobsen T. Schubotz R. I. Höfel L. Cramon D. Y. V. (2006). Brain correlates of aesthetic judgment of beauty. Neuroimage 29 276285. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.010 16087351 James C. E. Altenmüller E. Kliegel M. Krüger T. van de Ville D. Worschech F. (2020). Train the brain with music (TBM). Brain plasticity and cognitive benefits induced by musical training in elderly people in Germany and Switzerland, a study protocol for an RCT comparing musical instrumental practice to sensitization to music. BMC Geriatr. 20:418. 10.1186/s12877-020-01761-y 33087078 Johnsrude I. S. Penhune V. B. Zatorre R. J. (2000). Functional specificity in the right human auditory cortex for perceiving pitch direction. Brain 123 155163. 10.1093/brain/123.1.155 10611129 Kandel E. Schwartz J. H. Jessell T. M. (2013). Principles of neural science, 5th Edn. London: Robinson. Koechlin E. Basso G. Pietrini P. Panzer S. Grafman J. (1999). The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition. Nature 399, 148151. Koechlin E. Ody C. Kouneither F. (2003). The architecture of cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Science 302 11811185. 10.1126/science.1088545 14615530 Kraus N. Anderson S. (2014). Music benefits across lifespan: Enhanced processing of speech in noise. Hear. Rev. 21 1821. Kraus N. Anderson S. White-Schwoch T. Fay R. R. Popper A. N. (2017). The frequency-following response. A window into human communication. Cham: ASA Press. 10.1007/978-3-319-47944-6 Kraus N. Chandrasekaran B. (2010). Music training for the development of auditory skills. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11 599605. 10.1038/nrn2882 20648064 Kraus N. Nicol T. (2005). Brainstem origins for cortical “what” and “where” pathways in the auditory system. Trends Neurosci. 28 176181. 10.1016/j.tins.2005.02.003 15808351 Kringelbach M. L. (2005). The human orbitofrontal cortex: Linking reward to hedonic experience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6 691702. 10.1038/nrn1747 16136173 Linnavalli T. Putkinen V. Lipsanen J. Huotilainen M. Tervaniemi M. (2018). Music playschool enhances children’s linguistic skills. Sci. Rep. 8:8767. 10.1038/s41598-018-27126-5 29884891 Milner A. D. Goodale M. A. (2008). Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia 46 774785. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.005 18037456 Moreno S. Bialystok E. Barac R. Schellenberg E. G. Cepeda N. J. Chau T. (2011). Short-term music training enhances verbal intelligence and executive function. Psychol. Sci. 22 14251433. 10.1177/0956797611416999 21969312 Moreno S. Marques C. Santos A. Santos M. Castro S. L. Besson M. (2009). Musical training influences linguistic abilities in 8-year-old children: More evidence for brain plasticity. Cereb. Cortex 19, 712723. Moreno S. Wodniecka Z. Tays W. Alain C. Bialystok E. (2014). Inhibitory control in bilinguals and musicians: Event related potential (ERP) evidence for experience-specific effects. PLoS One 9:e94169. 10.1371/journal.pone.0094169 24743321 Münte T. F. Altenmüller E. Jäncke L. (2002). The musician’s brain as a model of neuroplasticity. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3 473478. 10.1038/nrn843 12042882 Nieminen S. Istók E. Brattico E. Tervaniemi M. Huotilainen M. (2011). The development of aesthetic responses to music and their underlying neural and psychological mechanisms. Cortex 47 11381146. 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.05.008 21665202 Nikjeh D. A. Lister J. J. (2012). Influence of music training on pre-attentive auditory-neural processing across the lifespan. Perspect. Hear. Hear. Disord. Res. Diagn. 16 4754. 10.1044/hhd16.2.47 Pallesen K. J. Brattico E. Bailey C. J. Korvenoja A. Koivisto J. Gjedde A. (2010). Cognitive control in auditory working memory is enhanced in musicians. PLoS One 5:e11120. 10.1371/journal.pone.0011120 20559545 Pantev C. Oostenveld R. Engelien A. Ross B. Roberts L. E. Hoke M. (1998). Increased auditory cortical representation in musicians. Nature 392 811814. 10.1038/33918 9572139 Patel S. H. Azzam P. N. (2005). Characterization of N200 and P300: Selected studies of the event-related potential. Int. J. Med. Sci. 2:147. 10.7150/ijms.2.147 16239953 Poeppel D. (2003). The analysis of speech in different temporal integration windows: Cerebral lateralization as ‘asymmetric sampling in time’. Speech Commun. 41 245255. 10.1016/S0167-6393(02)00107-3 Ramnani N. Owen A. M. (2004). Anterior prefrontal cortex: Insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5 184194. 10.1038/nrn1343 14976518 Rauschecker J. P. (2012). Ventral and dorsal streams in the evolution of speech and language. Front. Evol. Neurosci. 4:7. 10.3389/fnevo.2012.00007 22615693 Rauschecker J. P. Scott S. K. (2009). Maps and streams in the auditory cortex: Nonhuman primates illuminate human speech processing. Nat. Neurosci. 12 718724. 10.1038/nn.2331 19471271 Remington E. D. Egger S. W. Narain D. Wang J. Jazayeri M. (2018). A dynamical systems perspective on flexible motor timing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22 938952. 10.1016/j.tics.2018.07.010 30266152 Roca M. Torralva T. Gleichgerrcht E. Woolgar A. Thompson R. Duncan J. (2011). The role of Area 10 (BA10) in human multitasking and in social cognition: A lesion study. Neuropsychologia 49 35253531. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.003 21930138 Roden I. Grube D. Bongard S. Kreutz G. (2014). Does music training enhance working memory performance? Findings from a quasi-experimental longitudinal study. Psychol. Mus. 42 284298. 10.1177/0305735612471239 Sala G. Gobet F. (2020). Cognitive and academic benefits of music training with children: A multilevel meta-analysis. Mem. Cogn. 48 14291441. 10.3758/s13421-020-01060-2 32728850 Särkämö T. Tervaniemi M. Huotilainen M. (2013). Music perception and cognition: Development, neural basis, and rehabilitative use of music. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. 4 441451. 10.1002/wcs.1237 26304229 Sarvas J. (1987). Basic mathematical and electromagnetic concepts of the biomagnetic inverse problem. Phys. Med. Biol. 32 1122. 10.1088/0031-9155/32/1/004 Schellenberg E. G. (2011). Examining the association between music lessons and intelligence. Br. J. Psychol. 102 283302. 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02000.x 21751987 Scherg M. (1990). “Fundamentals of dipole source potential analysis,” in Auditory evoked magnetic fields and electric potentials. Advances in audiology, 6th Edn, eds Grandori F. Hoke M. Romani G. L. (Basel: Karger), 4069. Schneider P. Groß C. Bernhofs V. Christiner M. Benner J. Turker S. (2022). Short-term plasticity of neuro-auditory processing induced by musical active listening training. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1517 176190. 10.1111/nyas.14899 36114664 Schneider P. Scherg M. Dosch H. G. Specht H. J. Gutschalk A. Rupp A. (2002). Morphology of Heschl’s gyrus reflects enhanced activation in the auditory cortex of musicians. Nat. Neurosci. 5 688694. 10.1038/nn871 12068300 Schneider P. Sluming V. Roberts N. Scherg M. Goebel R. Specht H. (2005). Structural and functional asymmetry of lateral Heschl’s gyrus reflects pitch perception preference. Nat. Neurosci. 8 12411247. 10.1038/nn1530 16116442 Schönwiesner M. Rübsamen R. Von Cramon D. Y. (2005). Hemispheric asymmetry for spectral and temporal processing in the human antero-lateral auditory belt cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 22 15211528. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04315.x 16190905 Seither-Preisler A. Parncutt R. Schneider P. (2014). Size and synchronization of auditory cortex promotes musical, literacy, and attentional skills in children. J. Neurosci. 34 1093710949. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5315-13.2014 25122894 Semendeferi K. Armstrong E. Schleicher A. Zilles K. Van Hoesen G. W. (2001). Prefrontal cortex in humans and apes: A comparative study of area 10. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 114 224241. 10.1002/1096-8644(200103)114:3<224::AID-AJPA1022>3.0.CO;2-I Semendeferi K. Teffer K. Buxhoeveden D. P. Park M. S. Bludau S. Amunts K. (2011). Spatial organization of neurons in the frontal pole sets humans apart from great apes. Cereb. Cortex 21 14851497. 10.1093/cercor/bhq191 21098620 Serrallach B. Groß C. Bernhofs V. Engelmann D. Benner J. Gündert N. (2016). Neural biomarkers for dyslexia, ADHD, and ADD in the auditory cortex of children. Front. Neurosci. 10:324. 10.3389/fnins.2016.00324 27471442 Shahin A. Roberts L. E. Pantev C. Trainor L. J. Ross B. (2005). Modulation of P2 auditory-evoked responses by the spectral complexity of musical sounds. Neuroreport 16 17811785. 10.1097/01.wnr.0000185017.29316.63 Sharma A. Martin K. Roland P. Bauer P. Sweeney M. H. Gilley P. (2005). P1 latency as a biomarker for central auditory development in children with hearing impairment. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 16 564573. 10.3766/jaaa.16.8.5 16295243 Sieroka N. (2005). Quasi-hearing in Husserl, Levinson, and Gordon. J. Br. Soc. Phenomenol. 36 422. 10.1080/00071773.2005.11007461 Skoe E. Krizman J. Anderson S. Kraus N. (2015). Stability and plasticity of auditory brainstem function across the lifespan. Cereb. Cortex 25 14151426. 10.1093/cercor/bht311 24366906 Slater J. Skoe E. Strait D. L. O’Connell S. Thompson E. Kraus N. (2015). Music training improves speech-in-noise perception: Longitudinal evidence from a community-based music program. Behav. Brain Res. 291 244252. 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.05.026 26005127 Sur S. Sinha V. K. (2009). Event-related potential: An overview. Ind. Psychiatry J. 18 7083. 10.4103/0972-6748.57865 21234168 Tervaniemi M. Hugdahl K. (2003). Lateralization of auditory-cortex functions. Brain Res. Rev. 43 231246. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2003.08.004 14629926 Tierney A. T. Krizman J. Kraus N. (2015). Music training alters the course of adolescent auditory development. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112 1006210067. 10.1073/pnas.1505114112 26195739 Trainor L. J. Desjardins R. N. Rockel C. (1999). A comparison of contour and interval processing in musicians and nonmusicians using event-related potentials. Austral. J. Psychol. 51 147153. 10.1080/00049539908255352 Virtala P. Partanen E. Tervaniemi M. Kujala T. (2018). Neural discrimination of speech sound changes in a variable context occurs irrespective of attention and explicit awareness. Biol. Psychol. 132 217227. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.01.002 29305875 Wan C. Y. Schlaug G. (2010). Music making as a tool for promoting brain plasticity across the life span. Neuroscientist 16 566577. 10.1177/1073858410377805 20889966 Warner R. M. (2012). Applied statistics: From bivariate through multivariate techniques. (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications), 726778. Wengenroth M. Blatow M. Bendszus M. Schneider P. (2010). Leftward lateralization of auditory cortex underlies holistic sound perception in Williams syndrome. PLoS One 5:e12326. 10.1371/journal.pone.0012326 20808792 Wengenroth M. Blatow M. Heinecke A. Reinhardt J. Stippich C. Hofmann E. (2014). Increased volume and function of right auditory cortex as a marker for absolute pitch. Cereb. Cortex 24 11271137. 10.1093/cercor/bhs391 23302811 Wolfensteller U. von Cramon D. Y. (2011). Strategy-effects in prefrontal cortex during learning of higher-order S–R rules. Neuroimage 57 598607. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.048 21554965 Wong P. C. M. Skoe E. Russo N. M. Dees T. Kraus N. (2007). Musical experience shapes human brainstem encoding of linguistic pitch patterns. Nat. Neurosci. 10 420422. 10.1038/nn1872 17351633 Zatorre R. J. Belin P. (2001). Spectral and temporal processing in human auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 11 946953. 10.1093/cercor/11.10.946 11549617 Zatorre R. J. Chen J. L. Penhune V. B. (2007). When the brain plays music: Auditory–motor interactions in music perception and production. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8 547558. 10.1038/nrn2152 17585307 Zatorre R. J. Salimpoor V. N. (2013). From perception to pleasure: Music and its neural substrates. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110 1043010437. 10.1073/pnas.1301228110 23754373
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016www.holdzhu.net.cn
      www.fupwdo.com.cn
      hunlizhe.com.cn
      gtwfsl.com.cn
      gedan888.org.cn
      gxxsgj.com.cn
      www.iccon.com.cn
      ohf2e.org.cn
      www.qrynje.com.cn
      www.tysapi.org.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p