Front. Neurosci. Frontiers in Neuroscience Front. Neurosci. 1662-453X Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fnins.2018.00246 Neuroscience Conceptual Analysis Music Evolution in the Laboratory: Cultural Transmission Meets Neurophysiology Lumaca Massimo 1 * Ravignani Andrea 2 3 4 Baggio Giosuè 5 1Center for Music in the Brain, Department of Clinical Medicine, Aarhus University and The Royal Academy of Music Aarhus/Aalborg, Aarhus, Denmark 2Artificial Intelligence Lab, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium 3Research Department, Sealcentre Pieterburen, Pieterburen, Netherlands 4Language and Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, Netherlands 5Language Acquisition and Language Processing Lab, Department of Language and Literature, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway

Edited by: Aleksey Nikolsky, Independent Researcher, Los Angeles, CA, United States

Reviewed by: McNeel Gordon Jantzen, Western Washington University, United States; Laura Verga, Maastricht University, Netherlands; Vera Kempe, Abertay University, United Kingdom; Seana Coulson, University of California, San Diego, United States

*Correspondence: Massimo Lumaca massimo.lumaca@gmail.com

This article was submitted to Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience, a section of the journal Frontiers in Neuroscience

16 04 2018 2018 12 246 21 09 2017 29 03 2018 Copyright © 2018 Lumaca, Ravignani and Baggio. 2018 Lumaca, Ravignani and Baggio

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

In recent years, there has been renewed interest in the biological and cultural evolution of music, and specifically in the role played by perceptual and cognitive factors in shaping core features of musical systems, such as melody, harmony, and rhythm. One proposal originates in the language sciences. It holds that aspects of musical systems evolve by adapting gradually, in the course of successive generations, to the structural and functional characteristics of the sensory and memory systems of learners and “users” of music. This hypothesis has found initial support in laboratory experiments on music transmission. In this article, we first review some of the most important theoretical and empirical contributions to the field of music evolution. Next, we identify a major current limitation of these studies, i.e., the lack of direct neural support for the hypothesis of cognitive adaptation. Finally, we discuss a recent experiment in which this issue was addressed by using event-related potentials (ERPs). We suggest that the introduction of neurophysiology in cultural transmission research may provide novel insights on the micro-evolutionary origins of forms of variation observed in cultural systems.

cultural transmission diffusion chains signaling games iterated learning music universals music diversity neural predictors mismatch negativity (MMN)

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      There has recently been a surge of interest in the biological and cultural origins, and evolution of music (Wallin et al., 2001; McDermott and Hauser, 2005; Patel, 2010). Music is prominent in virtually all human societies, and in its most sophisticated versions it is only attested in humans. This fact raises two important questions: how did music originate? And how did it evolve in its current forms? One intriguing issue here, especially in relation to the cognitive and neural bases of music evolution (Honing et al., 2015), is that of the evolution of musical structure. Musical systems are structured at several levels, from melody and harmony to rhythm and composition, in ways that may resemble the organization of other human generative systems, such as language (Jackendoff and Lerdahl, 1983; Jackendoff, 2009). The analogy between language and music may be pushed further, if one considers aspects of music that may be understood “semantically.” Listening to music can evoke a wide range of extra-musical experiences, from emotional feelings (e.g., the sadness suggested by Albinoni's Adagio in G minor) to the mental imagery of specific referents (e.g., characters or ideas in Wagnerian Leitmotifs) (Patel, 2010). Musical structures can and often do relate to a world of possible experiences and non-musical phenomena (Lerdahl, 2003) expressively (by being associated to internal affective states, e.g., emotional qualities), if not representationally (via relations of reference and truth, as language does) (Patel, 2010).

      In this work, we focus on the cultural origins of musical syntax: the set of principles governing the combination of melody and rhythm into “well-formed” sequences (for a discussion on the evolution of semantic structures see Lumaca and Baggio, 2017, 2018; Ravignani and Verhoef, 2018). Some aspects of musical syntax, such as the organization of temporal structure and pitch intervals, display widespread distribution and striking cross-cultural similarities. For example, the tendency to use small intervals in non-polyphonic melodic phrases, or “proximity,” has been observed across several musical traditions of the world, including indigenous tunes from North America, Europe, and Asia (Dowling, 1968; Von Hippel, 2000). Despite some exceptions, such as Scandinavian and Swiss yodeling music, proximity is a prominent feature of melodic structure. These shared attributes are known as “musical universals.” Nevertheless, their form and frequency differ across and within different musical traditions of the world (Lomax, 1977; Rzeszutek et al., 2012; Savage et al., 2015). How can we explain both the invariance and the variation of structure in music? Which processes underlie the cross-cultural convergence toward common music traits or their diversification? In this paper, we suggest that neuroscience can provide critical methodological and theoretical tools for testing and generating hypotheses on this complex matter.

      This article is organized as follows. We start by presenting a recent theoretical perspective in which music is understood as an evolving cultural system, adapting to the human brain [sections Linking Biological and Cultural Levels of Analysis and From Cultural Transmission to Neurophysiology (and Back)]. In section The Cognitive Level: Diffusion Chains and the Evolution of Musical Regularities in the Lab, we describe studies that support this view using data from behavioral experiments. In section The Neural Level: Constraints Imposed by a Neuronal Niche Drive the Emergence of Regularities, we transpose our analysis of cultural adaptation to the neural level. Partly using the “neuronal recycling hypothesis” as a theoretical framework (Dehaene and Cohen, 2007), we argue that music can adapt to a “neuronal niche” defined by the specific information processing constraints imposed by neural circuits originally evolved for auditory streaming.

      To our knowledge, no one until recently has investigated this hypothesis by means of brain imaging or neurophysiology. In section Neural Predictors in Cultural Evolution Research, we describe a recent experiment in which this hypothesis was tested combining behavioral and neurophysiological methods. Finally (section The Neural Origins of Cultural Variation), we suggest that the introduction of concepts and methods from neuroscience in music evolution, and cultural evolution in general, can provide new insights on the process of cultural variation.

      Linking biological and cultural levels of analysis

      Music may be seen as a complex adaptive system, shaped by various biological, environmental, and cultural factors. This has made it difficult for musicologists and cognitive scientists to analyze the evolutionary origins of musical structure. The predominant view during the last century was the cultural account, where music was seen as an entirely socio-cultural construct, free to vary with virtually no biological and environmental constraints on its structure and content (Nettl, 1983; Repp, 1991; Blacking et al., 1995). The striking diversity of musical forms, as attested across and within cultures, and over human history, seems to support this notion (Lomax, 1968; Henry, 1976). Yet, this account has been challenged by experiments in psychology and neuroscience, together supporting a broadly biological account of the origins of music. Several studies point to the existence of perceptuo-cognitive biases and constraints in music processing and production (e.g., Trehub, 2000; Drake and Bertrand, 2001; Zatorre, 2001; Peretz and Zatorre, 2005; Deutsch, 2012) with some parallels in other species (Fitch, 2015). On this view, prototypical properties of music, such as a relatively steady beat, smooth melodic contours, tonality, and a narrow distance between adjacent tones (or “pitch proximity”), derive from built-in functional properties of the brain (McDermott and Hauser, 2005), which tend to manifest themselves in most human cultures (Lerdahl, 1992; Savage et al., 2015).

      A recent view is that neither the “cultural account” nor the “biological account” can independently provide a satisfactory theory of the origins and evolution of musical structure (Trainor, 2015). Cultural accounts typically focus on the evolution of musical systems, while biological accounts investigate the evolution of the human capacity to perceive, appreciate, and produce music (also including musicality; Honing et al., 2015). These different accounts, however, may be connected within a more complete explanatory framework, if one accepts that music is neither an entirely arbitrary cultural construct nor strictly a biological product. Much like natural language, music is a cultural construct, which nonetheless rests upon, and is partly shaped by, human neurobiology. Our neurobiological makeup determines the scope and constraints of human auditory memory capacity, hierarchical sequence processing, attention, perceptual hearing threshold, and auditory scene analysis (Snyder, 2008; Deutsch, 2012). This is now a central tenet in the field of music cognition, and it is becoming increasingly accepted in cultural analyses of music, too. The open question is how neurobiological capacities, biases and constraints manifest themselves in actual musical systems (Trainor, 2015).

      From cultural transmission to neurophysiology (and back)

      Answering this question requires theories, models, and empirical data that can effectively bridge the gap between the classical chasms of (cultural) evolutionary science: between individual-level and population-level processes, micro-evolutionary and macro-evolutionary processes (Mesoudi, 2011). Specifically, one important question is how the individual's neurobiological endowment manifests itself in music at the population level. This issue was already known in linguistics as the “problem of linkage” (Kirby, 1999). A possible answer is “through cultural transmission.” Music, much like language, is not only a richly structured symbolic system, but also a set of behaviors that is maintained over time by intergenerational transmission (Morley, 2013; Le Bomin et al., 2016).

      During intergenerational transmission, cultural information must survive a “memory bottleneck” (Deacon, 1997): the set of all neurobiological biases or constraints that bind our capacity to infer (and store) the “rules” that govern a system of information1. The properties of the cultural system that fit best the human neurobiological filter—e.g., those that make information easier to process, encode, and recall—will have greater likelihood of being passed on to the next generation. If this view is correct, in the long run the neurobiological endowment of individuals should be reflected in the musical corpus at the population-level.

      This view of transmission, emphasizing adaptation of fast-changing cultural systems to a largely stable neurocognitive architecture, was developed in evolutionary linguistics to account for the emergence of structure in human languages, including putative linguistic universals (Christiansen and Chater, 2008). Recent methodological advances (Mesoudi, 2015; Edmiston et al., 2018) have provided support for this view in controlled laboratory conditions. In most experiments, groups of individuals engage in simple, controlled forms of knowledge transmission, for example from a participant (a sender) to another (a receiver), along a diffusion chain. Each participant represents a “generation,” and each interaction between participants allows for the passage of information across generations (Esper, 1925; Bartlett, 1932). The set of items transmitted along a diffusion chain (e.g., linguistic or musical phrases) is a finite sample drawn from the (infinite) set of items that learners have to generalize from. A challenge for research on cultural transmission is to show that core properties of the artificial systems being transmitted are also properties of the actual cultural systems being modeled and that the mechanisms at work in artificial conditions are also at work in real cultural evolution. In a landmark study, Kirby et al. (2008) showed how miniature “languages” emerge in the course of transmission from initial random associations of signals and meanings. When these pairings are transmitted across “generations” of participants, some regularities emerge, including compositionality (Hockett, 1960), as observed in human language. This result supports the view that core properties of language can be explained by the interplay of individual cognitive biases (sensu Brighton et al., 2005) and iterated cultural learning and transmission. Recent studies on animal models of cultural learning further support this conclusion (e.g., for non-human primates see Claidière et al., 2014; for a seminal study on zebra finches see Fehér et al., 2009).

      One way to start bridging this gap in the musical domain, is to assume that music, like language, is a complex adaptive cultural system, shaped for thousands of years by cycles of transmission, acquisition, and use (Morley, 2013). Following this view, neurobiological biases and constraints, as discussed above, brought out through cultural transmission, would exert effects on the form and structure of music (Merker et al., 2015; Trainor, 2015; Mehr et al., 2018). This mechanism could explain some properties of temporal (rhythm, meter) and spectral (melody, harmony) dimensions of musical structure, which are likely to be the result of adaptations to the combined pressures of neural constraints and various socio-cultural forces (Merker, 2006; Merker et al., 2015; Trainor, 2015). This would in principle apply to both invariants—putative cultural universals shared by musical systems or traditions (Savage et al., 2015)—and variation among individuals, generations, and traditions.

      This point is not new. Lévi-Strauss (1960) had already observed that some structural regularities observed across cultures (e.g., the fact that symbolic material tends to be organized in binary oppositions) are reflections of principles of brain organization. Therefore, neuroscience is expected to contribute to explanations of the emergence and evolution of structural regularities, including their convergence and diversity. However, to date this issue has been addressed only by behavioral studies, and only to explain some invariant aspects of musical structure. In the next section, we summarize three of these lines of experimental work in the field of music evolution.

      The cognitive level: diffusion chains and the evolution of musical regularities in the lab

      In recent experiments, a diffusion chain method was used to study how music evolves in the lab (Ravignani et al., 2016). This study aimed to test whether human psychological biases, amplified by cultural transmission, can explain the emergence of rhythmic universals (Trehub, 2015). In this experiment, participants were given a drumstick and an electronic drum pad. Participants in the first generation listened to 32 randomly generated, hence a-rhythmic, patterns of beats (the input), and were asked to reproduce each of them to the best of their abilities (the output). The “imperfect” output produced by this first generation of participants became the input for the next generation, whose task was to perform the rhythm they heard, and so on, along a diffusion chain. This paradigm is known as “iterated learning” (IL) (Kirby et al., 2008). Given the difficulty to memorize these patterns, errors were introduced in the emerging system of drumming sequences, slightly modifying the original patterns at each generation. Across generations, patterns became increasingly structured and easier to learn. After 8 generations, at the end of each diffusion chain, patterns showed regularities similar to those found across musical traditions of the world. These universal rhythmic regularities included a tendency toward small integer ratios (e.g., 1:1 and 2:1) of intervals between beat durations, and a relatively steady beat, also termed “isochrony” (Savage et al., 2015). This study represents the very first attempt to “grow” musical universals in the lab (Fitch, 2017), and sheds light on the cognitive and cultural mechanisms underlying the creation and vertical transmission of music (Le Bomin et al., 2016).

      An IL study by Verhoef (2012) investigated the cultural evolution of combinatorial structures in musical systems. Participants were first exposed to a set of 12 whistles that they had to imitate immediately after listening by using a slide whistle (training phase). Next, they were asked to reproduce the whole set of signals as they remembered it (recall phase). The sequences generated by a participant were used to train the next one in the diffusion chain, and so on, until the end of the chain. In the course of transmission, structural regularities emerged, as predicted by previous computer simulations (de Boer, 2000). In the last generations, fewer discrete units were reused by individuals in concatenations, repetitions, or mirror forms to produce the entire vocabulary of whistles. Combinatoriality is a “design feature” of human language (Hockett, 1960) and it applies to musical structure, too. For instance, the authors observed that two distinct whistles were often combined into a single pattern by the next generation of individuals. Also, participants tended to produce mirror forms out of single patterns, so that more elements were shared between signals of the same set. With fewer units to memorize, organized in this manner, the set of signals was more structured, more compressed, and easier to learn and reproduce.

      A more recent attempt to study music evolution in the lab is the work by Lumaca and Baggio (2017). The authors used a different model of cultural transmission than IL: multi-generational signaling games (MGSGs) (Moreno and Baggio, 2015; Nowak and Baggio, 2016). MGSGs are in essence an iterated variant of signaling games (Lewis, 1969; Skyrms, 2010) that combine basic aspects of semiotic models of coordination and communication (e.g., horizontal transmission; Galantucci and Garrod, 2011) with the intergenerational transmission of IL (Kirby et al., 2008). Two-person signaling games were organized in diffusion chains of 8 generations each. In each game, the sender and receiver were expected to converge, through repeated interactions, on a common code: a signaling system where 5 isochronous melodic riffs were associated to basic or compound emotions. This design can contribute to model different aspects of music transmission: first, a degree of alignment of internal states between musical senders (e.g. composers) and receivers (e.g., an audience) at two main levels, the structural and affective (Temperley, 2004; Bharucha et al., 2011); second, a partial asymmetry in information flow from senders to receivers, which is present in language and music transmission (e.g., from composers to listeners, from teachers to pupils, etc.). In each signaling trial, the sender was presented on the screen with one of the 5 equiprobable emotions (visualized as human facial expressions) and was asked to compose a 5-note isochronous riff on the computer keyboard. The receiver, after he listened to the riff via headphones, was asked to choose one of the 5 expressive faces displayed on the screen (i.e., the one possibly seen by the sender). A feedback was then presented simultaneously to both participants' screens, showing the expressive face seen by the sender and the one chosen by the receiver for the same melodic signal. This procedure was repeated at each successive trial. At the end of the game, the receiver (generation n) became the sender in the next game, with the same structure and a new participant as a receiver (generation n + 1), and so on, until the chain was completed. Senders were always asked to transmit the code they had learned in the previous game. Therefore, recall errors in the melodic signals (possibly “innovations”) were introduced throughout the experiment. The authors observed the gradual evolution over generations of several structural features of musical phrases: pitch proximity and continuity, symmetry, and motivic structure.

      Despite differences in their assumptions and methods, those three experiments have reached similar conclusions: the immediate effects of psychological constraints on the musical systems may be weak, but they are amplified in the course of inter-generational transmission (Boyd and Richerson, 1988; Kalish et al., 2007; Kirby et al., 2007; Thompson et al., 2016) or iterated reproduction (Jacoby and McDermott, 2017), leading the evolution of musical structures along non-random paths. If principles of auditory organization and memory constraints operate in similar ways also in the production and perception of actual music, they could similarly shape the evolution of historical systems in the course of iterated transmission. Convergence toward some of the musical structures found across populations (Savage et al., 2015) could be then explained, to some extent, by adaptation to a special niche, constituted by a restricted set of low-level perceptual and memory processes. In the rest of the paper we will refer to this special niche as “neuronal niche” (Dehaene and Cohen, 2007).

      The neural level: constraints imposed by a neuronal niche drive the emergence of regularities

      In recent years, there has been an increasing interest in how the brain accommodates and shapes novel cultural symbolic systems (Dehaene and Cohen, 2007). A leading hypothesis is that some cortical circuits, initially evolved as a result of specific selective pressures, are later “recycled” to accommodate novel cultural functions (Dehaene and Cohen, 2007; Simon et al., 2013; Dehaene et al., 2015; Skeide et al., 2017). Therefore, the acquisition of novel functions is constrained, however weakly, by prior human evolution. Once “culturally recycled,” pre-existing systems and mechanisms maintain some of their original capacities and limitations, providing a neuronal niche within which culture may adapt and evolve. This also means that the variability observed in cultural systems is limited by brain structure and function across individuals and groups.

      If this hypothesis is correct, near-universal characteristics of music (Savage et al., 2015) may be traced back to the computational infrastructure of human auditory cortex and other (e.g., motor, attentional etc.) areas of the brain. Trainor (2015) related the origins of certain invariant musical features as adaptations to bottom-up neural mechanisms of auditory scene analysis (ASA), such as the sequential sound segregation and integration of within-stream elements (Bregman, 1994). These specific mechanisms have evolved specifically to detect and localize multiple sources of auditory objects and to extract regularities from the acoustic environment. They often involve the perceptual grouping of single-event auditory stimuli into auditory streams and operate following Gestalt principles of proximity, similarity, and continuity (Deutsch, 1999). They are automatic (pre-attentive), they emerge early in human development (Demany, 1982; Winkler et al., 2003), and they are widely conserved across species (Fay, 2008). This point shows that the ASA neural circuitry is likely phylogenetically older than human music. Thus, the exaptation (or evolutionary re-use) (Gould and Vrba, 1982) of this more ancient biological mechanism by music should impose constraints on the way music is stored and organized in the brain, and accordingly, on the way it is recalled during transmission. In this regard, perceptual and memory recall advantages have been reported for tone streams that conform to Gestalt principles of organization (Bendixen et al., 2010; Loui, 2012; Rohrmeier and Cross, 2013). The cross-cultural tendency to organize music following these principles (Huron, 2001), in addition to the findings reported by cultural transmission research (Verhoef, 2012; Ravignani et al., 2016; Lumaca and Baggio, 2017), may support the idea that the neurocomputational constraints of the human auditory system constitute a filter through which musical material must pass, adapt, and eventually evolve.

      It is surprising that up until recently, no one has attempted to find (counter-) evidence of cultural adaptation using neural measures. Research has shown that even recently-encoded information is shaped by perceptual or memory constraints into more compressed and abstract forms (Tamariz, 2017). Yet, the neural mechanisms underlying this phenomenon remain unknown. One reason is arguably our limited understanding of how information is represented in the brain (Mesoudi et al., 2006). Current whole-brain methods, such as functional magnetic resonance (fMRI), are not well-suited to investigate the precise basis of mental representations (but see Haynes and Rees, 2006; Johnson and Johnson, 2014; Zadbood et al., 2017). Another issue is to establish a link between neural constraints on learning—neural activity underlying specific, fast, and accurate encoding processes (Sadtler et al., 2014)—and cultural adaptation. Electrophysiological methods, such as multi-unit recordings, seem ideal for this purpose, but they are too invasive to be performed on healthy individuals. Various animal models of social learning—in songbirds, primates, and other species—have provided useful information in this respect (Araki et al., 2016; Gadagkar et al., 2016; Tchernichovski and Lipkind, 2016). None of these species possesses cultural behaviors as rich and complex as human music. However, some of their behaviors exhibit structured patterns, which are maintained over time through inter-generational transmission. Cultural transmission, in turn, can shape animal vocal behavior so as to fit species-specific learning constraints (Fehér et al., 2009; Fitch, 2009).

      The application of techniques and models used in language evolution allow researchers of animal behavior to explore the biology of culturally transmitted systems in simpler and more controlled conditions, and to answer questions about cultural adaptation that cannot be directly answered in humans using current methods (but see next section for indirect answers). For example, Araki et al. (2016) used cellular recordings to demonstrate the existence in zebra finches of constraints on neuronal temporal coding that limit song acquisition to certain species-specific temporal features. Juvenile birds acquire their songs by imitating adult tutors. Although zebra finches are not bound to learn only specific sequences, they do show significant consistencies in their vocal repertoires (Lachlan et al., 2016). Do these consistencies result from adaptation of song material to the zebra finch neural constraints on learning? Araki et al. (2016) found that a subset of neurons in the zebra finch auditory cortex responds synchronously and selectively to patterns of inter-syllable silent gap durations, which are typical of their songs. The same cell population was unresponsive to other species' songs. Temporal coding mechanism like this are thought to preserve the species-specific song identity from any random drifts that may be introduced during cultural transmission.

      Critically, the same mechanisms might underlie learning behaviors that resemble cultural adaptation in humans. When presented with the songs of other species, zebra finches tend to gradually adjust the duration of inter-syllable intervals toward their own (species-specific) songs' temporal structures, in a way similar to the human adjustment of random auditory stimuli toward Gestalt features. To our knowledge, this work provides the first cellular-level support of the idea of a neurobiological basis of cultural adaptation. It remains to be determined to what extent their findings can be generalized to other species. Would similar neuronal constraints operate in humans? Could they explain perceptual predispositions for some musical features (e.g., for small intervals and isochronous beat)? Are those neuronal constraints species-specific or, instead, are they shared with other species (Nicolai et al., 2014)? Another critical question is whether inter-individual variability in the neural filter is reflected in forms of cultural variation, for example in participant behavior during transmission, or in the shape taken by cultural systems as a result of it. Cross-individual variability is typically regarded as a source of noise in cultural transmission research, and is often removed by means of various procedures. The idea of linking individual neural variability with cultural variation may lend itself well to investigations using brain imaging and electrophysiology, but no one until recently has adopted this approach in cultural transmission research.

      Neural predictors in cultural evolution research

      In a recent experiment, Lumaca and Baggio (2016) addressed some of these issues using a neural predictors approach (Berkman and Falk, 2013). This entails use of neuroimaging (fMRI, PET) or electrophysiological methods (EEG/ERPs, MEG) to identify neural predictors of behavior (for examples in the music domain, see Golestani et al., 2002; Zatorre et al., 2012; Zatorre, 2013). Lumaca and Baggio (2016) used neural predictors of signaling behavior as a first approach to examine whether and how symbolic systems adapt to human neural information processing systems, and to assess the effects of inter-individual variation in neural information processing on three core cultural behaviors: social learning, transmission, and regularization of signal sequences. To this purpose, the authors used one of the best-investigated brain signatures of auditory processing, the mismatch negativity (MMN) (Näätänen et al., 1978).

      The MMN is a fronto-central negative wave, evoked by violations of some perceptual regularity (Paavilainen, 2013) which is picked up by the brain in a visual or auditory stimulus stream. The limited influence of attentive processes on the MNN (Paavilainen, 2013) and its onset (~200 ms from the relevant stimulus) suggest that the MMN is a low-level marker of auditory processing. The encoding of regularities from an auditory input, possibly through the same ASA mechanisms reported above, is an antecedent condition for the elicitation of the MMN (Näätänen et al., 2001). The efficiency of these mechanisms is revealed by the MMN latency and amplitude (Näätänen et al., 1993; Tervaniemi et al., 2001). Larger amplitudes or shorter latencies are typically associated to more accurate representations of the input material and, thus, they are taken as proxies of more efficient encoding mechanisms. The MMN has been used to study how efficiently an individual's auditory system extracts and encodes regularities from acoustic inputs, and how this process may affect linguistic and musical behaviors. For example, differences in ERP responses in infants have been successfully used in various studies to predict cognitive and linguistic development (Molfese and Molfese, 1997; Choudhury and Benasich, 2011). Overall, these studies open up the possibility of using low-level neural markers to predict individual behavior during transmission and acquisition of language, music, and cultural material more generally. Structural properties of symbolic systems may thus be understood as adaptations to information processing bottlenecks during cultural transmission (Kirby, 2001; Tamariz and Kirby, 2015). It should then be possible, for example, to find a relationship between individual brain processing capabilities or limitations, and the degree of regularization imposed by each individual on the cultural material that is being transmitted and acquired.

      Neurophysiological (ERP) evidence for this type of effect was provided by Lumaca and Baggio (2016) in the domain of melodic structure. The authors combined ERPs with diffusion chains on two successive days. On day 1, they identified a neural correlate of extracting regularities from 5-tone sequences in musically naïve individuals in a classical auditory oddball paradigm. ERPs were recorded while participants were presented with randomly interleaved standard (80%) and deviant (20%) stimuli: there was no task for the participants, who were watching a silent movie throughout the session. On day 2, participants played a reduced version of MGSGs, with melodic systems of the same kind used by Lumaca and Baggio (2017). Each participant played the first signaling game as receiver (learner) and the second as sender (transmitter)2. The main question addressed by the authors was whether constraints and biases on auditory processing could drive the melodic material toward known Gestalt principles of perceptual organization (Lumaca and Baggio, 2017). The results showed that inter-individual variation in neural information processing, as revealed by the latency of the MMN on day 1, predicted learning and transmission of melodic signaling systems in the MGSGs on day 2. Specifically, individuals with longer MMN latencies performed “worse” in the MGSGs, showing lower coordination, transmission, and accuracy. Yet, these participants introduced more innovations than participants with shorter MMN latencies. Inter-individual variation in neural auditory processing (or regularity encoding) may be sufficient to discriminate “better” from “worse” transmitters, as observed in the cultural transmission of music (Sawa, 2002). However, perhaps the most interesting finding was that participants with longer MMN latencies introduced more regularities in the artificial tone system, reproducing more often melodic structures that were more compressed (signals from the same set became more similar), more proximal (temporally adjacent elements in the signals were closer in pitch), and smoother (the sequences showed a coherent melodic direction) than the sequences they originally received. To our knowledge, this study is the first demonstration that three essential processes underlying cultural evolution (i.e., social learning, transmission, and innovation), and three near-universal properties of melodic structure (i.e., proximity, continuity, and compression) are constrained by the organization of sensory and memory systems in the brain. The MMN is only “the tip of the iceberg” here. The MMN is likely to reflect auditory scene analysis and encoding mechanisms. Constraints on these mechanisms, as revealed (among others) by MMN latencies, may represent a “neuronal niche” through which cultural material must pass, adapt, and evolve (see below). In a cultural evolutionary context, this finding may provide clues to the origins of forms of variation observed in cultural symbolic systems. We discuss this point in the next paragraph.

      The neural origins of cultural variation

      Human cultural traits show a myriad different forms across world cultures. Music, like language, provides an excellent example of this diversity, within and between populations (Lomax, 1959; Rzeszutek et al., 2012). For instance, the tendency toward the use of intervals of small size or the division of the octave (2:1) into a limited number of tones (or “discreteness”) as observed in several cultures (Merriam et al., 1956; Dowling, 1968) is counterbalanced by significant diversity, within and between those cultures, in the relative frequency of such traits (Savage et al., 2015). The frequency distribution of proximal intervals (<700 cents; Savage et al., 2015) differs across musical traditions, with variation being mostly confined to the interval range 0 (unison) to 6 semitones (Huron, 2001). A similar diversity was found in the “tonal material” of musical cultures (i.e., the total set of discrete pitches within an octave), which spans from the 12 semitones of the Western musical scale to the 22–24 microtonal steps of North Indian and Arabic scales (Malm, 1967; Ayari and McAdams, 2003).

      The evolutionary mechanisms that affect the relative frequency of musical characters, such as random cultural drifts and biased selection, have been extensively studied in recent years (Mesoudi, 2015). For example, MacCallum et al. (2012) used a biologically-inspired evolutionary system to explore the effects of “aesthetic” selection on the frequency distribution of musical characters. A population of listeners was asked to rate the pleasantness of randomly generated tunes. The top-rated tunes recombined or mutated into novel variants that were in turn evaluated by a new generation of consumers. The authors reported an over-time increase of characters classically regarded as “musical,” such as isochrony and chordal clarity. This work was the first of its kind to show that consumers' preferences can deeply shape the evolution of music in the near absence of learning and memory pressures. It is still controversial whether aesthetic preferences are just a social construct, changing over time, or if instead they are themselves stable information processing biases (for an in-depth discussion on this topic see Hodges, 2009; Huron, 2009). In a recent model, Reber et al. (2004) combined the two proposals. Specifically, the authors put forward the hypothesis that aesthetic preferences result from an interaction between knowledge-dependent stylistic rules and information processing fluency for certain stimulus properties (e.g., symmetry, clarity, and the amount of information content) (Nieminen et al., 2011). This may explain the evolution of music toward specific features, such as symmetry and chordal clarity (MacCallum et al., 2012; Verhoef, 2012; Lumaca and Baggio, 2017). A similar proposal was made by Haiman (2011) to explain the emergence of symmetric compounds in language. These arguments are still hypothetical, but we are now starting to understand the effects of these biases on the cultural evolution of music (Savage and Brown, 2007). Specifically, we know that these processes can enhance the diversity of musical behaviors and forms, but they can also produce local homogeneity3. While those mechanisms can explain how musical variants spread over time in a population, the sources of variability remain to a large extent elusive.

      Up until now, only four main mechanisms of variation have been considered in music: creative innovation (e.g., via original musical composition), borrowing (through blending or syncretism), translation (from one tonal system to another; Alekseyev, 1986), and random mutation (errors in music copying or performance) (Savage and Brown, 2007). Lumaca and Baggio (2016) provided evidence for an additional mechanism: individual neural variability. One could argue that every individual in a population represents a distinct and unique “neuronal niche” (Dehaene and Cohen, 2007), through which cultural material is filtered and to which it may eventually adapt. Minor inter-individual differences in neural information processing can manifest themselves in differences in musical behavior. Moreover, they can be amplified and spread via different cultural evolutionary mechanisms. Small differences in learning or information processing can have large system-level effects, if they are amplified by cultural transmission.

      One tenet of cultural transmission research is that cultural systems evolve toward certain prior distributions, known as “cognitive attractors” or “inductive biases” (Sperber, 1996; Griffiths et al., 2008). Strong versions of this account have been challenged by recent modeling work (Navarro et al., 2017). The convergence toward priors holds in the (implausible) scenario where all learners are endowed with the same identical prior. However, when learners instantiate (slightly) different constraints, the emerging cultural systems may reflect the more idiosyncratic biases of some individuals. In light of our findings, one could suggest that individuals with “tighter bottlenecks” exert a disproportionately large effect on the evolution of musical structures (see Ravignani et al., 2018 for some issues concerning this view). Similarly, differences between populations in brain function and anatomy may, at least in part, be reflected in differences in the structure of the symbolic systems in use. This account has recently found some support in language evolution research. Dediu and Ladd (2007) have shown that the population-level frequencies of two human genes involved in brain growth, Microcephalin and ASPM, are reliably associated with the presence or absence of linguistic tones in that population. The authors' proposal is that variants of these genes may determine small biases at the individual level in the processing and acquisition of linguistic tones, which may in turn give rise to distinct language variants. Those variants are hardly detectable in individual subjects, because tonal and non-tonal languages can be acquired by any individual, independently of genetic variants (Ladd et al., 2008). But when their effects are amplified by inter-generational transmission (Kirby et al., 2008), these variants may give rise to measurable, large-scale population differences.

      Dediu and Ladd (2007) is the first study suggesting that variation, as observed in cultural traits and in their distribution, may originate in interindividual neurogenetic variability. Lumaca and Baggio (2016) provide converging neurophysiological evidence in support of this view (for the genetic bases of inter-individual variation in musicality, see Gingras et al., 2015). Genetic and neural variability are not the only source of cultural variation, but they are likely to play a prominent role in any future theory of the biological roots of culture. For example, Brown et al. (2014) have shown that musical and genetic diversity may correlate to some degree. After sampling a set of traditional songs from 9 indigenous populations in Taiwan, they measured the relative distance for 41 properties of song structure and performance-style. Music and genetic distance among the populations were significantly correlated. A similar relation was found in Eurasian populations (Pamjav et al., 2012). The study of genetic and neural variability may help address questions that were considered taboo in ethnomusicology since fairly recently: for example, whether a causal relationship exists between the distribution of some gene variants and aspects of musical systems and behaviors (Jordania, 2006, p. 101; Nikolsky, 2015). Such a theory requires the synergic and coordinated effort of genetics, neuroscience, and research on cultural evolution. The recent drive toward a “grand synthesis” of the latter discipline (Brewer et al., 2017) makes this possibility somewhat more likely.

      Conclusions

      In this paper, we have argued that some of the most fundamental (and still unresolved) issues in music evolution can be addressed using the methods of cognitive neuroscience. This approach so far suggests a novel hypothesis on the mechanisms behind forms of cultural variation in musical systems. This line of work can also shed light on the “problem of linkage” (Kirby, 1999). Up until recently, this problem has been framed at only two levels of explanation. At the behavioral level, individual behaviors (e.g., code changes) that serve coordination and communication are linked to population-level patterns. At the cognitive level, sensory or memory constraints in individuals are identified in order to account for properties (e.g., structural features) of cultural systems. We suggest that a third level, the neural level, should be taken into consideration when developing accounts of the origins and evolution of structure in cultural systems, as is the case for accounts of the organization and function of information processing systems (Marr, 1982; Baggio et al., 2012, 2014, 2016). Thus, we can address questions in the cultural domain such as: which sources produce cultural diversity (computational level); through which mechanisms it may arise (e.g., inter-individual variation; algorithmic level); and which physical substrates, if any, those mechanisms exploit (i.e., the human brain; implementational level). We believe that explanations at all three levels are necessary to understand human cultural transmission. This requires (1) analyzing the structural and dynamic properties of the cultural systems (or codes) themselves, (2) determining how those are shaped by perceptual and cognitive biases and constraints, and (3) identifying the biological roots of such biases and constraints using neural and genetic data. This proposal generates several new questions, such as: to what extent do neural processes drive cultural evolution? How does inter-individual variation in brain function and structure affect variation in cultural behaviors? How does the distribution of neural traits in a population affect the structure of the symbolic system itself? How do these traits interact with aesthetic processing biases and the environment at large in the cultural evolution of music? How specific and accurate can neuroprediction get in the context of cultural evolution? Here, we hope to have shown that these questions are worth asking, and are largely amenable to scientific inquiry.

      Author contributions

      ML wrote the article. AR and GB made additional contributions and edited the manuscript. All authors approved the manuscript for publication.

      Conflict of interest statement

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      We are grateful to Monica Tamariz, Bruno Gingras, and Aleksey Nikolsky for their helpful comments during the revision of the manuscript. Center for Music in the Brain is funded by the Danish National Research Foundation (DNRF117).

      References Alekseyev E. (1986). Early Folkloric Intonation. Pitch Aspect [Pаннефольклорное Интонирование: Звуковысотный Аспект]. Moscow: Sovetskii Kompozitor. Araki M. Bandi M. M. Yazaki-Sugiyama Y. (2016). Mind the gap: Neural coding of species identity in birdsong prosody. Science 354, 12821287. 10.1126/science.aah679927940872 Ayari M. McAdams S. (2003). Aural analysis of Arabic improvised instrumental music (taqsim). Music Percept. 21, 159216. 10.1525/mp.2003.21.2.159 Baggio G. Stenning K. van Lambalgen M. (2016). Semantics and Cognition, in The Cambridge Handbook of Formal Semantics, eds Aloni M. Dekker P. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 756774. Baggio G. van Lambalgen M. Hagoort P. (2012). Language, Linguistics and Cognition, in Handbook of the Philosophy of Linguistics, eds Kempson R. Fernando T. Asher N. (Amsterdam, NL: Elsevier), 325355. Baggio G. van Lambalgen M. Hagoort P. (2014). Logic as Marr's computational level: four case studies. Top. Cogn. Sci. 7, 287298. 10.1111/tops.1212525417838 Bartlett F. C. (1932). Remembering: An Experimental and Social Study. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Bendixen A. Denham S. L. Gyimesi K. Winkler I. (2010). Regular patterns stabilize auditory streams. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 128, 36583666. 10.1121/1.350069521218898 Berkman E. T. Falk E. B. (2013). Beyond brain mapping: using neural measures to predict real-world outcomes. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22, 4550. 10.1177/096372141246939424478540 Bharucha J. J. Paroo K. Curtis M. (2011). Alignment of brain states: response to commentaries, in Language and Music as Cognitive Systems, eds Rebuschat P. Rohrmeier M. Hawkins J. A. Cross I. (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 195198. Blacking J. Byron R. Nettl B. (1995). Music, Culture, and Experience: Selected Papers of John Blacking. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Boyd R. Richerson P. J. (1988). Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Bregman A. S. (1994). Auditory Scene Analysis: The Perceptual Organization of Sound. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Brewer J. Gelfand M. Jackson J. C. MacDonald I. F. Peregrine P. N. Richerson P. J. . (2017). Grand challenges for the study of cultural evolution. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 1:70. 10.1038/s41559-017-007028812714 Brighton H. Smith K. Kirby S. (2005). Language as an evolutionary system. Phys. Life Rev. 2, 177226. 10.1016/j.plrev.2005.06.001 Brown S. Savage P. E. Ko A. M. S. Stoneking M. Ko Y. C. Loo J. H. . (2014). Correlations in the population structure of music, genes and language. Proc. R. Soc. B. 281:20132072. 10.1098/rspb.2013.207224225453 Choudhury N. Benasich A. A. (2011). Maturation of auditory evoked potentials from 6 to 48 months: prediction to 3 and 4 year language and cognitive abilities. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 320338. 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.05.03520685161 Christiansen M. H. Chater N. (2008). Language as shaped by the brain. Behav. Brain Sci. 31, 489489. 10.1017/S0140525X0800499818826669 Claidière N. Smith K. Kirby S. Fagot J. (2014). Cultural evolution of systematically structured behaviour in a non-human primate. Proc. Biol. Sci. 281:20141541. 10.1098/rspb.2014.154125377450 Deacon T. (1997). The Symbolic Species. New York, NY: W.W.Norton. de Boer B. (2000). Self-organization in vowel systems. J. Phon. 28, 441465. 10.1006/jpho.2000.0125 Dediu D. Ladd D. R. (2007). Linguistic tone is related to the population frequency of the adaptive haplogroups of two brain size genes, ASPM and Microcephalin. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 1094410949. 10.1073/pnas.061084810417537923 Dehaene S. Cohen L. (2007). Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron 56, 384398. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.00417964253 Dehaene S. Cohen L. Morais J. Kolinsky R. (2015). Illiterate to literate: behavioural and cerebral changes induced by reading acquisition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 234244. 10.1038/nrn392425783611 Demany L. (1982). Auditory stream segregation in infancy. Infant Behav. Dev. 5, 261276. 10.1016/S0163-6383(82)80036-2 Deutsch D. (1999). Grouping mechanisms in music, in The Psychology of Music 2nd Edn, ed Deutsch D. (San Diego, CA: Academy Press), 99134. Deutsch D. (2012). The Psychology of Music 3rd edn. San Diego, CA: Academy Press. Dowling W. J. (1968). Rhythmic fission and perceptual organization. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 44:369. 10.1121/1.1970461 Drake C. Bertrand D. (2001). The quest for universals in temporal processing in music. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 930, 1727. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05722.x11458828 Edmiston P. Perlman M. Lupyan G. (2018). Repeated imitation makes human vocalizations more word-like. Proc. R. Soc. B 285:20172709. 10.1098/rspb.2017.270929514962 Esper E. A. (1925). A technique for the experiment investigation of associative interference in artificial linguistic material. Lang. Monographs 1, 147. Fay R. R. (2008). Sound source perception and stream segregation in nonhuman vertebrate animals, in Auditory Perception and Sound Sources, eds Yost W. A. Popper A. N. Fay R. R. (New York, NY: Springer), 307323. Fehér O. Wang H. Saar S. Mitra P. P. Tchernichovski O. (2009). De novo establishment of wild-type song culture in the zebra finch. Nature 459, 564568. 10.1038/nature0799419412161 Fitch W. T. (2009). Animal behaviour: birdsong normalized by culture. Nature 459, 519520. 10.1038/459519a19478774 Fitch W. T. (2015). Four principles of bio-musicology. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 370:20140091. 10.1098/rstb.2014.009125646514 Fitch W. T. (2017). Cultural evolution: lab-cultured musical universals. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1:0018. 10.1038/s41562-016-0018 Gadagkar V. Puzerey P. A. Chen R. Baird-Daniel E. Farhang A. R. Goldberg J. H. (2016). Dopamine neurons encode performance error in singing birds. Science 354, 12781282. 10.1126/science.aah683727940871 Galantucci B. Garrod S. (2011). Experimental semiotics: a review. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:11. 10.3389/fnhum.2011.0001121369364 Gingras B. Honing H. Peretz I. Trainor L. J. Fisher S. E. (2015). Defining the biological bases of individual differences in musicality. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 370:20140092. 10.1098/rstb.2014.009225646515 Golestani N. Paus T. Zatorre R. J. (2002). Anatomical correlates of learning novel speech sounds. Neuron 35, 9971010. 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00862-012372292 Gould S. J. Vrba E. S. (1982). Exaptation—a missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology 8, 415. 10.1017/S0094837300004310 Griffiths T. L. Kalish M. L. Lewandowsky S. (2008). Review. Theoretical and empirical evidence for the impact of inductive biases on cultural evolution. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363, 35033514. 10.1098/rstb.2008.014618801717 Haiman J. (2011). Competing motivations, in The Oxford handbook of Linguistic Typology, ed Song J. J. (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press), 148165. Haynes J.-D. Rees G. (2006). Decoding mental states from brain activity in humans. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 523534. 10.1038/nrn193116791142 Henry E. O. (1976). The variety of music in a north Indian village: reassessing cantometrics. Ethnomusicology 20:49. 10.2307/850820 Hockett C. F. (1960). The origin of speech. Sci. Am. 203, 8996. 10.1038/scientificamerican0960-8814402211 Hodges D. A. (2009). The neuroaesthetics of music, in The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology, eds Hallam S. Cross I. Thaut M. (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press), 247262. Honing H. ten Cate C. Peretz I. Trehub S. E. (2015). Without it no music: cognition, biology and evolution of musicality. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 370:20140088. 10.1098/rstb.2014.008825646511 Huron D. (2001). Tone and voice: a derivation of the rules of voice-leading from perceptual principles. Music Percept. 19, 164. 10.1525/mp.2001.19.1.1 Huron D. (2009). Aesthetics, in The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology, eds Hallam S. Cross I. Thaut M. (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press), 151159. Jackendoff R. (2009). Parallels and nonparallels between language and music. Music Percept. 26, 195204. 10.1525/mp.2009.26.3.195 Jackendoff R. S. Lerdahl F. (1983). A Generative Theory of Tonal Music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Jacoby N. McDermott J. H. (2017). Integer ratio priors on musical rhythm revealed cross-culturally by iterated reproduction. Curr. Biol. 27, 359370. 10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.03128065607 Johnson M. R. Johnson M. K. (2014). Decoding individual natural scene representations during perception and imagery. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:59. 10.3389/fnhum.2014.0005924574998 Jordania J. (2006). Who Asked the First Question? The Origins of Human Choral Singing, Intelligence, Language and Speech. Tbilisi: Logos. Kalish M. L. Griffiths T. L. Lewandowsky S. (2007). Iterated learning: intergenerational knowledge transmission reveals inductive biases. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 14, 288294. 10.3758/BF03194066 Kirby S. (1999). Function, Selection, and Innateness: The Emergence of Language Universals. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Kirby S. (2001). Spontaneous evolution of linguistic structure-an iterated learning model of the emergence of regularity and irregularity. IEEE Trans. Evol. Comput. 5, 102110. 10.1109/4235.918430 Kirby S. Cornish H. Smith K. (2008). Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: an experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 1068110686. 10.1073/pnas.070783510518667697 Kirby S. Dowman M. Griffiths T. L. (2007). Innateness and culture in the evolution of language. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 52415245. 10.1073/pnas.060822210417360393 Lachlan R. F. van Heijningen C. A. A. Ter Haar S. M. Ten Cate C. (2016). Zebra finch song phonology and syntactical structure across populations and continents—a computational comparison. Front. Psychol. 7:980. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.0098027458396 Ladd D. R. Dediu D. Kinsella A. R. (2008). Languages and genes: reflections on biolinguistics and the nature-nurture question. Biolinguistics 2, 114126. Le Bomin S. Lecointre G. Heyer E. (2016). The evolution of musical diversity: the key role of vertical transmission. PLoS ONE 11:e0151570. 10.1371/journal.pone.015157027027305 Lerdahl F. (1992). Cognitive constraints on compositional systems. Contemp. Music Rev. 6, 97121. 10.1080/07494469200640161 Lerdahl F. (2003). Two Ways in which music relates to the world. Music Theor. Spectr. 25, 367373. 10.1525/mts.2003.25.2.367 Lévi-Strauss C. (1960). Éloge de l'anthropologie. Inaugural Lecture at Collège de France. Lewis D. (1969). Convention: A Philosophical Study. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Lomax A. (1959). Folk song style. Am. Anthropol. 61, 927954. 10.1525/aa.1959.61.6.02a00030 Lomax A. (1968). Folk song Style and Culture. Washington, DC: American Association for the Advancement of Science. Lomax A. (1977). Universals in song. World Music 19, 117129. Loui P. (2012). Learning and liking of melody and harmony: further studies in artificial grammar learning. Top. Cogn. Sci. 4, 554567. 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01208.x22760940 Lumaca M. Baggio G. (2016). Brain potentials predict learning, transmission and modification of an artificial symbolic system. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 19701979. 10.1093/scan/nsw11227510496 Lumaca M. Baggio G. (2017). Cultural transmission and evolution of melodic structures in multi-generational signaling games. Artif. Life 23, 406423. 10.1162/ARTL_a_0023828786724 Lumaca M. Baggio G. (2018). Signaling games and the evolution of structure in language and music: a reply to Ravignani and Verhoef (2018). Artif. Life. MacCallum R. M. Mauch M. Burt A. Leroi A. M. (2012). Evolution of music by public choice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 1208112086. 10.1073/pnas.120318210922711832 Malm W. P. (1967). Music Cultures of the Pacific, the Near East, and Asia. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Marr D. (1982). Visual information processing: the structure and creation of visual representations, in Recognition of Pattern and Form, ed Albrecht D. (Berlin: Springer), 5987. McDermott J. Hauser M. (2005). The origins of music: innateness, uniqueness, and evolution. Music Percept. 23, 2959. 10.1525/mp.2005.23.1.29 Mehr S. A. Singh M. York H. Glowacki L. Krasnow M. M. (2018). Form and function in human song. Curr. Biol. 28, 356368. 10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.04229395919 Merker B. (2006). The uneven interface between culture and biology in human music (commentary). Music Percept. 24, 9598. 10.1525/mp.2006.24.1.95 Merker B. Morley I. Zuidema W. (2015). Five fundamental constraints on theories of the origins of music. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 370:20140095. 10.1098/rstb.2014.009525646518 Merriam A. P. Whinery S. Fred B. G. (1956). Songs of a rada community in trinidad. Antropos 51, 157174. Mesoudi A. (2011). Cultural Evolution: How Darwinian Theory Can Explain Human Culture and Synthesize the Social Sciences. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Mesoudi A. (2015). Cultural evolution: a review of theory, findings and controversies. Evol. Biol. 43, 481497. 10.1007/s11692-015-9320-0 Mesoudi A. Whiten A. Laland K. N. (2006). Towards a unified science of cultural evolution. Behav. Brain Sci. 29, 329347. 10.1017/S0140525X0600908317094820 Molfese D. L. Molfese V. J. (1997). Discrimination of language skills at five years of age using event-related potentials recorded at birth. Dev. Neuropsychol. 13, 135156. 10.1080/87565649709540674 Moreno M. Baggio G. (2015). Role asymmetry and code transmission in signaling games: an experimental and computational investigation. Cogn. Sci. 39, 918943. 10.1111/cogs.1219125352016 Morley I. (2013). The Prehistory of Music: Human Evolution, Archaeology, and the Origins of Musicality. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Näätänen R. Gaillard A. W. Mäntysalo S. (1978). Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted. Acta Psychol. 42, 313329. 10.1016/0001-6918(78)90006-9685709 Näätänen R. Schröger E. Karakas S. Tervaniemi M. Paavilainen P. (1993). Development of a memory trace for a complex sound in the human brain. Neuroreport 4, 503506. 10.1097/00001756-199305000-000108513127 Näätänen R. Tervaniemi M. Sussman E. Paavilainen P. Winkler I. (2001). “Primitive intelligence” in the auditory cortex. Trends Neurosci. 24, 283288. 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01790-211311381 Navarro D. J. A. Perfors A. Kary A. Brown S. Donkin C. (2017). When extremists win: on the behavior of iterated learning chains when priors are heterogeneous. CogSci. 847852. Nettl B. (1983). The Study of Ethnomusicology: Twenty-nine Issues and Concepts. Chicago, IL: University of Illinois Press. Nicolai J. Gundacker C. Teeselink K. Güttinger H. R. (2014). Human melody singing by bullfinches (Pyrrhula pyrrula) gives hints about a cognitive note sequence processing. Anim. Cogn. 17, 143155. 10.1007/s10071-013-0647-623783267 Nieminen S. Istók E. Brattico E. Tervaniemi M. Huotilainen M. (2011). The development of aesthetic responses to music and their underlying neural and psychological mechanisms. Cortex 47, 11381146. 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.05.00821665202 Nikolsky A. (2015). Evolution of tonal organization in music mirrors symbolic representation of perceptual reality. Part-1: Prehistoric. Front. Psychol. 6:1405. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.0140526528193 Nowak I. Baggio G. (2016). The emergence of word order and morphology in compositional languages via multigenerational signaling games. J. Lang. Evol. 1, 137150. 10.1093/jole/lzw007 Paavilainen P. (2013). The mismatch-negativity (MMN) component of the auditory event-related potential to violations of abstract regularities: a review. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 88, 109123. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.03.01523542165 Pamjav H. Juhasz Z. Zalan A. Nemeth E. Damdin B. (2012). A comparative phylogenetic study of genetics and folk music. Mol. Genet. Genomics. 287, 337349. 10.1007/s00438-012-0683-y22392540 Patel A. D. (2010). Music, Language, and the Brain. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Peretz I. Zatorre R. J. (2005). Brain organization for music processing. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 56, 89114. 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.07022515709930 Ravignani A. Delgado T. Kirby S. (2016). Musical evolution in the lab exhibits rhythmic universals. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1:0007. 10.1038/s41562-016-0007 Ravignani A. Thompson B. Grossi T. Delgado T. Kirby S. (2018). Evolving building blocks of rhyth m: how human cognition creates music via cultural transmission. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 10.1111/nyas.1361029508405 Ravignani A. Verhoef T. (2018). Which melodic universals emerge from repeated signaling games? Artif. Life. 24. Reber R. Schwarz N. Winkielman P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver's processing experience? Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 8, 364382. 10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_315582859 Repp B. H. (1991). Some cognitive and perceptual aspects of speech and music, in Music, Language, Speech and Brain, eds Sundberg J. Nord L. Carlson R. (Stockholm: MacMillan Press), 257268. Rohrmeier M. Cross I. (2013). Artificial grammar learning of melody is constrained by melodic inconsistency: Narmour's principles affect melodic learning. PLoS ONE 8:e66174. 10.1371/journal.pone.006617423874388 Rzeszutek T. Savage P. E. Brown S. (2012). The structure of cross-cultural musical diversity. Proc. Biol. Sci. 279, 16061612. 10.1098/rspb.2011.175022072606 Sadtler P. T. Quick K. M. Golub M. D. Chase S. M. Ryu S. I. Tyler-Kabara E. C. . (2014). Neural constraints on learning. Nature 512, 423426. 10.1038/nature1366525164754 Savage P. E. Brown S. (2007). Toward a new comparative musicology. Anal. Approach. World Music 2, 148197. Savage P. E. Brown S. Sakai E. Currie T. E. (2015). Statistical universals reveal the structures and functions of human music. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 89878992. 10.1073/pnas.141449511226124105 Sawa G. D. (2002). Oral transmission in Arabic music, past and present. Oral Trad. 4, 254265. Simon G. Lanoë C. Poirel N. Rossi S. Lubin A. Pineau A. . (2013). Dynamics of the anatomical changes that occur in the brains of schoolchildren as they learn to read. PLoS ONE 8:e81789. 10.1371/journal.pone.008178924367494 Skeide M. A. Kumar U. Mishra R. K. Tripathi V. N. Guleria A. Singh J. P. . (2017). Learning to read alters cortico-subcortical cross-talk in the visual system of illiterates. Sci. Adv. 3:e1602612. 10.1126/sciadv.160261228560333 Skyrms B. (2010). Signals: Evolution, Learning, and Information. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Snyder B. (2008). Memory for music, in Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology, eds Hallam S. Cross I. Thaut M. (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press), 107117. Sperber D. (1996). Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Tamariz M. (2017). Experimental studies on the cultural evolution of language. Annu. Rev. Linguist. 3, 389407. 10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011516-033807 Tamariz M. Kirby S. (2015). Culture: copying, compression, and conventionality. Cogn. Sci. 39, 171183. 10.1111/cogs.1214425039798 Tchernichovski O. Lipkind D. (2016). Encoding vocal culture. Science 354, 12341235. 10.1126/science.aal320527940834 Temperley D. (2004). Communicative pressure and the evolution of musical styles. Music Percept. 21, 313337. 10.1525/mp.2004.21.3.313 Tervaniemi M. Rytkönen M. Schröger E. Ilmoniemi R. J. Näätänen R. (2001). Superior formation of cortical memory traces for melodic patterns in musicians. Learn. Mem. 8, 295300. 10.1101/lm.3950111584077 Thompson B. Kirby S. Smith K. (2016). Culture shapes the evolution of cognition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 45304535. 10.1073/pnas.152363111327044094 Trainor L. J. (2015). The origins of music in auditory scene analysis and the roles of evolution and culture in musical creation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 370:20140089. 10.1098/rstb.2014.008925646512 Trehub S. E. (2000). Human processing predispositions and musical universals, in The Origins of Music, eds Wallin N. L. Merker B. Brown S. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 427448. Trehub S. E. (2015). Cross-cultural convergence of musical features. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 88098810. 10.1073/pnas.151072411226157132 Verhoef T. (2012). The origins of duality of patterning in artificial whistled languages. Lang. Cogn. 4, 357380. 10.1515/langcog-2012-001923637710 Von Hippel P. (2000). Redefining pitch proximity: tessitura and mobility as constraints on melodic universals. Music Percept. 17, 315327. 10.2307/40285820 Wallin N. L. Merker B. Brown S. (2001). The Origins of Music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Winkler I. Kushnerenko E. Horváth J. Ceponiene R. Fellman V. Huotilainen M. . (2003). Newborn infants can organize the auditory world. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 1181211815. 10.1073/pnas.203189110014500903 Zadbood A. Chen J. Leong Y. C. Norman K. A. Hasson U. (2017). How we transmit memories to other brains: constructing shared neural representations via communication. Cereb. Cortex 27, 49885000. 10.1093/cercor/bhx20228922834 Zatorre R. J. (2001). Neural specializations for tonal processing. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 930, 193210. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05734.x11458830 Zatorre R. J. (2013). Predispositions and plasticity in music and speech learning: neural correlates and implications. Science 342, 585589. 10.1126/science.123841424179219 Zatorre R. J. Delhommeau K. Zarate J. M. (2012). Modulation of auditory cortex response to pitch variation following training with microtonal melodies. Front. Psychol. 3:544. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.0054423227019

      Funding. AR was supported by funding from the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement No 665501 with the research Foundation Flanders (FWO) (Pegasus2 Marie Curie fellowship 12N5517N awarded to AR), and a visiting fellowship in Language Evolution from the Max Planck Society (awarded to AR).

      1Our definition of “memory bottleneck” includes constraints on perceptual grouping; capacity and temporal limits of auditory memory, serial processing, and attention; constraints on the neurodynamics of the auditory system; perceptual hearing thresholds. We limited this list to constraints “directly” related to basic aspects of perception and cognition. We acknowledge that constraints of a different nature might have a formative power over musical structures (e.g., motoric, motoric-expressive, physiological, cross-modal, and semantics).

      2In signaling games with fixed roles, including all MGSGs, the receiver tends to learn the code transmitted by the sender. In other words, there is asymmetry in the division of coordination labor between the sender and the receiver, with most coordination work (most code changes) falling to the latter (Nowak and Baggio, 2016).

      3The re-use of Wagner's musical ideas by other composers during Nazi Germany and the emergence and maintenance of stylistic clusters in contemporary pop music are clear examples of biased selection.

      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016hzdklc.com.cn
      www.liujifutu.com.cn
      gzchst.com.cn
      www.nncq.com.cn
      vrvision.net.cn
      qr8d4f.net.cn
      www.npchain.com.cn
      tychain.com.cn
      www.nrefs.com.cn
      www.manytour.net.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p