Front. Hum. Neurosci. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience Front. Hum. Neurosci. 1662-5161 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00400 Neuroscience Review Making Memories: Why Time Matters Kelley Paul 1 * Evans M. D. R. 2 3 Kelley Jonathan 3 1Sleep, Circadian and Memory Neuroscience, The Open University, Milton Keynes, United Kingdom 2Sociology and Applied Statistics Program, University of Nevada, Reno, Reno, NV, United States 3Sociology, University of Nevada, Reno, Reno, NV, United States

Edited by: Srikantan S. Nagarajan, University of California, San Francisco, United States

Reviewed by: Andreas Knoblauch, Hochschule Albstadt-Sigmaringen, Germany; Krzysztof Zagorski, Kozminski University, Poland

*Correspondence: Paul Kelley drpaulkelley@gmail.com
16 10 2018 2018 12 400 26 11 2017 18 09 2018 Copyright © 2018 Kelley, Evans and Kelley. 2018 Kelley, Evans and Kelley

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

In the last decade advances in human neuroscience have identified the critical importance of time in creating long-term memories. Circadian neuroscience has established biological time functions via cellular clocks regulated by photosensitive retinal ganglion cells and the suprachiasmatic nuclei. Individuals have different circadian clocks depending on their chronotypes that vary with genetic, age, and sex. In contrast, social time is determined by time zones, daylight savings time, and education and employment hours. Social time and circadian time differences can lead to circadian desynchronization, sleep deprivation, health problems, and poor cognitive performance. Synchronizing social time to circadian biology leads to better health and learning, as demonstrated in adolescent education. In-day making memories of complex bodies of structured information in education is organized in social time and uses many different learning techniques. Research in the neuroscience of long-term memory (LTM) has demonstrated in-day time spaced learning patterns of three repetitions of information separated by two rest periods are effective in making memories in mammals and humans. This time pattern is based on the intracellular processes required in synaptic plasticity. Circadian desynchronization, sleep deprivation, and memory consolidation in sleep are less well-understood, though there has been considerable progress in neuroscience research in the last decade. The interplay of circadian, in-day and sleep neuroscience research are creating an understanding of making memories in the first 24-h that has already led to interventions that can improve health and learning.

Circadian timing long-term memory social time spaced learning

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Circadian cycles create the patterns of wake and sleep in which short and long-term memories are created. Social factors leading to mistiming wake and disrupting sleep can have a negative impact on these memory functions. Memory processes in the initial 24-h cycle are critical in determining what is encoded into long-term memory (LTM) rather than left to fade in short term memory (STM). There are many stimuli that can trigger LTM but, in this review, we focus on spaced repetition leading to LTM of bodies of complex information, an essential process in learning.

      The fundamental role of circadian timing processes has been emphasized by the award of the Nobel Prize in 2017 in physiology or medicine to Hall, Rosbash, and Young for their discoveries of the genetic mechanisms that create circadian 24-h timing in drosophila and humans alike (Bargiello et al., 1984; Siwicki et al., 1988; Hardin et al., 1990; Liu et al., 1992; Price et al., 1998). Circadian 24-h cellular mechanisms are genetic, evolutionarily conserved, and found across all photosensitive forms of life (Bass and Lazar, 2016). In the last 10 years circadian timing has been shown to have a direct impact on human disease, immunity, metabolism, and neurodegeneration (Man et al., 2016; Musiek and Holtzman, 2016; Panda, 2016; Turek, 2016).

      Circadian timing can also have a direct impact on human LTM and learning. Circadian clocks are driven by complex genetic and neuroscientific processes. In turn these clocks co-ordinate most 24-h activity including physiological, metabolic, organ and brain functions, including wake/sleep and cognition. When circadian timing is disrupted, then the biological clocks in different tissues can become uncoupled, resulting in a state of internal desynchrony that can have negative impacts on memory creation and health (Foster et al., 2013). Here we consider the central role of modern societies in creating desynchrony when social and biological times are in conflict (Merton, 1936; Sorokin and Merton, 1937; Zerubavel, 1982, 1985; Foster et al., 2013).

      There are many ways in which LTM can be created, though it is only recently that the neuron's intracellular synaptic plasticity mechanisms have been discovered in mice (Fields et al., 2005, Fields, 2005). In Fields' research these mechanisms required a “time code” of periods without stimuli to encode LTM. Specifically, the timing pattern was three stimuli separated by two 10-min periods without stimuli. Later, other researchers applied this timing pattern to humans learning complex bodies of structured information (Kelley and Whatson, 2013).

      The first section explores recent advances in our understanding of circadian neuroscience, chronotypes, and sleep deprivation. These advances identify research-based applications for improving learning (and health) in adolescent education through synchronizing education times to circadian time. The second section focuses on advances in LTM neuroscience, and an application for improving learning based on these advances leading to creating LTM in humans of complex bodies of structured information. Both circadian and LTM neuroscience are then considered in memory consolidation in sleep.

      Circadian neuroscience Biological and social time

      Like most life on earth, our physiology, health and behaviors have 24-h rhythms that follow the increasing and decreasing light levels throughout the day and night. These changes in light are the most significant environmental time system and have led to a variety of biological systems to attune animals to circadian timing. In mammals, circadian timing is in effect a two-stage process, with cells individually generating a 24-h rhythm and a master circadian pacemaker establishing more consistent timing throughout the body. In humans the 24-h pacemaker is the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN) in the hypothalamus, and the SCN itself is synchronized, or “entrained,” to the variations in light, specifically irradiance. The specific biological mechanism for sensing these changes are triggered when blue light of ~480 nm wavelength enters the eye and strikes photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (pRGC) (Foster and Hankins, 2007). Generally, humans respond most strongly to light levels in twilight (before dawn or after sunset) that entrain circadian timing to the 24-h day.

      This pRGC/SCN timing mechanism was discovered in mammals only recently and has implications for human functions including making memories. As circadian time systems adjust physiology and behavior in preparation for expected requirements during the day-night cycle, synchronization to changes in light are critical. Even sensing a single photon of light can be important: the absorption of a ~480 nm wavelength photon by 11-cis-retinaldehyde and the photoisomerization of this molecule in a pRGC leads to a strong signal being sent from the eye directly to the SCN (Wald, 1968; Foster and Hankins, 2007). The number of pRGC cells in the eye is relatively low, and consequently our sense of time depends on the gross amount of light of the appropriate wave length (Lucas et al., 2014). Changing levels of illuminance directly regulate pRGC activity and thus the SCN, creating an automatic, unconscious circadian time system. Indeed, totally blind subjects can retain normal circadian time function, demonstrating the separation of vision and time functions in the human eye (Zaidi et al., 2007).

      Socially created lighting environments can also negatively impact on making memories as light itself has an important role in securing optimal physical and cognitive performance (Cajochen, 2007). Light has a direct impact on important aspects of creating memory: light levels act as a neurophysiological stimulant that increases alertness, attention, and improves reaction times. The direct response to bright light has differing impacts depending on the time as has been demonstrated in several ingenious studies (Cajochen et al., 2000; Phipps-Nelson et al., 2003; Rüger et al., 2006). While sunlight and very high light levels can boost human alertness, attention, cognition, and performance, low light levels can have the reverse effect (Lockley and Gooley, 2006; Lockley et al., 2006). For example, Rüger and colleagues concluded light levels may have a wide range of applications, ranging from optimizing a work environment to treating depressed patients. In contrast, many work environments rely on artificial light, reducing light levels far below the lux level of natural light, lowering light-induced alertness, and decreasing access to light with appropriately timed changes in illuminance that are critical in the pRGC/SCN timing system. The overall impact in schools, universities and employment is creating a more uniform, lower light experience throughout the day and increasing light at night, and this may have many direct and indirect impacts. In comparison to natural light levels many artificial light environments can negatively impact on natural circadian timing and health (Chang et al., 2015).

      Although modern social behaviors, artificial light, and many other factors may cause individuals to become desynchronized from their circadian timing system, our research team found the most significant chronic desynchronization arises from the introduction of socially determined time or universal time (UTC), and fixed times for education, work and other activities. In order to measure desynchronization caused by UTC, our research team developed a new method of measuring time. We reasoned that UTC uses socially determined time zones, daylight savings time, and discounts changes in illuminance within time zones. In contrast, changes in light depend only on geophysical location and the pRGC/SCN timing mechanism, leading us to measure biological time as Geophysical Biological Time (GBT). GBT uses only the environmental cues of changing light in a specific geophysical location to calculate biological time and determining optimal timings for human activities (Bass and Lazar, 2016; Evans et al., 2017). The differences between GBT and UTC can be important, as shown in studies of Time Zone anomalies (Yasseri et al., 2012) and Daylight Savings Time (Coren, 1996).

      Chronotypes, sleep deprivation, and desynchronization

      Although the circadian pRGC/SCN timing system entrains individuals to the GBT 24-h day, there are wide variations between different chronotypes in their responses to light. Chronotypes are most often determined through surveys of wake and sleep times, adjusted for working and non-working days. In a long-running series of chronotype questionnaires building upon use of time survey-based research, Roenneberg and his colleagues established very large data sets that identify chronotype's different wake and sleep patterns (Roenneberg et al., 2004, 2007). From this data and other studies, it has emerged $that chronotype varies considerably with extreme early types waking up when extreme late types fall asleep (Roenneberg et al., 2007). Sleep duration and timing also varies with age, sex, and genetic factors (Foster et al., 2013.

      An individual's timing system links to circadian rhythms and 24-h changes in behavior, most noticeably in sleep/wake patterns. Other changes are not often visible, yet circadian rhythmicity is found in almost every physiological, metabolic, and behavioral system, for example core body temperature (Lucas et al., 2014). Desynchronization can therefore have real-life implications in human health and performance (Zhang et al., 2014).

      Sleep deprivation influences many aspects of health in ways that impact directly and indirectly on memory and learning processes. Since 1964 sleeping <6.5 h each day has been known to be associated with a range of illnesses and higher death rates (Hammond, 1964). This finding has been the subject of decades of research using a range of methodologies (Cappuccio et al., 2010). Although the links between poor sleep, illnesses and other disorders are well-established, solutions have been more difficult to identify. In part this is because of the nature of sleep: it is complex state, involving multiple brain regions, neurotransmitter pathways, and hormones (Foster and Wulff, 2005). The recommended duration of sleep in adulthood is 7–9 h (National Sleep Foundation, 2017), and sleep of <7 h is associated with a range of health risks, poor mood, and potentially mental health problems, as well as poor cognitive function (Czeisler, 2011). The time-in-day of sleep onset is important as well (as night shifts demonstrate): broadly speaking matching individual's biological sleep onset time is optimal, and the greater the divergence from that time the greater sleep disruption. Indeed, sleep disruption has been shown to impact directly on emotional, cognitive and somatic functions that impact directly on health and learning (Foster and Wulff, 2005; Wulff et al., 2011).

      Sleep deprivation is clearly associated with poorer cognitive processes (Pilcher and Huffcutt, 1966; Thomas et al., 2000; Van Dongen et al., 2003), and studies have also found poor communication, decreased concentration and cognitive performance, unintended sleeps, decreased motor performance, increased risk taking and changes in mood pattern, specifically depression (Foster and Wulff, 2005: Kelley et al., 2015). These factors directly or indirectly impact negatively on learning and memory processes (Curcio et al., 2006: Diekelmann and Born, 2010; Escribano et al., 2012). Other studies have shown restricted sleep is associated with health issues that may also affect learning and memory including impaired immune response, metabolic disorders, diabetes, hypertension, anxiety, depression, and obesity (Luyster et al., 2012), arising from the neuroscience of sleep loss (Krause et al., 2017).

      Circadian timing and adolescent education: later starts that improve learning and health

      The differences between sleep patterns in adolescence and social times for education lead to chronic sleep deprivation. This can be illustrated by considering the school starting time controversy in U.S. middle and high schools. In the U.S. education start times average 08:03 in middle and high schools (Wheaton et al., 2015), and this is too early for older adolescents, leading to poorer health and academic performance (Schmidt et al., 2009; Kirkby et al., 2011; Basch et al., 2014). Changing to later start times in education has been strongly advocated by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, the American Academy of Pediatrics, and the American Medical Association (American Academy of Pediatrics, 2014). The specific recommended change is that middle and high schools should open no earlier than 08:30. The recommendation was based on research showing health, mental health and performance were at greater risk when start times were earlier.

      The circadian biology of adolescence explains the problems of school timing. Starting with the pioneering of Carskadon (Carskadon et al., 1993; Carskadon and Dement, 2005; Millman, 2005; Hagenauer et al., 2009; Carskadon, 2011), US school times were demonstrated to be desynchronized from adolescent biological timing. It has been suggested that the end of changes in the adolescence tendency to later wake/sleep circadian timing should be used as the marker for the end of puberty (Roenneberg et al., 2004). This proposal was based on evidence that in childhood there is a shift to later wake/ sleep times and females start this process about 1.4 years earlier than males and end this process about 1.4 years earlier, both in line with puberty (but see Sørensen et al., 2012). The genetic basis of these changes by age and sex were confirmed in a range of studies (Duffy et al., 2011; Giedd et al., 2012) many after Roenneberg's proposal. The estimated difference between the mean wake/sleep time of adolescents and the mean of other ages ranges from 2 to 2.7 h in different studies. Such a major difference in wake/ sleep times has been taken as the underlying cause of the mismatch between adolescent wake/sleep times and U.S. school start times. The temporal misalignment between the sleep timing for adolescence and educational institutions' usual start hours causes significant sleep loss. Sleep loss, in turn, impairs academic performance and elevates risks of obesity, depression, and drug abuse. The biological mechanisms through which early starts increase health risks and lower performance are well-established (Carrell et al., 2011; Kirkby et al., 2011). These biological changes in the timing of sleep propensity underlie the conflict with education start times and, just as in sleep-deprived adults, chronic sleep deprivation has negative impacts on cognition (Lockley et al., 2004).

      The medical and circadian research evidence for later school starts is overwhelming. The medical community's recommendation that middle and high schools in the U.S. should start no earlier than 08:30 is clearly justified. Schools in many states and other countries such as South Korea are changing to later starts and finding students sleep more, achieve better results and better health. However, moving starting times later than 08:30 does not clarify what wake time is optimal for adolescents of different ages. This issue was addressed by our research team, growing out of our work with Oxford University's Sleep and Circadian Neuroscience Institute. Synchronizing education to adolescent circadian neuroscience evidence led to projections that for adolescents at age 16 starting times within the range 10:00–10:30 were appropriate, whereas starting at age 18 starting times of 11:00–11:30 were appropriate (Kelley et al., 2015). Our research trial was in a UK school that changed its 08:50 start to a 10:00 starting time for 2 years. There were highly significant improvements: absence due to illness fell by 50%, and academic outcomes rose 17% compared to schools nationally. We concluded school starting times that were better aligned with adolescent sleep, chronotype patterns and circadian neuroscience research are practical and beneficial (Kelley et al., 2017). Yet it was becoming clear that desynchrony between circadian and social timing can affect different ages and chronotypes in different ways.

      Circadian and social time synchronization

      When there is circadian synchrony, as when social time matches biological time, humans function well. When there is desynchrony it can lead to pathologies and dysfunction, including poorer cognitive function. An important factor in sleep loss is different chronotypes, and the failure to recognize the mismatch between starting times and sleep patterns, and the differing impact on chronotype groups, has limited our understating of the complexities of circadian timing and social time. For example, though the number of evening chronotypes increases with adolescence, there remain morning and in-between chronotypes within the adolescent population. Definitely morning and morning chronotypes suffer far less sleep deprivation with early starts in adolescence, or even have no sleep deprivation caused by school starting times. In contrast, the sleep deprivation for definitely evening chronotypes is far greater and can reach 3 h a day or more. Therefore, circadian chronotype analysis is necessary to more accurately assess the impact on groups and individuals.

      These circadian time analyses may help explain the findings that evening chronotypes perform less well in schools, are more likely to have depressive symptoms, and have higher levels of obesity and is consistent across studies of older adolescents (Giannotti et al., 2002; Randler and Frech, 2006; Escribano et al., 2012; Hsu et al., 2012; Preckel et al., 2013). Studies of evening types specifically show these symptoms from relatively early in adolescence through university, and this supports a thesis that evening chronotypes are more likely to be negatively affected by sleep loss than other chronotypes (Kelley et al., 2015). Alternatively, later start hours are associated with longer sleep, better health, and better performance, and most studies conclude that the key variable is sleep loss compared to adequate sleep (Borlase et al., 2013; Wahlstrom et al., 2014).

      The conflict between biological and social time is endemic in modern society because of the use of social time systems that are not synchronized to biological time leading to unintended consequences. This may prove difficult to change as social time itself has a historical legacy of industrialization, institutionalization and traditions such as a conventional 9-to-5 working day (Ford, 1926). Although it is not within the scope of this review, societies generally are slow to change, especially in relation to work and life patterns of behavior. To better understand social processes, sociologists have developed a range of analytical tools to track changes and their causes, often working with extremely large data sets. One specific methodology used extensively in sociology is surveys, including the measurement of time. Prior sociological research on survey design showed that during exploratory research very detailed questions about time-related actions and feelings produced much more reliable data than summary questions (Tourangeau et al., 2000; Schaeffer and Presser, 2003). In circadian research large-scale research into circadian patterns of sleep and wake have taken a different survey approach, concentrating just on biological behaviors.

      Our research team created a new sociological method to assess chronotype variations in undergraduates using a 24-h time scale of when subjects were at their best. Analysis of the data from 190 undergraduates aged 18–19 was similar to Roenneberg's data suggesting very wide circadian variations indeed (Roenneberg et al., 2007). Using the more detailed time scores meant the new sociological method could track changes in self-assessed alertness throughout wake. The best time for starting using this approach was 11:00 or 12:00. Using the neuroscience-based approach based on local light changes in Reno, Nevada the best time was 11.27 GBT. Projections from our earlier work suggested 11:00 to 11:30. There are two important findings arising from chronotype variations and being at their best measures for each hour: first, any fixed morning start time in the university would lead to some sleep deprivation or disadvantage some chronotype group, and second, no hour in the day for learning would be optimal for all chronotype groups (Evans et al., 2017). The conclusion that much later starting times were required in this age range was expected, but the conclusion that there was no single starting time produced optimal times for all chronotypes was not. In a social time frame, small differences between chronotypes' optimal performance are manageable, but very large differences would pose fundamental questions about current social timing. Thus, desynchrony between circadian and social timing can affect different ages and chronotypes in different ways.

      Progress in genetic circadian research has also been rapid in the period 2013–2018. An elegant experiment demonstrated sleep loss and adequate sleep in the same subjects, and the genetic mechanisms of sleep loss (Möller-Levet et al., 2013). The 26 female and male participants (mean age 27.5) had a week of sleep restriction of 5.7 h, and a week of adequate sleep of 8.5 h. Controlling for a wide range of variables, whole-blood RNA samples were used in transcriptome analysis, and these showed 711 genes up- or down-regulated by insufficient sleep. These changes demonstrated the negative impacts of sleep deprivation and the positive impact of adequate sleep in the same people (see also Jonsson et al., 2016). Insufficient sleep disrupted circadian regulation of genes and was not sufficient to maintain alertness and performance during wake. This was a very clear demonstration of the importance of sleep and the underlying biological mechanisms associated with a shift from adequate sleep to restricted sleep. Möller-Levet's research can be examined in relation to time patterns in sleep itself, specifically sleep cycles that are repeated during the full duration of sleep. Sleep cycles are about 90 min long, and occur four or five times in a sleep episode (Carskadon and Dement, 2005; Abel et al., 2013). Each sleep cycle is characterized by four sleep stages including rapid eye movement (REM), slow wave sleep and sleep spindles all of which are critical in the consolidation of learning and long-term memories. Restricting subjects to 5.7 h would only allow three complete sleep cycles, and adequate sleep of 8.5 h would allow more than 5 complete sleep cycles. More recently genetic circadian research has demonstrated long-term circadian disruption impacts on human heath (Turek, 2016), immunity (Man et al., 2016), metabolism (Panda, 2016), and neurodegeneration (Musiek and Holtzman, 2016).

      Circadian synchrony when social time matches biological time presents many challenges, but the alternative of continuing to allow desynchrony that causes poorer health, mental health and cognitive function is untenable. The surge in home working in many countries including the U.S. enables better, even optimal circadian synchrony, is the most positive example of society adjusting to neuroscientific realities, even if it is unintentional consequence of new technologies. Seemingly intractable issues such as night shifts are amenable to improvement by matching chronotypes to working hours (Vetter et al., 2015), and better night shift and recovery work patterns (Cappuccio et al., 2009). The direct challenge to social times in schools is based on medical, sleep, and circadian evidence, and indicates a significant change in attitudes to social time. In general, the case has been made for circadian synchrony that protects sleep duration, while chronotype variation is less well-understood.

      Long-term memory neuroscience LTM and learning complex bodies of structured information

      New techniques such as scanning technologies have allowed a better understanding of the neuronal ensembles formed in memory processes. Experimental results suggest learning and memory occur in the context of continual structural synaptic plasticity such as formation and elimination of synapses (Chklovskii et al., 2004; Holtmaat and Svoboda, 2009). Understanding these processes can be enhanced through modeling potential mechanisms to explain experimental findings (Knoblauch and Sommer, 2016; Knoblauch, 2017) or apply them in a new context (Popovych et al., 2015; Abdou et al., 2018). The most significant recent discoveries are in glia biology, and the roles of astrocytes and myelin in cognition and learning (Fields et al., 2014). For example, myelination sheathes regulate the speed and synchrony of synaptic firing, are vital in information processing, links between different cortical regions, and fundamental memory and cognition processes. In contrast, myelin also limits critical periods for learning through inhibiting proteins that reduce synaptogenesis and axon sprouting. Astrocytes impact on synaptic plasticity, long-term potentiation (LTP) in cortex and hippocampus, spike-timing dependent cortical, plasticity, hippocampal long-term depression (LTD) and working memory. In addition, astrocytes can severely restrict LTM formation, restrict LTM consolidation in sleep, and directly influence the SCN during sleep (Brancaccio et al., 2017).

      The degree of synchronization of socially determined times to circadian time can impact on learning and memory processes within each 24-h cycle. On waking alertness and performance varies with sleep loss the night before. Wake alertness varies through the day by chronotype and other circadian and biological factors but environmental stimuli are not bound by these predictive biological in-day patterns. When alertness is low stimuli are more likely to trigger short-term memories (STM), and LTM is less frequent unless the stimuli are highly relevant, emotional, novel, repeated, or extreme (Fields, 2005).

      LTM processes are the fundamental basis of learning and memory processes and, like the circadian processes, they are biologically determined through evolutionarily-conserved timing mechanisms. Here we focus on cAMP response element-binding protein (CREB) in intracellular LTM functions and timing and the associated CREB-based spaced learning (CBSL) to illustrate biological timing of human LTM processes. The molecular and systems biology of memory has been well-established in broad terms during the last century. Progress has often been slow, with hypotheses that have been proved only after decades of research. Learning is characterized as the biological processes in acquiring information from the environment and consolidation in sleep (Atkinson and Shiffrin, 1968; Kandel et al., 2014). Studies of Aplysia to mammals provide a coherent model of simple neuronal plasticity developing to more complex processes in mammals.

      In the early studies of memory, differences between Pavlovian memory encoding in dogs (Pavlov, 2010) and Ebbinghaus's memory retrieval and loss in humans (Ebbinghaus, 1913) emerged. Ebbinghaus's initial findings continue to be researched, notably memory retrieval testing where the intervals between retrieval practices were shown to be important (Cepeda et al., 2008; Karpicke and Roediger, 2008; Karpicke and Bauernschmidt, 2011; Karpicke and Blunt, 2011; Smith et al., 2016), and the similar timing issues in the processes in memory loss (Scoville and Milner, 1957; Gong et al., 2003).

      In this review we focus on the initial circadian cycle, and on purposeful memory encoding and consolidation. LTM formation is now understood to depend on Long-Term Potentiation (LTP) and late-long -term potentiation L-LTP. L-LTP depends on protein kinases such as MARK and PKA signaling to CREB and is dependent on gene transcription (Frey et al., 1993; Bourtchuladze et al., 1994; Abel et al., 1997; English and Sweatt, 1997; Perazzona et al., 2004). Repeated but spaced periods of stimuli can initiate intracellular mechanisms activating genes, initiating the production of proteins (Scharf et al., 2002; Morris, 2003; Hernandez and Abel, 2008). These studies show proteins then can strengthen sensitized synapses, triggering LTP, and LTM encoding (Frey and Morris, 1997; Lu et al., 2008; Fields, 2011; Moncada et al., 2011). The discovery of a critical period for these processes had been identified earlier (Nguyen et al., 1994).

      Behavioral studies with different species showed that the timing of stimuli was a critical variable in LTM encoding. In a classic study, honeybees were trained to find food locations with 30 s, 3 min, and 10-min spaces with no training stimuli. The honeybees trained with 10 min spaces were the only group that achieved memory encoding and consolidating, reaching almost 100% accuracy on the third day, demonstrating long-term memories had been created (Menzel et al., 2001). In Drosophila spaced learning can produce stabilized LTM and de novo protein synthesis CREB-dependent gene transcription (Perazzona et al., 2004). Further Drosophila research demonstrated de novo protein synthesis and LTM after spaced learning in vitro and in vivo, and a gating mechanism for LTM formation (Chen et al., 2012; Plaçais and Preat, 2013). This has led to a further study demonstrating the evolutionary gain in gating LTM encoding, as creating LTP demands high energy input, in contrast with STM (Plaçais and Preat, 2013). The biological processes of LTP within neurons and synaptic plasticity between neurons create LTM through processes termed synaptic tagging and capture, an increasingly robust model of memory formation in mammals, in a process described in a thorough review (Redondo and Morris, 2011). Within neurons, encoding in transient memory traces in synapses (“tagging”) can be followed by a strengthening process of LTP and L-LTP that can become encoded into LTM. Other neural activity processing before, during and after this can interfere with the encoding process. Synaptic tagging and capture has four elements: the expression of synaptic potentiation with the setting of a local synaptic tag; synthesis and distribution of plasticity related proteins (PrPs); capture of proteins by tagged synapses; and stabilization of synaptic strength (Redondo and Morris, 2011). These processes operate in a distinct time window: the initial stimuli and tagging occurring in the first period of 0–30 min; and 30 min to 2 h for PrPs processes including CREB-dependent gene transcription.

      LTM and CBSL

      Broadly speaking, LTP and LTM encoding processes occur in relatively short time scales. LTM can be created by extreme stress events (Pitman et al., 2012), and rapid, involuntary memory creation is the norm in humans. Purposeful learning of skills or in education can be effortful and, at times, making memories proves difficult. The solutions must lie in the biology of encoding memories. Studies in mammalian memory processes in vitro rat hippocampus cells clarified the importance of neural impulse activity in triggering LTP that had been first identified in studies of nervous system development (Hebb, 1949; Itoh et al., 1995). Later studies used a pattern of three stimuli separated by 10-min spaces that opened voltage-sensitive calcium channels in the cell membrane, activating signaling pathways to the nucleus. The pattern of impulse firing was echoed in the patterns of calcium flashes, and these were visualized using calcium-sensitive dye and scanning laser confocal microscopy to track pulses of calcium influx following stimuli (Fields et al., 2005). These and other related studies demonstrated there was no need for a specific signaling molecule from the membrane to the nucleus, as the spaced pattern of action potential activity led to CREB phosphorylation and DNA synthesis of zif 268, a gene associated with memory (Dudek and Fields, 2002). Parallel studies in the biochemistry of memory over many years have clarified LTP processes further (Baudry et al., 2011).

      Studies on living rats showed patterns of action potential activity have a critical role in long-term synaptic change, providing a direct link between in vitro studies of protein-dependent LTP and behavioral studies of LTM retention (Shires et al., 2012). These temporal patterns in LTM encoding have recently been linked to specific molecular LTP processes in a time scale of minutes (Naqib et al., 2012), and, in humans, more rapidly in fMRI studies of face recognition (Xue et al., 2011). There were areas of research into LTM processes that appeared to have clear implications in human learning arising from the importance of time patterns in LTM encoding. These time codes were effective in triggering LTP and LTM in mammals, as R. Douglas Fields and others had demonstrated and thus showed the temporal aspects of Ca2+ dynamics linked the stimulus and cellular response.

      Although a single neuron can receive signals from thousands of other neurons, it only has one method of sending a message- firing an impulse through the axion or being silent. Fields argued that it follows that genes in neurons are also turned on and off by the patterns of impulse firing. Fields' lab had been investigating nervous system development and plasticity to find how the brain self-organized itself in nervous system development. They tested fetal mice neurons and were able to show that different time patterns controlled different genes (Itoh et al., 1995). Yet there remained another communication issue- having been triggered by a pattern of impulse firing to impact on a gene, how did the signal travel through the neuron itself and give the nucleus the right message?

      Information reaches the nucleus by regulating the influx of calcium ions through voltage-sensitive channels in the cell membrane. Intracellular channels in the brain have high levels of calcium ions, CA+2 yet inside the neuron levels are 20,000 times lower. When the neurons fire calcium channels open briefly converting the electrical signaling to a chemical time code CREB relays to a neuron's nucleus. Fields' Lab was able to show different temporal patterns regulated different genes: it was not the quantity of calcium entering the neuron but the temporal code it established.

      What was the temporal code that triggered LTP and LTM? After many trials, the lab found if the same high-frequency stimulus is applied repeatedly (three times in their experiments) spaced by 10 min without stimuli, the synapse becomes strengthened in LTP. This CREB-based spaced learning (CBSL) code could occur in the first half hour or up to 2 h later as indicated in the synaptic tagging and capture model. As humans need to acquire LTM of large bodies of complex information in education, CBSL attracted my attention. After years of development (Kelley, 2007), CBSL was finally successfully demonstrated with adolescent subjects and national educational tests in 2013. Surprisingly, the study found that in English national tests subjects taught using CBSL learned quicker and performed equally as well subjects taught conventionally (Kelley and Whatson, 2013).

      Other researchers used CBSL successfully in different contexts (Vaz, 2014; Garzia et al., 2016; Hägglöf, 2016) leading to support for wider use in education (Churches et al., 2017) and field trials. The billion-euro FuturICT project proposal recommended CBSL as 1 of 10 ways of meeting education's Grand Challenge to enable people to learn at orders of magnitude more effectively (Reynolds, 2010; Johnson et al., 2012; Bocconi et al., 2013). The UK's Open University Institute of Technology and Singapore's National Institute of Education cited CBSL as a promising education innovation to create LTM (Sharples et al., 2016).

      A very different application of CBSL suggests potential applications in medical training (Boettcher et al., 2018). In this random controlled study subjects had comparable baseline skills in suture procedures, but subjects that trained via CBSL were superior in terms of improved laparoscopic performance, knot quality, knot score and suture strength. Additionally, CBCL decreased anxiety and impression of challenge compared to controls. The authors concluded CBSL is very suitable for complex motor skill acquisition like laparoscopic suturing and recommended CBSL be used in training courses and surgical programs.

      Ironically the key research question in CBSL development and trials had been whether CBSL timing could create any LTM at all. Even the idea of using a specific time code drawn from mammal in vitro experiments with humans was considered impossible by many. It is a measure of CBSL research to date that it has raised the possibility that CBSL could be equally effective and more time-efficient. Perhaps more significant, CBSL shows the how detailed neuroscience research in LTM processes can be used to create potentially powerful applications in education and medical practice.

      Memory consolidation in sleep

      During wake learning can be manipulated and easily tested, whereas this is not possible in sleep. This stark contrast between wake and sleep processes has meant that consolidation processes in sleep are less well-understood. New findings suggest while wake is a brain state that enables encoding, sleep is a brain state that enables memory consolidation, and models of memory consolidation processes are beginning to emerge in which the role of REM and slow-wave sleep (SWS) in memory is associated with relevant experience to be re-enforced (Rasch and Born, 2013; Sara, 2017). In a ground-breaking f MRI study of human adults, it was possible to demonstrate that encoding and retrieval memory processes were anatomically distinct (Gabrieli et al., 1997). This distinction in location arose from testing the differences between familiar and unfamiliar images, demonstrating specialization of mnemonic processes in the human medial temporal lobe memory systems. These findings also demonstrated f MRI neuroimaging could be a powerful tool for tracking memory systems. More frequently human studies of consolidation in sleep rely on tests before and after sleep, and these have indicated that novel, reward, intense, repeated, or emotional stimuli are more likely to be encoded and consolidated (Carr et al., 2011; Wilhelm et al., 2011). Some studies indicate specific features, such as negative emotional events are encoded more often that neutral events (Wiesner et al., 2015), and repetition is most effective when there are specific time codes as in CBSL. Consolidation processes in sleep remain only partially understood, and can occur immediately after a single night's sleep, short periods of quiet restfulness during wake (Carr et al., 2011) or after a longer period.

      Human and animal studies alike do not yet answer the fundamental question of consolidation: how do sleep processes identify which memories to consolidate? Logically, new memories that have been through the process of LTP to LTM during wake should trigger consolidation, but the biological processes are only just emerging. Following LTM memory consolidation processes appear to include both hippocampal and cortical sharp-wave ripples with sleep spindles, suggesting cortical ripples may form part of the hippocampal-cortical dialogue during NREM sleep and may be crucial to memory consolidation (Khodagholy et al., 2017; Kitamura et al., 2017). It also appears new LTM may be incorporated into a shared neural ensemble or schema (Andersen et al., 1971; Cai et al., 2016). This process may occur during synaptic down-scaling during slow wave sleep (SWS) (Tononi and Cirelli, 2006; Vyazovskiy et al., 2008). Other studies show postsynaptic dendritic spines are formed on different sets of dendritic branches in response to different learning tasks and are protected from being eliminated when multiple tasks are learned and reactivated during subsequent non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep (Yang et al., 2014). This in turn suggests consolidation of new memories into shared neural ensembles may occur during SWS during sharp-wave ripple complexes (SPW-Rs) in sleep that replay neural learned sensory information (Sutherland and McNaughton, 2000; Lewis and Durrant, 2011). In contrast disrupting SPW-Rs, and therefore replay, impairs memory retention and memory consolidation (Girardeau et al., 2009; Ego-Stengel and Wilson, 2010).

      Yet how can these complex memory consolidation processes be co-ordinated on a cellular level? In an elegant study, it was possible to show that firing antidromic action potentials was probably a key mechanism (Bukalo et al., 2013). Neurons that participate in SPW-Rs are distinguished from non-participating neurons by firing antidromically, inducing synaptic depression throughout the cell. Subsequent synaptic stimulation delivered after antidromic firing triggers a long-lasting increase in synaptic strength. More generally, this process enables the creation of new neuronal ensembles. It is therefore reasonable to hypothesize that memory consolidation itself is the active processes that occur during SWS, enabled by entopic neuronal firing, linking new memories to other neuronal ensembles and possibly enabling transferring memories through SPW-Rs synchronisations. This would be consistent with sleep deprivation or disruption having a negative impact on hippocampus-dependent memory in both humans and rats and indicating a similar impact on memory consolidation itself during sleep (Abel et al., 2013; Brancaccio et al., 2017; Krause et al., 2017). In humans, different brain areas respond differently by circadian rhythmicity and sleep deprivation: in sustained-attention and cognitive tasks cortical responses decreased significantly with sleep deprivation and circadian misalignment while subcortical areas showed variation that primarily followed circadian modulation (Muto et al., 2016). This ground-breaking f MRI study suggests that cognitive processes are more liable to poorer performance from circadian and sleep disruption than other brain functions. This may indicate that both sleep deprivation and circadian misalignment of normal sleep periods might have a negative impact both on encoding and consolidation processes. These complex neuroscience issues are by no means fully resolved.

      Conclusion

      The first 24 h of a new memory occurs in a circadian temporal frame in which alertness can be different for individuals, dependent on whether the individual wakes sleep deprived or in synchrony with their circadian rhythms. Social time in UTC can be different than GBT, and start times for school and work tend to be early in most societies, and this will disadvantage evening chronotypes. Sleep deprivation can have a negative impact on learning and memory processes, decreasing the ability to encode new memories though LTP and LTM processes, and may disrupt consolidation in sleep. Individuals have distinctive circadian patterns by chronotype that determine the best times for cognitive functions, as shown in optimum times in their 24-h profiles that identify best times (Evans et al., 2017). Studies of learning and memory that do not control for circadian variations may be flawed or fail to identify chronotypes as a significant factor in the variations in outcomes.

      Currently rigorous application of discoveries in learning and memory neuroscience to human learning and memory are rare. It may be that CBSL is the first such application using a specific time code of activity and rest and drawn from primary neuroscience research that is able to offer detailed account of learning in the first 24-h period. There is every reason to think that different timing protocols will be identified by human neuroscience research. Although sleep and LTM consolidation are less accessible to experiments, sleep has time patterns of activity and rest every 90 min that can be tracked (Carskadon and Dement, 2005) and there is growing evidence that can clarify the different functions of REM and SWS sleep in consolidation processes (Li et al., 2017).

      Learning and memory is information that is encoded in the pattern of neuron firing after LTP and LTM processes, and during these processes the necessary biological mechanisms are attuned to temporal signals that are both internal and external. This review has considered the internal timing mechanisms in the first 24 h of a memory, yet this is only the starting point. The interactions between circadian, encoding and consolidating time codes in the first 24 h is perhaps the best-understood LTM research area and offers an opportunity to identify more optimal time patterns for learning in individuals and groups. This may, in turn, inform better social times for learning and work.

      Author contributions

      PK: neuroscience. ME and JK: social time and sociology.

      Conflict of interest statement

      PK is Chair of the Science Advisory Board of Download Learning, UK and on the Advisory Board of the charity Start School Later, U.S. The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      References Abdou K. Shehata M. Choko K. Nishizono H. Matsuo M. Muramatsu S. I. . (2018). Synapse-specific representation of the identity of overlapping memory engrams. Science 360, 12271231. 10.1126/science.aat381029903972 Abel T. Havekes R. Saletin J. M. Walker M. P. (2013). Sleep, plasticity and memory from molecules to whole-brain networks. Curr. Biol. 23, R774R788. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.02524028961 Abel T. Nguyen P. V. Barad M. Deuel T. A. Kandel E. R. Bourtchouladze R. (1997). Genetic demonstration of a role for PKA in the late phase of LTP and in hippocampus-based long-term memory. Cell 88, 615626. 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)81904-29054501 American Academy of Pediatrics (2014). Adolescent Sleep Working Group: school start times for adolescents. Pediatrics 134, 642649. 10.1542/peds.2014-1697 Andersen P. Bliss T. V. Skrede K. K. (1971). Unit analysis of hippocampal population spikes. Exp. Brain Res. 13, 208221. 10.1007/BF00234086 Atkinson R. C. Shiffrin R. M. (1968). Human memory: a proposed system and its control processes. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. 2, 89195. 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60422-3 Bargiello T. A. Jackson F. R. Young M. W. (1984). Restoration of circadian behavioral rhythms by gene transfer in Drosophila. Nature 312, 752754. 10.1038/312752a0 Basch C. E. Basch C. H. Ruggles K. V. Rajan S. (2014). Prevalence of sleep duration on an average school night among 4 nationally representative successive samples of American high school students, 2007–2013. Prev. Chronic Dis. 11:E216. 10.5888/pcd11.14038325496556 Bass J. Lazar M. A. (2016). Circadian time signatures of fitness and disease. Science 354, 994999. 10.1126/science.aah496527885004 Baudry M. Bi X. Gall C. Lynch G. (2011). The biochemistry of memory: the twenty-six year journey of a ‘new and specific hypothesis’. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 95, 125133. 10.1016/j.nlm.2010.11.015 Bocconi S. Kampylis P. Punie Y. (2013). Framing ICT-enabled innovation for learning: the case of one-to-one learning initiatives in Europe. Euro. J. Edu. 48, 113130. 10.1111/ejed.12021 Boettcher M. Boettcher J. Mietzsch S. Krebs T. Bergholz R. Reinshagen K. (2018). The spaced learning concept significantly improves training for laparoscopic suturing: a pilot randomized controlled study. Surg. Endosc. 32, 154159. 10.1007/s00464-017-5650-628643052 Borlase B. J. Gander P. H. Gibson R. H. (2013). Effects of school start times and technology use on teenagers' Sleep: 1999–2008. Sleep Biol. Rhythms 11, 4654. 10.1111/sbr.12003 Bourtchuladze R. Frenguelli B. Blendy J. Cioffi D. Schutz G. Silva A. J. (1994). Deficient long-term memory in mice with a targeted mutation of the cAMP-responsive element-binding protein. Cell 79, 5968. 10.1016/0092-8674(94)90400-67923378 Brancaccio M. Patton A. P. Chesham J. E. Maywood E. S. Hastings M. H. (2017). Astrocytes control circadian timekeeping in the suprachiasmatic nucleus via glutamatergic signaling. Neuron 93, 14201435. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.02.03028285822 Bukalo O. Campanac E. Hoffman D. A. Douglas Fields R. D. (2013). Synaptic plasticity by antidromic firing during hippocampal network oscillations. Proc. Nat. Acad. States. U.S.A. 110, 51755180. 10.1073/pnas.121073511023479613 Cai D. J. Aharoni D. Shuman T. Shobe J. Biane J. Song W. . (2016). A shared neural ensemble links distinct contextual memories encoded close in time. Nature 534, 115. 10.1038/nature1795527251287 Cajochen C. (2007). Alerting effects of light. Sleep Med. Rev. 11, 453464. 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.00917936041 Cajochen C. Zeitzer J. M. Czeisler C. A. Dijk D. J. (2000). Dose-response relationship for light intensity and ocular and electroencephalographic correlates of human alertness. Behav. Brain Res. 115, 7583. 10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00236-910996410 Cappuccio F. P. Bakewell A. Taggart F. M. Ward G. Ji C. Sullivan J. P. . (2009). Implementing a 48 h EWTD-compliant rota for junior doctors in the UK does not compromise patients' safety: assessor-blind pilot comparison. QJM. Int. J. Med. 102, 271282. 10.1093/qjmed/hcp004 Cappuccio F. P. D'Elia L. Strazzullo P. Miller M. A. (2010). Sleep duration and all-cause mortality: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective Studies. Sleep 33, 585592. 10.1093/sleep/33.5.58520469800 Carr M. F. Jadhav S. P. Frank L. M. (2011). Hippocampal replay in the awake state: a potential substrate for memory consolidation and retrieval. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 147153. 10.1038/nn.273221270783 Carrell S. E. Maghakian T. West J. E. (2011). A's from Zzzz's? The causal effect of school start time on academic achievement of adolescents. Am. Econ. J. 3, 6281. 10.1257/pol.3.3.62 Carskadon M. A. (2011). Sleep's effects on cognition and learning in adolescence. Prog. Brain Res. 190, 137143. 10.1016/B978-0-444-53817-8.00008-621531249 Carskadon M. A. Dement W. C. (2005). Normal human sleep: an overview. Princ. Pract. Sleep Med. 4, 1323. 10.1016/B0-72-160797-7/50009-4 Carskadon M. A. Vieira C. Acebo C. (1993). Association between puberty and delayed phase preference. Sleep 16, 258262. 10.1093/sleep/16.3.2588506460 Cepeda N. Vul E. Rohrer D. Wixted J. Pashler H. (2008). Spacing effects in learning: a temporal ridgeline of optimal retention. Psychol. Sci. 19, 10951102. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02209.x Chang A. M. Aeschbach D. Duffy J. F. Czeisler C. A. (2015). Evening use of light-emitting eReaders negatively affects sleep, circadian timing, and next-morning alertness. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 12321237. 10.1073/pnas.141849011225535358 Chen C. C. Wu J. K. Lin H. W. Pai T. P. Fu T. F. Wu C. L. . (2012). Visualizing long-term memory formation in two neurons of the Drosophila brain. Science 335, 678685. 10.1126/science.121273522323813 Chklovskii D. B. Mel B. W. Svoboda K. (2004). Cortical rewiring and information storage. Nature 431, 782788. 10.1038/nature0301215483599 Churches R. Dommett E. Devonshire I. (2017). Neuroscience for Teachers: Applying Research Evidence from Brain Science. London: Crown House Publishing Ltd. Coren S. (1996). Daylight savings time and traffic accidents. N. Engl. J. Med. 334, 924925. 10.1056/NEJM1996040433414168596592 Curcio G. Ferrara M. De Gennaro L. (2006). Sleep loss, learning capacity and academic performance. Sleep Med. Rev. 10, 323337. 10.1016/j.smrv.2005.11.00116564189 Czeisler C. A. (2011). Impact of sleepiness and sleep deficiency on public health—utility of biomarkers. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 7 (Suppl.), S6S8. 10.5664/JCSM.134022003334 Diekelmann S. Born J. (2010). The memory function of sleep. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 114126. 10.1038/nrn2762 Dudek S. M. Fields R. D. (2002). Somatic action potentials are sufficient for late-phase LTP-related cell signaling. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 19, 39623967. 10.1073/pnas.062510599 Duffy J. F. Cain S. W. Chang A. M. Phillips A. J. K. Münch M. Y. Gronfier C. . (2011). Sex difference in the near-24-hour intrinsic period of the human circadian timing system. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 1560215608. 10.1073/pnas.101066610821536890 Ebbinghaus H. (1913). Memory: A Contribution to Experimental Psychology. New York, NY: Columbia University. 25206041 Ego-Stengel V. Wilson M. A. (2010). Disruption of ripple-associated hippocampal activity during rest impairs spatial learning in the rat. Hippocampus 20, 110. 10.1002/hipo.2070719816984 English J. D. Sweatt J. D. (1997). A requirement for the mitogen-activated protein kinase cascade in hippocampal long-term potentiation. J. Biol. Chem. 272, 1910319106. 10.1074/jbc.272.31.191039235897 Escribano C. Diaz-Morales J. F. Delgado P. Collado M. J. (2012). Morningness/eveningness and school performance among spanish adolescents: further evidence. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 22, 409413. 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.12.008 Evans M. D. R. Kelley P. Kelley J. (2017). Identifying the best times for cognitive functioning using new methods: matching university times to undergraduate chronotypes. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:188. 10.3389/fnhum.2017.0018828469566 Fields R. D. (2005). Making memories stick. Sci. Am. 292, 7481. 10.1038/scientificamerican0205-7415715394 Fields R. D. (2011). Imagining learning: the search for a memory trace. Neuroscientist 17, 185196. 10.1177/1073858410383696 Fields R. D. Araque A. Johansen-Berg H. Lim S. S. Lynch G. Nave K. A. . (2014). Glial biology in learning and cognition. Neuroscientist 20, 426431. 10.1177/107385841350446524122821 Fields R. D. Lee P. R. Cohen J. E. (2005). Temporal integration of intracellular Ca2+ signaling networks in regulating gene expression by action potentials. Cell Calcium 37, 433442. 10.1016/j.ceca.2005.01.01115820391 Ford H. (1926). Why I favor five days' work with six days' pay. World's Work, 613616. Foster R. G. Hankins M. W. (2007). Circadian vision. Curr. Biol. 17, R746R751. 10.1016/j.cub.2007.07.00717803920 Foster R. G. Peirson S. N. Wulff K. Winnebeck E. Vetter C. Roenneberg T. (2013). Sleep and circadian rhythm disruption in social jetlag and mental illness. Prog. Mol. Biol. Transl. Sci. 119, 325346. 10.1016/B978-0-12-396971-2.00011-723899602 Foster R. G. Wulff K. (2005). The rhythm of rest and excess. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 407414. 10.1038/nrn167015861183 Frey U. Huang Y. Y. Kandel E. R. (1993). Effects of cAMP simulate a late stage of LTP in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Science 260, 16611661. 10.1126/science.83890578389057 Frey U. Morris R. G. (1997). Synaptic tagging and long-term potentiation. Nature 385, 533536. 10.1038/385533a09020359 Gabrieli J. D. Brewer J. B. Desmond J. E. Glover G. H. (1997). Separate neural bases of two fundamental memory processes in the human medial temporal lobe. Science 276, 264266. 10.1126/science.276.5310.2649092477 Garzia M. Mangione G. R. Longo L. Pettenati M. C. (2016). Spaced learning and innovative teaching: school time, pedagogy of attention and learning awareness. Res. Edu. Media 8, 2237. 10.1515/rem-2016-0004 Giannotti F. Cortesi F. Sebastiani T. Ottaviano S. (2002). Circadian preference, sleep and daytime behaviour in adolescence. J. Sleep Res. 11, 191199. 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2002.00302.x12220314 Giedd J. N. Raznahan A. Mills K. L. Lenroot R. K. (2012). Review: magnetic resonance imaging of male/female differences in human adolescent brain anatomy. Biol. Sex Diff. 3:19. 10.1186/2042-6410-3-1922908911 Girardeau G. Benchenane K. Wiener S. I. Buzsáki G. Zugaro M. B. (2009). Selective suppression of hippocampal ripples impairs spatial memory. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 12221223. 10.1038/nn.238419749750 Gong Y. Chang L. Viola K. L. Lacor P. N. Lambert M. P. Finch C. E. . (2003). Alzheimer's disease-affected brain: presence of oligomeric Aβ ligands (ADDLs) suggests a molecular basis for reversible memory loss. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 1041710422. 10.1073/pnas.1834302100 Hagenauer M. H. Perryman J. I. Lee T. M. Carskadon M. A. (2009). Adolescent changes in the homeostatic and circadian regulation of sleep. Dev. Neurosci. 31, 276284. 10.1159/00021653819546564 Hägglöf C. (2016). Spaced Learning i Naturkunskapsundervisning på Gymnasieskolan. Småland region: Linnaeus University. Hammond E. C. (1964). Some preliminary findings on physical complaints from a prospective study of 1,064,004 men and women. Am. J. Public Health Nat. Health 54, 1123. 10.2105/AJPH.54.1.1114117648 Hardin P. E. Hall J. C. Rosbash M. (1990). Feedback of the Drosophila period gene product on circadian cycling of its messenger RNA levels. Nature 343, 536540. 10.1038/343536a02105471 Hebb D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological Approach. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. Hernandez P. J. Abel T. (2008). The role of protein synthesis in memory consolidation: progress amid decades of debate. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 89, 293311. 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.09.01018053752 Holtmaat A. Svoboda K. (2009). Experience-dependent structural synaptic plasticity in the mammalian brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 647658. 10.1038/nrn2699 Hsu C. Y. Gau S. S. Shang C. Y. Chiu Y. N. Lee M. B. (2012). Associations between chronotypes, psychopathology, and personality among incoming college students. Chronobiol. Int. 29, 491501. 10.3109/07420528.2012.66899522497432 Itoh K. Stevens B. Schachner M. Fields R. D. (1995). Regulated expression of the neural cell adhesion molecule L1 by specific patterns of neural impulses. Science 270, 13691372. 10.1126/science.270.5240.13697481827 Johnson J. Shum S. B. Willis A. Bishop S. Zamenopoulos T. Swithenby S. . (2012). The FuturICT education accelerator. Eur. Phys. J. Special Topics 2141, 215243. 10.1140/epjst/e2012-01693-0 Jonsson A. Sjöström T. A. Tybrandt K. Berggren M. Simon D. T. (2016). Chemical delivery array with millisecond neurotransmitter release. Sci. Adv. 2:e1601340. 10.1126/sciadv.160134027847873 Kandel E. R. Dudai Y. Mayford M. R. (2014). The molecular and systems biology of memory. Cell 27, 163186. 10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.001 Karpicke J. D. Bauernschmidt A. (2011). Spaced retrieval: absolute spacing enhances learning regardless of relative spacing. J. Exp Psychol. Learn Mem. Cogn. 375, 12501257. 10.1037/a0023436 Karpicke J. D. Blunt J. R. (2011). Retrieval practice produces more learning than elaborative studying with concept mapping. Science 331, 772775. 10.1126/science.119932721252317 Karpicke J. D. Roediger H. L. (2008). The critical importance of retrieval for learning. Science 319, 966968. 10.1126/science.115240818276894 Kelley P. (2007). Making Minds: What's Wrong with Education-and What Should We Do About It? London: Routledge. Kelley P. Lockley S. W. Foster R. G. Kelley J. (2015). Synchronizing education to adolescent biology: ‘let teens sleep, start school later’. Learn. Media Technol. 40, 210226. 10.1080/17439884.2014.942666 Kelley P. Lockley S. W. Kelley J. Evans M. D. R. (2017). Is 8:30 a.m. still too early to start school? a 10:00 a.m. school start time improves health and performance of students aged 13–16. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:588. 10.3389/fnhum.2017.0058829276481 Kelley P. Whatson T. (2013). Making long-term memories in minutes: a spaced learning pattern from memory research in education. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:589. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.0058924093012 Khodagholy D. Gelinas J. N. Buzsáki G. (2017). Learning-enhanced coupling between ripple oscillations in association cortices and hippocampus. Science 358, 369372. 10.1126/science.aan620329051381 Kirkby M. Maggi S. D'Angiulli A. (2011). School start times and the sleep–wake cycle of adolescents: a review and critical evaluation of available evidence. Educ. Res. 40, 5661. 10.3102/0013189X11402323 Kitamura T. Ogawa S. K. Roy D. S. Okuyama T. Morrissey M. D. Smith L. M. . (2017). Engrams and circuits crucial for systems consolidation of a memory. Science 356, 7378. 10.1126/science.aam680828386011 Knoblauch A. (2017). Impact of structural plasticity on memory formation and decline, in Rewiring the Brain: A Computational Approach to Structural Plasticity in the Adult Brain, eds van Ooyen A. Butz M. (London: Elsevier/Academic Press), 361386. Knoblauch A. Sommer F. T. (2016). Structural plasticity, effectual connectivity, and memory in cortex. Front. Neuroanat. 10:63. 10.3389/fnana.2016.0006327378861 Krause A. J. Simon E. B. Mander B. A. Greer S. M. Saletin J. M. Goldstein-Piekarski A. N. . (2017). The sleep-deprived human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 404418. 10.1038/nrn.2017.5528515433 Lewis P. A. Durrant S. J. (2011). Overlapping memory replay during sleep builds cognitive schemata. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 343351. 10.1016/j.tics.2011.06.00421764357 Li W. Ma L. Yang G. Gan W. B. (2017). REM sleep selectively prunes and maintains new synapses in development and learning. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 427437. 10.1038/nn.447928092659 Liu X. Zwiebel L. J. Hinton D. Benzer S. Hall J. C. Rosbash M. (1992). The period gene encodes a predominantly nuclear protein in adult Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 12, 27352744. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-07-02735.19921613555 Lockley S. W. Cronin J. W. Evans E. E. Cade B. E. Lee C. J. Landrigan C. P. Rothschild J. M. . (2004). Effect of reducing interns' weekly work hours on sleep and attentional failures. N. Engl. J. Med. 351, 18291837. 10.1056/NEJMoa04140415509816 Lockley S. W. Evans E. E. Scheer F. A. Brainard G. C. Czeisler C. A. Aeschbach D. (2006). Short-wavelength sensitivity for the direct effects of light on alertness, vigilance, and the waking electroencephalogram in humans. Sleep 29, 161168. 10.1093/sleep/29.2.16116494083 Lockley S. W. Gooley J. J. (2006). Circadian photoreception: spotlight on the brain. Curr. Biol. 16, R795R797. 10.1016/j.cub.2006.08.03916979545 Lu Y. Christian K. Lu B. (2008). BDNF: a key regulator for protein synthesis-dependent LTP and long-term memory? Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 89, 312323. 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.08.01817942328 Lucas R. J. Peirson S. N. Berson D. M. Brown T. M. Cooper H. M. Czeisler C. A. . (2014). Measuring and using light in the melanopsin age. Trends Neurosci. 37, 19. 10.1016/j.tins.2013.10.00424287308 Luyster F. S. Strollo P. J. Jr. Zee P. C. Walsh J. K. (2012). Sleep: a health imperative. Sleep 35, 727734. 10.5665/sleep.184622654183 Man K. Loudon A. Chawla A. (2016). Immunity around the clock. Science 354, 9991003. 10.1126/science.aah496627885005 Menzel R. Manz G. Menzel R. Greggers U. (2001). Massed and spaced learning in honeybees: the role of CS, US, the intertrial interval, and the test interval. Learn. Mem. 8, 198208. 10.1101/lm.4000111533223 Merton R. K. (1936). The unanticipated consequences of purposive social action. Am. Sociol. Rev. 1, 894904. 10.2307/2084615 Millman R. P. (2005). Excessive sleepiness in adolescents and young adults: causes, consequences, and treatment strategies. Pediatrics 115, 17741786. 10.1542/peds.2005-077215930245 Möller-Levet C. S. Archer S. N. Bucca G. Laing E. E. Slak A. Kabiljo R. . (2013). Effects of insufficient sleep on circadian rhythmicity and expression amplitude of the human blood transcriptome. Proc. Nat. Soc. Sci. U.S.A. 110, E1132E1141. 10.1073/pnas.121715411023440187 Moncada D. Ballarini F. Martinez M. C. Frey J. U. Viola H. (2011). Identification of transmitter systems and learning tag molecules involved in behavioral tagging during memory formation. Proc. Nat. Soc. Sci. U.S.A 108, 1293112936. 10.1073/pnas.110449510821768371 Morris R. G. (2003). Long-term potentiation and memory. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 358, 643647. 10.1098/rstb.2002.123012740109 Musiek E. S. Holtzman D. M. (2016). Mechanisms linking circadian clocks, sleep, and neurodegeneration. Science 354, 10041008. 10.1126/science.aah496827885006 Muto V. Jaspar M. Meyer C. Kuss,é C. Chellappa S. L. Degueldre C. . (2016). Local modulation of human brain responses by circadian rhythmicity and sleep debt. Science 353, 687690. 10.1126/science.aad299327516598 Naqib F. Sossin W. S. Farah C. A. (2012). Molecular determinants of the spacing effect. Neural Plast. 2012:581291. 10.1155/2012/58129122548194 National Sleep Foundation (2017). Available online at: https://sleepfoundation.org/press-release/national-sleep-foundation-recommends-new-sleep-times/page/0/1 (Accessed November 20, 2017). Nguyen P. V. Abel T. Kandel E. R. (1994). Requirement of a critical period of transcription for induction of late phase LTP. Science 265, 11041108. 10.1126/science.80664508066450 Panda S. (2016). Circadian physiology of metabolism. Science 354, 10081015. 10.1126/science.aah496727885007 Pavlov I. P. (2010). Conditioned reflexes: an investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. Ann. Neurosci. 17, 136141. 10.5214/ans.0972-7531.101730925205891 Perazzona B. Isabel G. Preat T. Davis R. L. (2004). The role of cAMP response element-binding protein in Drosophila long-term memory. J. Neurosci. 24, 88238828. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4542-03.200415470148 Phipps-Nelson J. Redman J. R. Dijk D. J. Rajaratnam S. M. (2003). Daytime exposure to bright light, as compared to dim light, decreases sleepiness and improves psychomotor vigilance performance. Sleep 26, 695700. 10.1093/sleep/26.6.69514572122 Pilcher J. J. Huffcutt A. I. (1966). Effects of sleep deprivation on performance: a meta-analysis. Sleep 19, 318326. 10.1093/sleep/19.4.3188776790 Pitman R. K. Rasmusson A. M. Koenen K. C. Shin L. M. Orr S. P. Gilbertson M. W. . (2012). Biological studies of post-traumatic stress disorder. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 769787. 10.1038/nrn333923047775 Plaçais P. Preat T. (2013). To favour survival under food shortage, the brain disables costly memory. Science 339, 440442. 10.1126/science.1226018 Popovych O. V. Xenakis M. N. Tass P. A. (2015). The spacing principle for unlearning abnormal neuronal synchrony. PLoS ONE 10:e0117205. 10.1371/journal.pone.011720525714553 Preckel F. Lipnevich A. A. Boehme K. Brandner L. Georgi Könen T. Mursin K. . (2013). Morningness–eveningness and educational outcomes: the lark has an advantage over the owl at high school. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 83(Pt. 1), 114134. 10.1111/j.2044-8279.2011.02059.x23369178 Price J. L. Blau J. Rothenfluh A. Abodeely M. Kloss B. Young M. W. (1998). Double-time is a novel Drosophila clock gene that regulates PERIOD protein accumulation. Cell 94, 8395. 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)81224-69674430 Randler C. Frech D. (2006). Correlation between morningness – eveningness and final school leaving exams. Biol. Rhythm Res. 37, 233239. 10.1080/09291010600645780 Rasch B. Born J. (2013). About sleep's role in memory. Physiol. Rev. 93, 681766. 10.1152/physrev.00032.201223589831 Redondo R. L. Morris R. G. (2011). Making memories last: the synaptic tagging and capture hypothesis. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 1730. 10.1038/nrn296321170072 Reynolds D. (2010). Failure-Free Education? The Past, Present and Future of School Effectiveness and School Improvement. London: Routledge. Roenneberg T. Kuehnle T. Juda M. Kantermann T. Allebrandt K. Gordijn M. . (2007). Epidemiology of the human circadian clock. Sleep Med. Rev. 11, 429438. 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.00517936039 Roenneberg T. Kuehnle T. Pramstaller P. P. Ricken J. Havel M. Guth A. . (2004). A marker for the end of adolescence. Curr. Biol. 14, R1038R1039. 10.1016/j.cub.2004.11.03915620633 Rüger M. Gordijn M. C. Beersma D. G. de Vries B. Daan S. (2006). Time-of-day-dependent effects of bright light exposure on human psychophysiology: comparison of daytime and nighttime exposure. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 290, R1413R1420. 10.1152/ajpregu.00121.200516373441 Sara S. J. (2017). Sleep to remember. J. Neurosci. 37, 457463. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0297-16.201728100730 Schaeffer N. C. Presser S. (2003). The science of asking questions. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 29, 6588. 10.1146/annurev.soc.29.110702.110112 Scharf M. T. Woo N. H. Lattal K. M. Young J. Z. Nguyen P. V. Abel T. (2002). Protein synthesis is required for the enhancement of long-term potentiation and long-term memory by spaced training. J. Neurophysiol. 6, 27702777. 10.1152/jn.2002.87.6.2770 Schmidt C. Collette F. Leclercq Y. Sterpenich V. Vandewalle G. Berthomier P. . (2009). Homeostatic sleep pressure and responses to sustained attention in the suprachiasmatic area. Science 324, 516519. 10.1126/science.116733719390047 Scoville W. B. Milner B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 20, 1121. 10.1136/jnnp.20.1.1113406589 Sharples M. de Roock R. Ferguson R. Gaved M. Herodotou C. Koh E. . (2016). Innovating Pedagogy 2016: Open University Innovation Report 5. Milton Keynes: The Open University. Institute of Educational Technology, The Open University, Walton Hall, Milton Keynes, MK7 6AA. Shires K. L. Da Silva B. M. Hawthorne J. P. Morris R. G. Martin S. J. (2012). Synaptic tagging and capture in the living rat. Nat. Commun. 3:1246. 10.1038/ncomms225023212375 Siwicki K. K. Eastman C. Petersen G. Rosbash M. Hall J. C. (1988). Antibodies to the period gene product of Drosophila reveal diverse tissue distribution and rhythmic changes in the visual system. Neuron 1, 141150. 10.1016/0896-6273(88)90198-53152288 Smith A. M. Floerke V. A. Thomas A. K. (2016). Retrieval practice protects memory against acute stress. Science 354, 10461048. 10.1126/science.aah506727885031 Sørensen K. Mouritsen A. Aksglaede L. Hagen C. P. Mogensen S. S. Juul A. (2012). Recent secular trends in pubertal timing: implications for evaluation and diagnosis of precocious puberty. Hormone Res. Paediatr. 77, 137145. 10.1159/00033632522508036 Sorokin P. A. Merton R. K. (1937). Social time. Am. J. Sociol. 42, 615629. Sutherland G. R. McNaughton B. (2000). Memory trace reactivation in hippocampal and neocortical neuronal ensembles. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 10, 180186. 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00079-910753801 Thomas M. Sing H. Belenky G. Holcomb H. Mayberg H. Dannals R. . (2000). Neural basis of alertness and cognitive performance impairments during sleepiness. I. Effects of 24 h of sleep deprivation on waking human regional brain activity. J. Sleep Res. 9, 335352. 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2000.00225.x11123521 Tononi G. Cirelli C. (2006). Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. Sleep Med. Rev. 10, 4962. 10.1016/j.smrv.2005.05.00216376591 Tourangeau R. Rips L. J. Rasinski K. (2000). The Psychology of Survey Response. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Turek F. W. (2016). Circadian clocks: not your grandfather's clock. Science 354, 992993. 10.1126/science.aal261327885003 Van Dongen H. P. Maislin G. Mullington J. M. Dinges D. F. (2003). The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep 26, 117126. 10.1093/sleep/26.2.11712683469 Vaz A. C. (2014). Spaced Learning: Making Space for Neuroscience in the Classroom. E-Teals. Vetter C. Fischer D. Matera J. L. Roenneberg T. (2015). Aligning work and circadian time in shift workers improves sleep and reduces circadian disruption. Curr. Biol. 25, 907911. 10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.06425772446 Vyazovskiy V. V. Cirelli C. Pfister-Genskow M. Faraguna U. Tononi G. (2008). Molecular and electrophysiological evidence for net synaptic potentiation in wake and depression in sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 200208. 10.1038/nn203518204445 Wahlstrom K. Dretzke B. Gordon M. Peterson K. Edwards K. Gdula J. (2014). Examining the Impact of Later School Start Times on the Health and Academic Performance of High School Students: A Multi-Site Study. CAREI. St. Paul, MN: University of Minnesota. Wald G. (1968). Molecular basis of visual excitation. Science 162, 230239. 10.1126/science.162.3850.2304877437 Wheaton A. G. Ferro G. A. Croft J. B. (2015). School start times for middle school and high school students- United States, 2011–12 school year. MMWR Morb. Mortal. Wkly. Rep. 64, 809813. 10.15585/mmwr.mm6430a126247433 Wiesner C. D. Pulst J. Krause F. Elsner M. Baving L. Pedersen A. . (2015). The effect of selective REM-sleep deprivation on the consolidation and affective evaluation of emotional memories. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 122, 131141. 10.1016/j.nlm.2015.02.00825708092 Wilhelm I. Diekelmann S. Molzow I. Ayoub A. Mölle M. Born J. (2011). Sleep selectively enhances memory expected to be of future relevance. J. Neurosci. 31, 15631569. 10.1523/JNEUROSC.3575-10.201121289163 Wulff K. Gatti S. Wettstein J. G. Foster R. G. (2011). Sleep and circadian rhythm disruption in psychiatric and neurodegenerative disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 589599. 10.1038/nrn286820631712 Xue G. Mei L. Chen C. Lu Z. L. Poldrack R. Dong Q. (2011). Spaced learning enhances subsequent recognition memory by reducing neural repetition suppression. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 16241633. 10.1162/jocn.2010.2153220617892 Yang G. Lai C. S. W. Cichon J. Ma L. Li W. Gan W. B. (2014). Sleep promotes branch-specific formation of dendritic spines after learning. Science 344, 11731178. 10.1126/science.124909824904169 Yasseri T. Sumi R. Kertész J. (2012). Circadian patterns of wikipedia editorial activity: a demographic analysis. PLoS ONE 7:e30091. 10.1371/journal.pone.003009122272279 Zaidi F. H. Hull J. T. Peirson S. N. Wulff K. Aeschbach D. Gooley J. J. . (2007). Short-wavelength light sensitivity of circadian, pupillary, and visual awareness in humans lacking an outer retina. Curr. Biol. 17, 21222128. 10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.03418082405 Zerubavel E. (1982). The standardization of time: a sociohistorical perspective. Am. J. Sociol. 88 123. Zerubavel E. (1985). Hidden Rhythms: Schedules and Calendars in Social Life. Berkeley; Los Angeles, CA: University of California Press. Zhang R. Lahens N. F. Ballance H. I. Hughes M. E. Hogenesch J. B. (2014). A circadian gene expression atlas in mammals: implications for biology and medicine. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 1621916224. 10.1073/pnas.140888611125349387
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016www.l93fh.net.cn
      www.fcchain.com.cn
      hjlhhj.com.cn
      lr-ade.com.cn
      www.ijoclf.com.cn
      www.omseoe.com.cn
      perfdog.com.cn
      www.wybgyp.com.cn
      wztesr.com.cn
      wnxntp.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p