Front. Ethol. Frontiers in Ethology Front. Ethol. 2813-5091 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fetho.2023.1256380 Ethology Original Research Gaming the system: how predators induce prey to make themselves more vulnerable Ydenberg Ron * Young Sherry Sullivan-Lord Rachel Centre for Wildlife Ecology, Department of Biological Sciences, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada

Edited by: Rulon Clark, San Diego State University, United States

Reviewed by: Yann Henaut, ECOSUR Conservation and Biodiversity, Mexico; William Pitt, Smithsonian Conservation Biology Institute (SI), United States

*Correspondence: Ron Ydenberg, ydenberg@sfu.ca

11 10 2023 2023 2 1256380 10 07 2023 28 08 2023 Copyright © 2023 Ydenberg, Young and Sullivan-Lord 2023 Ydenberg, Young and Sullivan-Lord

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

We describe a natural situation that supports predictions of theoretical scenarios in which predators tactically influence the food - safety trade-off faced by prey to increase their vulnerability. By using low-cost ‘false attacks’ or otherwise advertising their presence, predators force prey to spend time in refuges or in other forms of safety-enhancing behavior, during which foraging is impaired or impossible. Prey must compensate by taking extra risks at other times or places to meet their energy requirements, and as a consequence become easier to capture. We used data on the occurrence of over-ocean flocking (OOF) by Pacific dunlins (Calidris alpina pacifica), and on the timing and success of attacks by peregrines. OOF is a safe but energetically expensive alternative to traditional roosting, and largely replaced the latter in Boundary Bay of southwest British Columbia as the presence of wintering peregrines rose during the 1990s. Peregrines appear to use ‘false’ or ‘non-serious’ attacks to shift the occurrence of OOF to a tidal time frame earlier than is ideal for dunlins, thereby creating later hunting opportunities during which dunlins were vulnerable than otherwise would have been the case. The shift increased dunlin mortality substantially. Tactics used by predators such as prominent perching, salient signals and unpredictable appearances, could have evolved because this forces prey to increase their level of caution, rendering them more vulnerable at other times or places.

predator - prey games escape foraging trade off deception compromise section-in-acceptance Foraging and Antipredator Behavior

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      It is rarely easy to catch prey, even for top predators considered to be highly effective. Based on a lifetime of watching wild peregrine falcons (Falco peregrinus) for example, Dekker (2009) estimated that the success rate of hunts is only ~14%. The ‘life – dinner’ principle attributes this to the idea that selection against unsuccessful escape is more stringent than against an unsuccessful hunt (but see Humphreys and Ruxton, 2020). Wolf and Mangel (2007) model a predator-prey behavioral game, novel in that it exposes a largely unexplored class of tactics that predators could theoretically use to heighten capture success.

      Wolf & Mangel’s model is motivated by transient (also called Bigg’s) killer whales (Orcina orca) hunting Steller sea lions, Eumetopias jubatus (Baird and Dill, 1995). In the model both killer whales and sea lions must capture enough prey to avoid starvation, and sea lions must also avoid capture by killer whales. Sea lions are safer when foraging close to a haulout to which they can flee for refuge if killer whales appear - but prey are less abundant there (‘Ashmole’s halo’; Gaston et al., 2008). The model explores whether killer whales could influence this behavioral trade-off to their advantage.

      When killer whales are absent, sea lions can forage without regard for the danger these top predators pose. As the presence of killer whales increases sea lions spend more time fleeing or on the refuge. If high enough, sea lions may have to forage at greater distances in order to meet their requirements. The accessibility of a haulout also affects the hunting tactics of killer whales. If sea lions are easily able to flee to a refuge, killer whales must increase their hunting activity to capture enough prey. In a similar vein, Beauchamp and Ruxton (2012) and Beauchamp (2016) asks whether and how predators could adjust the timing of attacks to influence hunting success. In this game prey are ‘responsive’ to predator stimuli, gradually relaxing their vigilance following an attack. A predator that waits longer between attacks thus faces less wary prey, but risks missing opportunities. Prey that remain wary for too long lose too much foraging time. In both these scenarios, the best tactic for each depends on that used by the other.

      Analyzing the interactions of strategic agents such as predators and prey requires a game theoretical approach (Houston and McNamara, 1999). Wolf and Mangel (2007) show how an evolutionary stable strategy (ESS) pair can be specified (using the ‘best response’ procedure; McNamara et al., 1999) at which killer whales cannot improve the rate of prey capture by altering the attack rate, nor can sea lions improve their survival by altering the foraging distance. But predators have an additional tactic: they can make ‘false’ or ‘non-serious’ attacks that avoid the costs (e.g. risk of injury, stalking time) of a serious predation attempt. Such attacks may have low probability of success, but they force prey to take evasive actions or otherwise increase caution. Killer whales, for example, may cruise near haulouts prominently displaying their presence, forcing sea lions to spend more time out of the water. Under some circumstances this could force sea lions to compensate by foraging at greater distances, where they can later be more easily captured.

      There are many descriptions in the literature of predators using tactics such as stealth, camouflage, lures or scaring techniques. Tactical deception of competitors for food (e.g. Munn, 1986) or mates (Jukema and Piersma, 2006) has also been described. For example, when scrambling against a competitor for a prey item, bluish-slate antshrikes (Thamnomanes schistogynus; a sentinel species in neotropical ant-following mixed-flocks of birds) sometimes give an alarm call - even if a predator is not present - gaining an advantage when the competitor pauses to scan for a predator. But do predatory tactics of the sort envisioned by Wolf and Mangel (2007) actually happen in nature? Here we describe a natural situation that appears to match their hypothetical scenario.

      Pacific dunlins (Calidris alpina pacifica) are shorebirds that winter in large groups on temperate mudflats. Falcons (merlins Falco columbarius and especially peregrines Falco peregrinus) are their most important predators (Page and Whitacre, 1975; Dekker et al., 2012). Raptors are generally much more successful when able to surprise prey (Cresswell, 1996). At Boundary Bay in southwestern British Columbia, the majority of hunts made by peregrines are ‘open’ (i.e. pursuit far out over the mudflat). However most kills are made by ‘stealth hunting’ (62%, Dekker and Ydenberg, 2004), even though the tidally-driven availability of dunlins foraging close enough to shoreline cover to be ambushed supports only 12% of the hunts observed. Open hunts are usually prolonged, but stealth hunts are short, and abandoned if initially unsuccessful.

      Dunlins and other shorebirds gather in large, dense groups to sleep and preen while the tide is high (‘roosting’). At Boundary Bay Pacific dunlins largely abandoned this traditional form of roosting during the 1990s, and instead took up ‘over ocean flocking’ (‘OOF’ or ‘high tide flight’; Dekker, 1998; Ydenberg et al., 2010). Diurnal high tide periods are now often spent circling in a loose flock high over the bay several hundred metres from shore. OOF has occasionally been described at other wintering locations, but appears uncommon. Roosting high on the intertidal zone along the edge of or even amongst salt marsh vegetation (Conklin and Colwell, 2007) or in nearby fields is dangerous because falcons can hunt by stealth at such locations. The steady rise after the mid-1970s in the number of peregrines present during winter on Boundary Bay (Ydenberg et al., 2017) made OOF the preferred behavioral mode for the high tide period.

      Wolf and Mangel’s (2007) hypothesis is summarized in Figure 1 . The underlying concept is that foragers facing a shortfall (such as of foraging time, or in body condition) reduce the effort devoted to anti-predator behavior in order to be able to compensate (McNamara and Houston, 1990). Reports from migratory stopover sites, for example, show that individual shorebirds with low body condition are over-represented among predator kills (Dierschke, 2003; Yosef et al., 2011), presumably because they take greater risks in foraging (see Lank and Ydenberg, 2003). The Wolf – Mangel hypothesis is that predators exploit this fact by forcing prey to spend extra time or energy in avoidance, so shifting their activity to times or places at which they are more vulnerable.

      upper panel Schematic presentation of the Wolf – Mangel hypothesis applied to the peregrine – dunlin game. As the tide rises dunlins must forage closer to the shoreline, where peregrines are able to hunt by stealth; it becomes more dangerous as the tide level rises (solid curve). Over-ocean flocking (OOF) is much safer than foraging or roosting on the mudflat, but is energetically expensive. Dunlins ideally allocate the affordable duration of OOF (3.1h) to the highest tide levels (‘ideal OOF’ bar). Peregrines can induce an early start to OOF with ‘false’ or ‘non-serious’ attacks (1), which advance the onset of OOF (2), thereby creating a window of hunting opportunity (3) after OOF ceases and the tide is still high. The highest kill rate is observed during this window. lower panel The predicted (red) and observed (gray) allocation of OOF by dunlins at Boundary Bay, British Columbia, as a proportion of each of the hourly intervals before and after high tide. Red shading indicates a shortfall in OOF allocation, which creates hunting opportunity for peregrines. Hatched gray shading indicates over-allocation to OOF, superfluous because the danger level is low anyway.

      Here we describe the temporal distribution of OOF by dunlins during the high tide period, the hunting activity of peregrines, measure the kill rate inflicted, estimate if and by how much OOF lowers mortality, and evaluate whether peregrines are able to affect the temporal distribution of OOF to their advantage, as predicted by the Wolf – Mangel hypothesis.

      Methods

      Peregrines and dunlins were observed during winter visits of one - three weeks to Boundary Bay on the Fraser River estuary in southwestern British Columbia (49°50’N, 123°0’W), made by Dick Dekker between1994 and 2012 (reported in Dekker, 1998; Dekker and Ydenberg, 2004; Dekker, 2013). Boundary Bay is 16 km in length with an intertidal zone several kilometres wide, bordered by 10 – 250m of salt marsh. The tidal rhythm is semi-diurnal, with amplitude up to 4.1m. Observations were made from the gravelled road on the 2m high dike backing the salt marsh, sometimes from a parked vehicle. Vantage points give unobstructed views (~2 km or more in each direction) of tidal flats and salt marsh. Some 20 - 40,000 dunlins are winter residents in the area, usually foraging in large flocks (Shepherd and Lank, 2004). Peregrines are present throughout the winter (Lank et al., 2003).

      Over-ocean flocking by dunlins was recorded during dawn – dusk watches on 34 days in January 2006 and October 2008. Based on the majority of the flock, dunlins were classed at intervals of several minutes as engaged in OOF, or in foraging (spread over the mudflat surface, mostly along the tideline; see Jiménez et al., 2015). Other activities such as roosting were observed only rarely. The start and end of OOF were noted. OOF is easily distinguished from the other types of flight used by dunlins (foraging flights, transit flights, escape flights; see Reurink et al., 2016). It involves all or most of the dunlins present, and the large flock is usually easily visible as it circles aloft over the bay (Dekker, 1998). The flight mode is distinct, with much gliding, and low speed (Hentze, 2012). In very high winds, the flock may stream to and fro a few metres above the waves.

      The time and success of peregrine attacks was systematically recorded on 151 days, using methods described by Dekker (2013); (Dekker and Ydenberg, 2004). An ‘attack’ or ‘hunt’ is defined as one completed attempt by a peregrine to seize a prey of which the outcome could be ascertained. An attack was considered successful if the falcon seized a dunlin or forced it into the water. Captured prey were often kleptoparasitized by bald eagles and occasionally by other raptors (Dekker et al., 2012). Bald eagles do not themselves catch dunlins, and dunlins largely ignore them. Kleptoparasitism increases the level of depredation on dunlins by peregrines, in order to replace the prey lost to kleptoparasites.

      Our analyses are confined to the 6h period centered on the diurnal high tide. The observation time reported here is somewhat lower than the total reported in Dekker (2013) and Dekker and Ydenberg (2004), because their observation periods sometimes extended beyond the 6h interval. We divided the 6h high tide periods into one-hour blocks (3 – 2h, 2 – 1, and 1 – 0h before and after high tide) which for convenience are termed the ‘3h- pre-high tide interval’, the ‘2h- post-high tide interval’, etc. Poor weather or darkness sometimes interrupted observations, and combined with the second diurnal high tide that occurs on some days to spread observation hours unequally across the 6h tidal window.

      The first objective of our analysis was to assess whether OOF reduces the mortality inflicted by peregrines. Dekker’s (1998); (see also Ydenberg et al., 2010) hypothesis that OOF is an effective anti-predator tactic is supported by the fact that almost all observed kills were of dunlins not engaged in OOF. To make quantitative estimates we compare ‘risk’ and ‘danger’, terms defined by Lank and Ydenberg (2003)); Ydenberg et al., 2022; see also Creel et al., 2019 who use the equivalent terms ‘realized risk’ and ‘inherent danger’). Risk is the actual mortality (measured as kills h-1, in each of the six hourly intervals). Danger is the mortality that would occur if prey undertook no antipredator behavior (here OOF). We estimate the level of danger by the mortality rate (kills h-1) while dunlins were not engaged in OOF in each of the six hourly intervals.

      We tabulated for each of the six hourly intervals the observation time, the proportion dunlins spent in OOF (hence also the time not spent in OOF), and the number of kills observed. From these summary data we calculated the overall kill rate (total number of kills observed divided by total observation time). For each of the six hourly intervals we calculated the risk (kills per observation hour); the relative risk (relative to the average kill rate); and the level of danger (kill rate while dunlins were not engaged in OOF).

      We took the average time spent in OOF on the days that it occurred as the estimate of the time that could be allocated to OOF. For greatest effectiveness as an anti-predator defense, the time available for OOF should be allocated in descending order to the most dangerous hourly intervals (i.e. when not engaging in OOF would lead to highest mortality). The hypothesis predicts that peregrines would benefit from shifting the allocation of OOF to reduce its occurrence in the most dangerous hourly intervals. We used simple correlations among the six hourly intervals to compare (i) risk and danger; (ii) the allocation (prop.) of time to OOF and risk; and (iii) the allocation (prop.) of time to OOF and danger.

      Results Over-ocean flocking

      OOF was observed on 23 of the 34 days (68%), and dunlins spent 69.8 (57%) of the total of 123 observation hours on these days engaged in OOF. The 11 days without OOF included 7 days on which high tide occurred long before dawn, 2 days with continuous rain, one windless day with an early high tide, and one apparently ‘normal’ day. The mean daily duration of OOF over all 34 days was 2.05h, but it lasted on average 3.1h on the 23 days that it did occur. The temporal distribution of OOF observed over the 6h high tide period is summarized in Table 1 . The 1h intervals immediately pre- and post-high tide are almost entirely devoted to OOF, with the allocation declining in both directions from the high tide point.

      The occurrence of over-ocean flocking (OOF) by dunlins during the 6h window centered on high tide, in Boundary Bay, British Columbia.

      INTERVAL1 3h prior 2h prior 1h prior 1h post 2h post 3h post TOTALS
      obs time (h)2 14 18 25 22 22 22 123
      time in OOF (h)3 4.75 10.75 22.17 20.92 9.75 1.50 69.84
      prop. in OOF4 0.34 0.60 0.89 0.95 0.44 0.07 0.55

      The mean duration of OOF over all 34 observation days is 2.05h, and 3.1h on the 23 days that it did occur. OOF peaks in the 1h intervals just before and just after high tide.

      1Six successive one-hour intervals, beginning 3h prior to high tide, and ending 3h post high tide.

      2The number of observation hours in each of the intervals.

      3The hours spent in over-ocean flocking (OOF) by dunlins during each of the intervals.

      4The proportion of the interval spent in over-ocean flocking (OOF) by dunlins.

      Predation by peregrines

      A total of 139 kills of dunlins by peregrines was recorded during 905 observation hours, for an overall average risk of 0.15 h-1. The temporal distribution of kills over the 6h high tide period is uneven, and is summarized in Table 2 . Kills occurred at a rate lower than the average during the intervals 2h- and 3h- prior to high tide, slightly less often than the average around high tide, and at a rate higher than the average 2h- and 3h- after high tide. As noted by both Dekker and Ydenberg (2004) and by Dekker (2013); (see his Figure 3) there is a sharp peak in the kill rate during the 2h- post-high tide interval. This feature is of special interest.

      The occurrence of dunlin kills by peregrines during the 6h window centered on high tide, in Boundary Bay, British Columbia.

      Interval 1 3h prior 2h prior 1h prior 1h post 2h post 3h post TOTALS
      Observation time (h)2 133 147 158 170 159 139 905
      No. kills observed 12 11 22 24 43 27 139
      Risk (kills per hour) 3 0.09 0.07 0.14 0.14 0.27 0.19 0.15
      Relative risk 4 0.59 0.49 0.91 0.92 1.76 1.27 1
      Danger 5 0.14 0.19 1.23 2.88 0.49 0.21 0.36

      The average kill rate is 0.15 kills h-1, but is lower before high tide, about the average rate around high tide, and higher than average after high tide.

      1Successive one-hour intervals, beginning 3h prior to high tide, and ending 3h post high tide.

      2The number of observation hours in each of the intervals.

      3Risk is the kill rate (kills h-1).

      4The observed kill rate relative to the overall average, which is 0.15 kills h-1.

      5‘Danger’ is the kill rate that would be observed if dunlins took no defensive behavior (OOF). For example, Table 1 records that 0.34 of the 3h-prior high tide interval is spent in OOF, so 0.66 is non-OOF, making the non-OOF time 87.8h. Danger = 12 kills/87.8h = 0.14 kills per non-OFF hour.

      The key concept in our analysis is ‘danger’ – the kill rate that would be observed if dunlins did not engage in OOF (i.e. took no defensive measures). In each of the six intervals danger (overall average 0.36h-1) is higher than the risk (overall average 0.15h-1), as expected if the function of OOF is to provide safety from peregrines ( Table 3 ). As previously asserted (Dekker and Ydenberg, 2004; Dekker, 2013), the intervals just before and especially that just after the high tide point are the most dangerous ( Table 3 ).

      Danger and the observed temporal allocation of over-ocean flocking by dunlins, compared with that predicted to minimize mortality (‘µ min’).

      INTERVAL1 3h prior 2h prior 1h prior 1h past 2h post 3h post TOTALS
      Danger2 0.14 0.19 1.23 2.88 0.49 0.21 0.36
      µ min allocation3 0.00 0.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 0.10 3.10
      observed allocation4 0.34 0.60 0.89 0.95 0.44 0.07 3.10
      difference (prop) 5 0.34 0.60 -0.11 -0.05 -0.56 -0.03

      The observed OOF allocation is shifted leftward (earlier) from that which would minimize mortality.

      1Successive one-hour intervals, beginning 3h prior to high tide, and ending 3h post high tide.

      2‘Danger’ is the kill rate that would be observed if dunlins did not engage in OOF, measured as kills non-OOF h-1. See Table 2 for a sample calculation.

      3The allocation of OOF predicted to minimize mortality. To do so the available daily OOF time (3.1h) should be allocated in descending order to the most dangerous times.

      4The observed allocation of OOF time, from Table 1 .

      5The difference between the mortality minimizing and actual allocations, with a minus sign indicating that less time is allocated than would minimize mortality.

      Danger and risk both vary strongly among the six hourly intervals, but their levels are not at all related (r = 0.007), because dunlins devote a varying amount of time to OOF in each interval. The allocation (prop. of time) to OOF in each of the intervals is correlated with the danger level (r = 0.82; Table 3 ), as expected if the amount of defensive behavior increases with the level of danger. To minimize risk, the limited amount of OOF available (3.1h) should be allocated to the most dangerous intervals, which in descending order are the 1h- post-tide interval (danger 2.88, from Table 3 ), the 1h- pre-tide interval (danger 1.23), and the 2h- post-tide interval (danger 0.49). The small remaining time (0.1h) could be allocated to either the 3h- post-tide (danger 0.21) or the 2h- pre-tide interval (danger 0.19), which are almost equally dangerous. We allocated this to the former, because it has slightly higher danger. The match between the observed allocation and this pattern is moderate (r = 0.65; Table 3 ). The intervals just prior to and just after high tide are as predicted devoted (almost) entirely to OOF. Much more time than predicted is allocated to the intervals 3h- and 2h- prior to high tide, and much less than predicted to the 2h-post tide interval ( Table 3 ).

      Discussion

      Over-ocean flocking is safer but energetically more expensive than traditional roosting (Ydenberg et al., 2010), analogous to the long flights shorebirds sometimes make to distant but safer roosting sites (Brennan et al., 1985; Rogers et al., 2006; Rosa et al., 2006; Johnston-González and Abril, 2019). We observed that OOF occurred on 68% of winter days, and lasted on average 3.1h. Two other data sets collected at Boundary Bay totalling an additional 62 observation days report OOF timing closely matching these measures (Out, 2010; Hentze, 2012).

      An hour of OOF is estimated to add ~3.0 – 4.3% to the daily energy requirement of wintering dunlins at Boundary Bay (Hentze, 2012; see his Table 3.2), even when taking into account attributes that lower the energetic cost of time in the air (i.e. much gliding, minimum power speed; see Hedenström and Alerstam, 1996). The increased daily energy expenditure resulting from OOF is presumably financed by additional foraging, which could occur at night (Shepherd and Lank, 2004), or at other portions of the tidal cycle (Jiménez et al., 2015). As expected if a trade-off is operating, the incidence of OOF is reduced under conditions that make meeting the daily energetic requirement more challenging. For example, OOF does not occur on days without wind (flight is more expensive) or with heavy rain (foraging on mudflats is less successful; dunlins may forage at upland sites in such weather; Shepherd and Lank, 2004), and it lasts less long on cold days (higher daily energy expenditure; Dekker, 2013). OOF does not occur at all when safe roosting options are available, as when ice (Dekker, 2013) or driftwood (pers. obs.) floating in the bay provides roost sites distant from shore. Also as expected if OOF functions to counter the danger posed by (diurnal) falcons, Hentze (2012) confirmed using radar that OOF does not occur nocturnally. If ongoing, it ends abruptly at dusk and begins abruptly at dawn.

      The Wolf and Mangel (2007); see Introduction) hypothesis holds that ‘false’ attacks made prior to high tide induce dunlins to initiate OOF sooner than they otherwise would. The energetic expense of OOF limits its duration, so an early start results in correspondingly early termination, which because the tide is still high creates stealth hunting opportunities. The peak in mortality during the 2h- post-high tide interval (0.27 kills h-1 Table 2 ) results from the relatively low allocation to OOF (0.44) during a time of high danger (0.49 kills h-1).

      One test of the hypothesis would measure the incidence of half-hearted and serious attacks in relation to the tidal cycle. But even experienced observers cannot easily differentiate such hunts, making any such classification subjective. In his detailed descriptions of peregrine hunting tactics, Dekker (2009) does not use the term ‘false’ attack, but he does describe ‘half-hearted’ and ‘aborted’ attacks, as does the other veteran observer of wild peregrines (Ratcliffe, 1962). Dekker (1980) explains these attacks as part of ‘ …… an extended process, lasting several hours with frequent pauses …. The initial phase might be considered as a “warming-up” period, until it merges into a relatively brief period of serious hunting.” This verbal description matches the Wolf - Mangel hypothesis.

      Nevertheless, it is possible that the temporal pattern we observed is not a consequence of peregrine tactics. For example, attacks by less skilled individual predators early in the high tide period may force dunlins into OOF, while skilled individuals concentrate their hunting just after high tide, raising the kill rate in that interval. Testing this hypothesis would require that individual predators be identifiable, but peregrines were not marked in Dekker’s studies. The frequency of observed hunts by peregrines at Boundary Bay (as well as in the Bay of Fundy, where semipalmated sandpipers Calidris pusilla also engage in OOF; Dekker at al., 2012) shows a marked peak 2 - 4 hours before high tide (unpubl. data). This is consistent with the Wolf – Mangel hypothesis, and does not seem readily explained by variation in skill among individuals.

      A further hypothesis that does not invoke prescient predatory tactics by peregrines is that dunlins have a relatively low level of OOF during the 2h- post-tide interval because foraging is especially valuable at this time. For example, food availability may be highest on a falling tide (Esselink and Zwarts, 1989). Foraging during this dangerous time would be advantageous if the intake rate is high enough that the amount of foraging (and hence exposure to peregrines) required at other times of day is so reduced that total mortality is lowered. For this condition to hold, the reduction in mortality at other times would have to be larger than that incurred during the 2h-post high tide interval. This seems unlikely, as this interval accounts for 31% (43/139) of the kills observed. The danger more than 2 hours from high tide is very low, making it improbable that enough savings could be generated to compensate.

      To minimize risk dunlins should allocate the daily quota of OOF (~3.1h) to the intervals with greatest danger, namely from 1h- pre-high tide to 2h- post-high tide ( Table 2 ). This is when the tide level forces dunlins closest to the shoreline where peregrines can hunt by stealth. The observed allocation of OOF does not match this predicted pattern ( Table 3 ) very closely. While the intervals just before and after high tide are as predicted devoted almost entirely to OOF, it occupies less than half of the 2h-post high tide interval ( Table 1 ). Further, much of the 2h- and 3h-pre high tide intervals are spent in OOF (0.60 and 0.34 respectively, together ~66 minutes), in spite of the fact that danger is low (0.14 and 0.19 kills h-1, respectively). The mortality-minimizing allocation predicts no OOF at all during these intervals.

      The relative kill rate in each interval is inversely proportional to the mismatch between the observed and predicted OOF allocations, with the mortality higher when too little time is spent in OOF ( Figure 2 ). Had dunlins used the presumed mortality-minimizing allocation, the total number of kills that would have been observed (assuming that the kill rate during OOF is 0) is estimated at 75.7, which is 63.3 (45.5%) fewer than observed. Even if we allow that OOF reduces mortality by just 50%, the mortality-minimizing allocation would result in 18.8 (13.5%) fewer deaths. These estimates are consistent with Wolf and Mangel’s (2007) model.

      OOF allocation and the mortality imposed by peregrines on wintering dunlins at Boundary Bay, British Columbia. Points represent the 6 one-hour intervals from 3h pre- to 3h post-high tide. The x-axis is the difference between the observed OOF allocation (proportion in each interval) and that which would minimize mortality, with negative numbers representing less time spent in OOF than is optimal. The relative risk (y-axis; from Table 2 ) is inversely proportional to this difference, high when too little time is spent in OOF, and low when too much time is spent in OOF.

      A general tactical consideration for predators is to match hunting effort to times and places at which prey are most active (Lang et al., 2019). The idea developed here is that predators could also hunt (or feign hunting) in order to shift some of their prey’s activity to times or places at which hunting is more successful. Such tactics may feature in many predator-prey interactions. Solitary predators with large territories such as pumas Felis concolor (Laundre, 2010) prowl about when or where captures are unlikely. The mere threat of their presence may be enough to require extra caution by prey, forcing them to take risks at other times or places. For example, Suselbeek et al. (2014) found that ocelots Leopardus pardalis are diurnally active even though captures of their main prey, tropical rainforest agoutis Dasyprocta punctata, are seldom made during daylight. Most agoutis are caught during twilight, before they enter the safety of their nocturnal burrows. However, the possible presence of ocelots during daytime requires agoutis to forage cautiously (i.e. more slowly), which results in the activity of some extending into twilight when ocelots have the advantage. Those with poor (low fruit availability) territories are forced to forage longest, enter their burrows latest, and are as a consequence at greatest risk. Predators in other systems may use analogous tactics. Prominent perching and soaring, unpredictable appearances, loud calls and odoriferous or otherwise salient signals that advertise a predator’s presence may all function as forms of ‘false attack’ that induce prey to alter behavior, possibly making them more vulnerable at other times or places.

      Data availability statement

      The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary materials. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

      Ethics statement

      The animal study was approved by Animal Care Committee, Simon Fraser University. The study was conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements.

      Author contributions

      RY: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. RS-L: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. SY: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing.

      Acknowledgments

      We dedicate this paper to Dick Dekker on his 90th birthday. At age 75 Dick successfully defended, in a memorable public event, a Ph.D. thesis submitted to Wageningen University entitled ‘Hunting by wild peregrines and other falcons’. The degree was hard-earned. His thesis, beautified by his own illustrations, was based on a lifetime of observation, much undertaken at great personal expense, meticulously and systematically recording data from each of thousands of field days. Armed with keen eyesight, vision and tenacity Dick became the world’s foremost watcher of wild falcons.

      Conflict of interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Publisher’s note

      All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

      References Baird R. W. Dill L. M. (1995). Occurrence and behaviour of transient killer whales: seasonal and pod-specific variability, foraging behaviour, and prey handling. Can. J. Zool. 73 (7), 13001311. doi: 10.1139/z95-154 Beauchamp G. (2016). Function and structure of vigilance in a gregarious species exposed to threats from predators and conspecifics. Anim. Behav. 116, 195–201. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.04.008 Beauchamp G. Ruxton G. D. (2012). Changes in anti-predator vigilance over time caused by a war of attrition between predator and prey. Behav. Ecol. 23, 368374. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arr194 Brennan L. A. Buchanan J. B. Herman S. G. Johnson T. M. (1985). Interhabitat movements of wintering dunlins in Western Washington. Murrelet 66, 1116. doi: 10.2307/3535459 Conklin J. R. Colwell M. A. (2007). Diurnal and nocturnal roost site fidelity of Dunlin (Calidris alpina pacifica) at Humboldt Bay, California. Auk 124 (2), 677689. doi: 10.1093/auk/124.2.677 Creel S. Becker M. Droge E. M’soka J. Matandiko W. Rosenblatt E. . (2019). What explains variation in the strength of behavioral responses to predation risk? A standardized test with large carnivore and ungulate guilds in three ecosystems. Biol. Conserv. 232, 164172. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2019.02.012 Cresswell W. (1996). Surprise as a winter hunting strategy in Sparrowhawks, Peregrines, and Merlins. Ibis 138, 684692. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-919X.1996.tb04770.x Dekker D. (1980). Hunting strategies of migrating Peregrines. Can. Field Nat. 94, 371382. Dekker D. (1998). Over-ocean flocking by Dunlins (Calidris alpina) and the effect of raptor predation at Boundary Bay, British Columbia. Can. Field-Nat. 112, 694697. Dekker D. (2009). Hunting tactics of Peregrines and other falcons. (Wageningen University and Research). Dekker D. (2013). High-tide flight by wintering Dunlin (Calidris alpina): a weather-dependent trade-off between energy loss and predation risk. Can. J. Zool. 91, 2529. doi: 10.1139/cjz-2012-0213 Dekker D. Out M. Tabak M. Ydenberg R. (2012). The effect of kleptoparasitic Bald Eagles and Gyrfalcons on the kill rate of Peregrine Falcons hunting Dunlins wintering in British Columbia. Condor 114, 290294. doi: 10.1525/cond.2012.110110 Dekker D. Ydenberg R. C. (2004). Raptor predation on wintering dunlins in relation to the tidal cycle. Condor 106, 415419. doi: 10.1093/condor/106.2.415 Dierschke V. (2003). Predation hazard during migratory stopover: are light or heavy birds under risk? J. Avian Biol. 34, 2429. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-048X.2003.03049.x Esselink P. Zwarts L. (1989). Seasonal trend in burrow depth and tidal variation in feeding activity of Nereis diversicolor . Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 56, 243254. doi: 10.3354/meps056243 Gaston A. J. Ydenberg R. C. Smith G. E. J. (2008). Ashmole’s halo and population regulation in seabirds. Mar. Ornithol. 35, 119126. Hedenström A. Alerstam T. (1996). Skylark optimal flight speeds for flying nowhere and somewhere. Beh Ecol. 7, 121126. doi: 10.1093/beheco/7.2.121 Hentze N. T. (2012). Characteristics of over-ocean flocking by Pacific dunlins (Calidris alpina pacifica) at Boundary Bay, British Columbia. (MSc) (Burnaby: Simon Fraser University). Houston A. I. McNamara J. M. (1999). Models of Adaptive Behaviour: An Approach Based on State (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press). Humphreys R. K. Ruxton G. D. (2020). Avian distraction displays: a review. Ibis (London, England), 162 (4), 1125–1145. doi: 10.1111/ibi.12814 Jiménez A. Elner R. W. Favaro C. Rickards K. Ydenberg R. C. (2015). Intertidal biofilm distribution underpins differential tide-following behavior of two sandpiper species (Calidris mauri and Calidris alpina) during northward migration. Estuar. Coast. Shelf Sci. 155, 816. doi: 10.1016/j.ecss.2014.12.038 Johnston-González R. Abril E. (2019). Predation risk and resource availability explain roost locations of Whimbrel Numenius phaeopus in a tropical mangrove delta. Ibis 161, 839853. doi: 10.1111/ibi.12678 Jukema J. Piersma T. (2006). Permanent female mimics in a lekking shorebird. Biol. Lett. 2, 161164. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2005.0416 Lang S. D. Mann R. P. Farine D. R. (2019). Temporal activity patterns of predators and prey across broad geographic scales. Beh Ecol. 30, 172180. doi: 10.1093/beheco/ary133 Lank D. B. Butler R. W. Ireland J. Ydenberg R. C. (2003). Effects of predation danger on migratory strategies of sandpipers. Oikos 103, 303319. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0706.2003.12314.x Lank D. B. Ydenberg R. C. (2003). Death and danger at migratory stopovers: problems with "predation risk". J. Avian Biol. 34, 225228. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-048X.2003.03250.x Laundre J. W. (2010). Behavioral response races, predator-prey shell games, ecology of fear, and patch use of pumas and their ungulate prey. Ecology 91, 29953007. doi: 10.1890/08-2345.1 McNamara J. M. Gasson C. E. Houston A. I. (1999). Incorporating rules for responding into evolutionary games. Nature 401, 368371. doi: 10.1038/43869 McNamara J. M. Houston A. I. (1990). The value of fat reserves and the tradeoff between starvation and predation. Acta Biotheor. 38, 3761. doi: 10.1007/BF00047272 Munn C. A. (1986). Birds that cry wolf. Nature 319, 143145. doi: 10.1038/319143a0 Out M. (2010). Escape from the Falcon's claws: how Dunlins manage timing of over-ocean flocking to lower predation risk. M.Sc. Thesis (Simon Fraser University). Page G. Whitacre D. F. (1975). Raptor predation on wintering shorebirds. Condor 77, 7383. doi: 10.2307/1366760 Ratcliffe D. A. (1962). Breeding density in the Peregrine Falco peregrinus and Raven Corvus corax . Ibis 104, 1339. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-919X.1962.tb08625.x Reurink F. Hentze N. Rourke J. Ydenberg R. (2016). Site-specific flight speeds of nonbreeding Pacific dunlins as a measure of the quality of a foraging habitat. Beh Ecol. 27, 803809. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arv223 Rogers D. I. Piersma T. Hassell C. J. (2006). Roost availability may constrain shorebird distribution: exploring the energetic costs of roosting and disturbance around a tropical bay. Biol. Conserv. 133, 225235. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2006.06.007 Rosa S. Encarnação A. L. Granadeiro J. P. Palmeirim J. M. (2006). High water roost selection by waders: maximizing feeding opportunities or avoiding predation? Ibis 148, 8897. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-919X.2006.00497.x Shepherd P. C. F. Lank D. B. (2004). Marine and agricultural habitat preferences of Dunlin wintering in British Columbia. J. Wildl Manage. 68, 6173. doi: 10.2193/0022-541X(2004)068[0061:MAAHPO]2.0.CO;2 Suselbeek L. Emsens W.-J. Hirsch B. T. Kays R. Rowcliffe J. M. Zamora-Gutierrez V. . (2014). Food acquisition and predator avoidance in a Neotropical rodent. Anim. Behav. 88, 4148. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.11.012 Wolf N. Mangel M. (2007). Strategy, compromise, and cheating in predator–prey games. Evol. Ecol. Res. 9, 12931304. Ydenberg R. C. Barrett J. Lank D. B. Xu C. Faber M. (2017). The redistribution of non-breeding dunlins in response to the post-DDT recovery of falcons. Oecologia 183, 11011110. doi: 10.1007/s00442-017-3835-2 Ydenberg R. C. Dekker D. Kaiser G. Shepherd P. C. F. Evans Ogden L. Rickards K. . (2010). Winter body mass and over-ocean flocking as components of danger management by Pacific dunlins. BMC Ecol. 10, 1. doi: 10.1186/1472-6785-10-1 Ydenberg R. C. Tavera E. A. Lank D. B. (2022). Danger, risk and anti-predator behavior in the life history of long-distance migratory sandpipers. J. Avian Biol. 53, e03002. doi: 10.1111/jav.03002 Yosef R. Gołdyn B. Zduniak P. (2011). Predation of migratory Little Stint (Calidris minuta) by Barbary Falcon (Falco pelegrinoides) is dependent on body mass and duration of stopover time. J. Ethol. 29, 257261. doi: 10.1007/s10164-010-0251-z
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016www.mdeykb.com.cn
      www.klmliw.com.cn
      www.lyliano.com.cn
      fcnfc.com.cn
      www.haowuz.com.cn
      sxchain.com.cn
      rqchain.com.cn
      mwbitx.com.cn
      www.slsxsw.com.cn
      www.wrchain.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p