Front. Behav. Neurosci. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience Front. Behav. Neurosci. 1662-5153 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.868473 Behavioral Neuroscience Review Inbred Mice Again at Stake: How the Cognitive Profile of the Wild-Type Mouse Background Discloses Pathogenic Effects of APP Mutations Ammassari-Teule Martine 1 2 * 1Laboratory of Psychobiology, Department of Experimental Neuroscience, Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy 2National Research Council, Institute of Biochemistry and Cell Biology, Rome, Italy

Edited by: Liana Fattore, CNR Neuroscience Institute (IN), Italy

Reviewed by: Kristen Onos, Jackson Laboratory, United States; Stacey Sukoff Rizzo, University of Pittsburgh, United States

*Correspondence: Martine Ammassari-Teule martine.teule@cnr.it

This article was submitted to Pathological Conditions, a section of the journal Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience

23 06 2022 2022 16 868473 02 02 2022 16 05 2022 Copyright © 2022 Ammassari-Teule. 2022 Ammassari-Teule

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Increasing efforts have been made in the last decades to increase the face validity of Alzheimer's disease (AD) mouse models. Main advancements have consisted in generating AD mutations closer to those identified in humans, enhancing genetic diversity of wild-type backgrounds, and choosing protocols much apt to reveal AD-like cognitive dysfunctions. Nevertheless, two aspects remain less considered: the cognitive specialization of inbred strains used as recipient backgrounds of mutations and the heuristic importance of studying destabilization of memory circuits in pre-symptomatic mice facing cognitive challenges. This article underscores the relevance of these behavioral/experimental aspects by reviewing data which show that (i) inbred mice differ in their innate predisposition to rely on episodic vs. procedural memory, which implicates differential sensitivity to mutations aimed at disrupting temporal lobe-dependent memory, and that (ii) investigating training-driven neural alterations in asymptomatic mutants unveils early synaptic damage, which considerably anticipates detection of AD first signs.

inbred mice cognitive challenges genetic background AD-related mutations cognitive profile Ministero della Salute10.13039/501100003196

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      The genetic bases of behavior have long been an exclusive matter of study for evolutionary biologists intended to verify the inheritance and conservation of behavioral traits across generations or species (Atchley and Fitch, 1993). The advent of behavioral genetics in the first decade of the 20th century represented the first attempt to estimate the weight of genomic variations in the expression of behavioral phenotypes (Dobzhansky, 1937; Fuller and Thompson, 1960). In this context, the utilization of tools specific to classical genetics in rodent populations brought to light three fundamental principles. First, complex behaviors are influenced by a very large number of genes, but the individual effect of each gene is very small. Second, complex behavioral traits cannot be classified into discrete categories but are continuously distributed in a way that approximates a normal (Gaussian) curve, with the majority of individuals around the central values of the distribution exhibiting similar phenotypes. Third, artificial selection methods (bidirectional selection or inbreeding) allow to generate subpopulations showing well-differentiated, sometimes opposing, phenotypes, whereas individuals in each subpopulation exhibit a remarkable phenotypic homogeneity. These observations highly attracted the attention of neuroscientists who identified a powerful tool in these methods to investigate variations in neural substrates underlying extreme, although normal, variations in behavior.

      Corollary to the demonstration that gene control behavior is the assumption that gene dysfunctions are pathogenic for behavior. In the late 20th century, molecular genetics made it possible to identify mutated genes in human patients showing a variety of diseases including those impacting cognition. The possibility to insert those genes in the mouse genome led to build up model organisms expected to recapitulate disease-specific neural and behavioral hallmarks. This objective was, however, partially achieved. Restricting our survey of the literature to data from Alzheimer's disease (AD) mouse models, it is apparent that the multiplicity of genomic manipulations aimed at overexpressing the three main human mutated proteins (APP, Tau, and presenilin (1) separately or jointly in likewise multiple wild-type (Wt) genetic backgrounds did not entirely reproduce, or even failed to model, the symptoms of human pathology. Hence, strategies to refine the face validity of AD models have been established, with a majority of those consisting in producing mutations closer to those observed in patients, and with little consideration of the cognitive profile of Wt mice, these mutations were expected to disrupt. Because neurodegenerative processes in AD patients start to develop in the temporal lobe, disrupt episodic and spatial memory but preserve motor-based procedural memory (Eldridge et al., 2002), the aim of this short review is to outline the importance of including (i) the cognitive specialization of inbred mouse strains used as recipient backgrounds and (ii) the choice of protocols anticipating detection of AD first signs, among the criteria to refine the face validity of AD mouse models.

      What Is an Inbred Mouse? Inbreeding

      Inbreeding consists of mating closely related individuals (sisters and brothers) taken from random bred populations for about 20 generations to produce a subpopulation whose members are homozygous, that is, have the same genotype. Inbred mice were initially generated by physiopathologists who identified the advantage of having individuals ruling out genetic variance and showing homogeneous traits to better circumscribe the nature and inheritance of several pathologies. For example, criteria of selection to start the production of inbred lines were “predisposition to develop neoplasia” to determine if cancer was inherited (Little, 1915), or “immunohistocompatibility response” to estimate the best immunological markers for tissue transplantation (Hellstrom, 1963). Incidentally, Bagg (1920) tested various inbred strains of mice in several multiple-choice mazes and found that the learning performance strongly varied between strains, whereas it was remarkably homogeneous within each strain. Later on, Vicari (1929) compared the time spent by DBA/2 and BALBc/ inbred mice, as well as by “Japanese Walzer” and “myencephalic bleb” mutant mice, to run a three-unit maze and observed that maze running times were strain-dependent. Some decades later, two seminal studies focusing on interstrain differences in learning and memory (Dennenberg, 1959; Bovet et al., 1969) have established the bases for behavioral genetics. Clearly, one advantage of inbreeding for neuroscientist interested in the genetic control of learning abilities is that no learning criterion is involved in the selection process. Thus, possible confounders like those evoked in the case of bidirectional selection where, for example, low and high learners in active avoidance might simply those having low and high pain thresholds or being less or more anxious (Río-Ȧlamos et al., 2015), were excluded. Another issue to be considered is that if inbred individuals are like homozygous twins, they all exhibit the phenotype of one single random bred individual. This means that their behavioral and neural traits are not distributed according to a normal curve, so an inbred population is in no way representative of a natural outbred population. As many strains have been accurately characterized, any neuroscientist interested in analyzing a particular behavioral or neural phenotype can select a priori either one strain expressing the behavioral or neural trait of interest, or several strains to be compared for their difference relating to this specific trait. Among the most commonly used strains, pure inbred (C57BL/6J, DBA/2J, BALB/c, FVB/NJ, 129SvEvTac) or mixed backgrounds (B6J/SJLJ) are predominant (Sultana et al., 2019). The DBA/2J mouse strain is often used to contrast the C57BL/6J strain, given that their genotype (Bottomly et al., 2011) and phenotype (Ingram and Corfman, 1980) are opposed in several aspects.

      Inbred Mice Show Task-Specific Learning and Memory Performance

      At the dawn of neuroscience, the most popular tasks performed to investigate learning in rodents were those designed by experimental animal behaviorists, which prevalently required to form motor habits or stimulus–response associations, and in which C57BL/6J mice (C57) were identified as poor learners. Specifically, C57 performed worse than BALBc, or DBA/2J (DBA), in the Lashley maze (Oliverio et al., 1972), the active avoidance (Bovet et al., 1969; Oliverio et al., 1972; Weinberger et al., 1992), and in situations of operant or instrumental conditioning (Renzi and Sansone, 1971) in which an elemental stimulus was used to initiate or stop responding. When O'Keefe and Nadel (1978) and Olton et al. (1978) identified the neural basis of spatial cognition in the hippocampal place cells, spatial tasks like the radial arm maze and the water maze were the golden standards to investigate cognitive functions and their alterations in rodents, especially in view of data showing that Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients with temporal lobe neurodegeneration were selectively impaired in episodic/declarative/spatial memory. We started testing C57 and DBA mice in a radial maze (Ammassari-Teule and Caprioli, 1985) and observed that C57 outperformed DBA, thereby reverting their previous status of bad learners. These findings were confirmed in other spatial protocols (Upchurch and Wehner, 1988, 1989; Passino et al., 2002) and extended to tasks which strongly rely on the hippocampus like reactivity to spatial novelty (Thinus-Blanc et al., 1996), contextual fear conditioning (Stiedl et al., 1999; Ammassari-Teule et al., 2000; Restivo et al., 2002), cross-maze place learning (Middei et al., 2004), and, very recently, pattern separation (Dickson and Mittleman, 2022). The C57 predisposition to do well in hippocampus-dependent tasks then prompted several groups to examine the structural and functional properties of their hippocampus. Compared to other strains, C57 were found to show a higher density of mossy fibers in the region inferior to the hippocampus (Barber et al., 1974; Lipp et al., 1988; Schwegler et al., 1988), an increased activity of hippocampal, but not cortical, protein kinase C (Wehner et al., 1990), and long-term stronger hippocampal potentiation (Matsuyama et al., 1997; Nguyen et al., 2000; Gerlai, 2001a; Jones et al., 2001).

      Considering strain-specific levels of performance, it became rapidly evident that a majority of tasks in which C57 performed poorly were those in which DBA performed well, and vice versa. Beyond their aforementioned superiority in the Lashley maze and active avoidance, DBA mice were found to score better than C57 in cue-based fear conditioning (Paylor et al., 1994; Ammassari-Teule et al., 2000) and ethanol-induced conditioned place preference (Cunningham and Shields, 2018). On the one hand, these findings pointed out the remarkable ability of DBA to detect relevant elemental sensory stimuli to rapidly form stimulus–response associations in pavlovian or instrumental conditioning paradigms, or to implement egocentric orientation in spatial tasks. On the other hand, their inability to form configural representations, either of the aversive context in conditioning tasks or of distal environmental cues for allocentric orientation in spatial tasks, was interpreted as the consequence of the poorly functional morphological, biochemical, and plastic properties of their hippocampus, which led to consider them as a natural model of hippocampal dysfunction (Paylor et al., 1993). Supporting this view, DBA mice behave randomly in the hippocampus-dependent simultaneous olfactory discrimination task and do not form training-induced dendritic spines in the hippocampus (Restivo et al., 2006).

      To Have, or Not to Have, an Outstandingly Functional Hippocampus: Advantages and Limitations in Relation to Different Memory Systems

      The notion of memory systems arises from observations initially carried out in human subjects (Cohen and Squire, 1980) and later in rodents (Packard et al., 1989; Packard and White, 1991; McDonald and White, 1993) that different types of memory are governed by dissociable brain substrates. For example, episodic, declarative, spatial, or context-based memory are supported by temporal lobe regions, among which the hippocampus plays a central role, whereas stimulus–response associations and procedural memory, including motor habits, are overall controlled by the striatum. The independence of memory systems was demonstrated by data showing that disrupting the neural support of one system leaves unaltered the operations supported by the other system or even improves the preserved system by suppressing conflictual responding (McDonald and White, 1995).

      In a majority of individuals, memory systems can be activated separately, concurrently, or sequentially, depending on the situation to copy with. For example, in the plus maze task (Packard and McGaugh, 1996), rodents were first trained to turn left to find a food reward in the unique baited arm and then exposed to probe trials in which they were released from the opposite starting arm. In those trials, they could either reproduce the motor response reinforced during training (turning left) and do not go to the baited arm or invert it (turning right) and go to the baited arm. Interestingly, the rats were found to turn right after a short training duration (1 week), consistent with hippocampus-based place learning, but to turn left after long training duration (3 weeks), consistent with striatum-based response learning. Interestingly, inactivation of the hippocampus after short training made the rats show striatum-based motor learning, whereas inactivation of the dorsolateral striatum after long training made them show hippocampal-dependent place learning. The point is that when C57 and DBA were trained in the same plus maze task, C57 showed place learning and predominant hippocampal activation after both short and long training, whereas DBA never relied on a particular system even they prevalently activated the dorsolateral striatum (Passino et al., 2002). Indeed, the consequence of the C57 inability to disengage the hippocampus is that inactivation of this region at any probe trial indeed disrupts place learning but does not promote response learning. Similarly, the consequence of the DBA/2 inability to rely on a particular system is that the inactivation of any region at any probe trial does not promote neither place nor response learning (Middei et al., 2004). Thus, contrary to what happens in rats, disrupting the neural substrate of one memory system but does not promote the utilization of another system in these genotypes. Indeed, the C57 propensity to engage the hippocampus in any situation they face is also observed in fear conditioning (FC) paradigms. Specifically, studies dissecting the neural bases of FC in outbred populations of rats have identified (i) tone fear conditioning (TFC) as an elemental associative learning system involving the basolateral amygdala (BLA) but not the dorsal hippocampus (Phillips and LeDoux, 1992; Paré et al., 2004) and (ii) contextual fear conditioning (CFC) as a configural learning system involving by both regions (Selden et al., 1991; Phillips and LeDoux, 1992). Different from that observed in these populations, C57 mice were found to concurrently activate the BLA and the dorsal hippocampus in both CTC and CFC, although they showed considerably less freezing in TFC than in CFC (Pignataro et al., 2013). In line with the view that recruiting the hippocampus in TFC is an obstacle to implement elemental stimulus–response associations, lesions to the dorsal hippocampus were found to enhance C57 TFC performance (Ammassari-Teule et al., 2002).

      Modifications of Experimental Parameters or Rearing Conditions Can Abolish Interstrain Differences in Learning and Memory

      The aforementioned experiments show that C57 predominantly form configural environmental representations in which elemental stimuli are embedded in and need to be disentangled to predict reinforcement and guide behavior. Thus, any manipulation of experimental factors that facilitates disentangling is expected to enhance cue-based performance in this mouse strain. This possibility was demonstrated in a study in which C57 and seven other mouse strains including DBA were trained to press a lever upon presentation of an elemental stimulus (tone or light) to avoid delivery of an electric footshock. DBA showed superior avoidance performance when the tone or the light was of short duration. However, a gradual increase in the stimulus duration was found to progressively abolish interstrain differences, thereby suggesting that C57 benefited for longer cue presentation to disentangle them from the context (Renzi and Sansone, 1971). Interestingly, Cunningham and Shields (2018) explored more recently the possibility that the most robust ethanol-induced conditioned place preference (CCP) shown by DBA compared to C57 might depend on strain differences in sensitivity to contextual cues. They found that compared to single cueing, multiple cueing increased CCP in both strains but that this effect disappeared more rapidly in DBA and was not sufficient to elevate the CCP performance of C57 to the level of DBA. They therefore concluded that CPP differences were due to a genotype-specific sensitivity to ethanol reward. Nevertheless, the fact that DBA mice outperform in amygdala-dependent conditioned taste aversion, that is, a task based on the association of a single gustatory stimulus with illness (Dudek and Fuller, 1978; Risinger and Cunningham, 1995), suggests a more global interpretation that DBA perform better due to the stronger BLA modulation of striatal-based elemental cue processing (Desmedt et al., 1998; Goode et al., 2016). Other examples which show that interstrain differences in fear conditioning vary or persist depending on whether freezing is recorded shortly or long after training (Nie and Abel, 2001; Balogh and Wehner, 2003) indicate that if the predominance of a memory system determines the nature of information each strain preferentially relies on, the time necessary to consolidate this information is a much important variable in the modulation of the behavioral responses. Indeed, manipulations like environmental enrichment (EE) or physical exercise modify cognitive abilities, attention, and anxiety in inbred mice but paradoxically exert improving or disrupting effects depending on the duration of and the age mice were exposed to these manipulations (Singhal et al., 2019). Among the principally observed strain-specific effects, EE was found to increase attention in C57 (van de Weerd et al., 2004), to either decrease (Chapillon et al., 1999) or increase (van de Weerd et al., 2004) anxiety in BALB/c, to reduce reactivity to novelty in both C57 and DBA (Dickson and Mittleman, 2021), and to accentuate C57 vs 129S6/SvEV differences in locomotor activity, anxiety, and social interactions (Abramov et al., 2008). Thus, no univocal, strain-independent beneficial effect of EE of behavior was observed.

      Alzheimer-Associated Mutations and Genotype of the Background Mouse

      From the creation of the first transgenic mice, it clearly appeared that controlling the characteristics of the recipient strain was crucial to reveal or maintain expected transgene effects because inserting a mutated gene in different mouse strains was found to produce variable phenotypes and because it was observed in some cases that a phenotype was progressively losing its specificity due to non-specific mutations or uncontrolled environmental effects, thereby preventing data reproducibility. Multiple research groups with an expertise in mouse behavior genetics (Crusio, 1996; Gerlai, 1996, 2001b; Wehner and Silva, 1996; Crawley et al., 1997; Wolfer and Lipp, 2000; Lassalle et al., 2008) identified such risks and proposed several solutions.

      The Same Mutated Gene in Different Backgrounds Produces Different Phenotypes First-Generation Models: Mutant HAPP Overexpression

      One of the first transgenic murine models of Alzheimer's disease is the Tg2576 mouse developed by Hsiao et al. (1996) which overexpresses human APP (isoform 695) containing the double mutation K670N, M671L (Swedish mutation) under the control of the hamster prion protein promoter. This hemizygous mutation was originally introduced in a C57(B6) × SJL F1 hybrid background and stabilized by repeatedly backcrossing mutant mice with B6 × SJL F1 hybrids. The main issue addressed at that time was confounding effects due to the insertion of the mutated gene and those due to the transgene per se. It was therefore proposed to backcross the transgenic lines to one or even more inbred strains for at least three generations before performing phenotypic characterization in F1 hybrids. This strategy, however, revealed to be inadequate as repeated backcrossing to inbred lines produced non-specific performance impairments, reduced fertility, and, in some cases (FVB/N or B6), was found to be lethal. This prompted Lassalle et al. (2008) to insert the HuAPP695-SWE transgene in three different backgrounds (homogeneous: C57; heterogeneous: CBAJ; and hybrid: B6J/SJLJ) and to perform phenotyping in F1 generations after only one generation backcrossing. Comparisons included evaluation of anxiety in the elevated plus maze, spatial learning in the water maze, and fear conditioning. The results showed that Tg (+) C57 mice were globally more active and less anxious than their Tg (-) counterparts, as well as than Tg (+) and Tg (-) in other backgrounds. The calculation of a spatial index in the water maze revealed that even though this index was rather comparable between Tg (-) B6/SJL, C57, and CBA, the genetic background significantly modulated the expression of the transgene, with the lower spatial index being found when the mutation was expressed in the C57 background. Fear conditioning data did not provide evidence of differences in CFC performance between the three Tg (+) and Tg (-) backgrounds possibly because the experiments were carried out in 17-month-old female mice and, hence, recruited additive effects of sex and age to those of background and mutation. Nevertheless, this study provided the first demonstration that the behavioral characteristic of the recipient mouse was defining the degree of mutation-induced cognitive impairment. After two years, Rustay et al. (2010) compared the effect of the Swe mutation in the B6/SJL and 129 backgrounds and reported more deleterious effects at late ages in the 129 backgrounds for parameters that were not properly cognitive (locomotor activity, spontaneous alternation) and for which Tg (-) 129 were scoring lower than Tg (-) B6/SJL. Interestingly, models in which three (APP, PS1, and tau) or five (two APP and three PS1) familiar AD mutations were concurrently inserted in mouse genomes were predominantly developed in a C57 background (Sterniczuk et al., 2010; Forner et al., 2021; Sil et al., 2022) or hybrid backgrounds with a B6 component. Confirming the inadequacy of the DBA background for AD mutations, insertion of the APPswe and PSEN1de9 mutated genes in DBA exacerbated lethal seizures and even lessened amyloid deposition (Jackson et al., 2015).

      Second- and Third-Generation Models: App Gene KI Mutations

      These models were generated using the App gene knock-in strategy to overproduce pathogenic Aβ without overexpressing APP with the objective to avoid artifacts due to APP overexpression per se. Murine Aβ sequences (Swedish, Beyreuther/Iberian, artic) were humanized by changing amino acids that differ between mice and humans and then introduced separately or concurrently to generate APPNL, APPNL−F, or APPNL−G−F mice expressing Wt human Aβ under control of the mouse App locus (Nilsson et al., 2014; Saito et al., 2014). The point is that if humanized Aβ does increase the face validity of these models as far as amyloidosis is concerned, the consequences of App-KI manipulation on neural and behavioral parameters do not differ much from those observed in the first-generation transgenic models. App-KI mice show the same increased glutamate release probability and intense astrocytosis/gliosis around the Aβ plaques, discordant results regarding hippocampal LTP (decreased or intact), and late memory impairments (Nilsson et al., 2014; Baglietto-Vargas et al., 2021; Benitez et al., 2021). Furthermore, the fact that robust amyloidosis and its metabolic consequences took at least 18 months to emerge has led to the development of third-generation models, that is, double KI mutants obtained by crossing APPNL−G−F mice with mice bearing PS1-KI (Sato et al., 2021) to accelerate the detection of mutation effects. In these studies, however, (i) the genetic background (until now C57) is never mentioned in the method section of articles, suggesting that it is per se irrelevant if the mutant mouse is viable; (ii) the focus is predominantly placed on the pathogenic inflammatory/metabolic cell alterations engendered by multiple Aβ species and on the possibility to rescue them by rectifying the mutated genomic sequences; (iii) mutation effects are investigated separately at the neural (Jun et al., 2020) or behavioral (Sakakibara et al., 2019; Sutoko et al., 2021) levels and therefore do not inform on the status of neural circuits when mice face cognitive challenges, that is, when these circuits actually come into play.

      Systems Neuroscience Approaches

      Memory formation requires changes in neuronal network connectivity mediated by modifications in the strength and number of synapses. Since the discovery that synapses are primary targets of Aβ oligomers (Selkoe, 2002), central to the validation, an AD mouse model is the demonstration that deficits in hippocampal-dependent memory associate with hippocampal synaptic dysfunctions. A survey of the literature indicates that dysfunctions including dendritic spine loss or long-term potentiation (LTP) deficit have been identified in the majority of AD mouse models but mostly under naive conditions. Increasing evidence reveals, however, that studying structural, functional, and molecular alterations which develop in the hippocampus when animals are given memory tasks anticipates detection of synaptic failure and unveils pathogenic or compensatory reorganization of brain circuits, which might otherwise not be observed in naive conditions.

      Training Discloses Neural Alterations in APP Mutants

      Heterozygous B6-Tg/Thy1APP23Sdz (APP23) mice show amyloid plaques in the hippocampus (Sturchler-Pierrat and Staufenbiel, 2000) and severe deficits in hippocampal-dependent tasks (Lalonde et al., 2002; Vloeberghs et al., 2006) around 12 months of age. When trained in a water maze at the age of 7 months, they swim regularly but show increased latencies and travel a longer distance to find the submerged platform than Wt C57 controls. Nevertheless, the fact that both groups reach the same level of performance at the end of training indicates that mutant mice exhibit more delay in learning than mice with an incapability to learn. Following training, mice were euthanized to evaluate the effect of the learning experience on dendritic spines and synaptic activity. Spine density measured on CA1 neuron dendrites in non-training and pseudo-training conditions was not found to vary between mutant and Wt mice. Differently, more spines were counted post-training in the mutant mice, thereby indicating that circuits unaltered at rest undergo stronger learning-induced remodeling. Indexes of basal synaptic transmission like input–output curves and paired-pulse facilitation were indistinguishable between genotypes in all experimental conditions, but CA3–CA1 long-term potentiation decayed more rapidly in the mutant mice (Middei et al., 2010). Together, these findings allow the following conclusions to be made. The observation that mutant mice perform same as the Wt mice at the end of training suggests that formation of novel synapses might compensate for the rapid decay of synaptic plasticity.

      Synaptic Compensatory Mechanisms in 2-Month-Old 3xTg-AD Mice

      The 6–8-week-old 3xTg-AD mice exhibit intact synaptic plasticity at rest. Nevertheless, differently from Wt mice, they show increased synaptic depression when their synaptic homeostasis is altered by suppression of ryanodine receptor (RyR)-evoked calcium signaling. The authors hypothesize that in baseline conditions, 3xTg-AD mice exhibit increased activity of this receptor which, by augmenting RyR-evoked calcium release, blocks the predisposition of mutant synapses to exhibit long-term depression (Chakroborty et al., 2012). The authors successively demonstrate that compensatory maintenance of synaptic plasticity is mediated by an augmentation of nitric oxide levels, a presynaptic regulator of calcium release which increases glutamatergic transmission (Chakroborty et al., 2015). Although this study does not examine the functional consequences of Ry-R manipulations in vivo, these data support the relevance of detecting compensatory synaptic changes in presymptomatic AD mice to be targeted by pharmacological approaches aimed to prolong them over time.

      Training Experience Reveals Neural Compensation in Pre-Symptomatic APP Mutants

      Tg2576 mice and their Wt C57 controls trained for CFC at the age of 2 months show the same reactivity to footshocks and exhibit immediate c-fos activation in the dorsal CA1 region of the hippocampus and the basolateral region of the amygdala (BLA). When returned 24 h later to the safe training context, all mice show intense freezing, but differently from Wt mice, mutant mice do not exhibit any sign of c-fos activation or dendritic spine remodeling in CA1, instead they show c-fos overactivation and dendritic spine remodeling in BLA, in line with the view that the latter region compensates for hippocampus failure and sustains their intact CFC performance. Examination of Aβ levels 24 h after CFC in the mutant mice non-returned to the conditioning cage indicates a selective increase in Aβ42 oligomers in CA1 but not BLA. This is shown by Western blot analyses using the amino-terminal specific anti-Aβ42 antibody AD54D2 and the carboxy-terminal specific anti-Aβ42 antibody (clone 295F2), as well as by immunofluorescent detection of Aβ using the D54D2 and the carboxy-terminal specific antibody 12F4. In the Wt mice, the Aβ42 signal is about undetectable in both regions at rest, and no rise is observed following CFC. Thus, CFC learning triggers immediate release of Aβ species in the hippocampus of cognitively asymptomatic Tg2576 mutants (Pignataro et al., 2019). Validation of a causal link between the CFC-induced Aβ rise and absence of hippocampus activation/remodeling in cognitively asymptomatic Tg2576 mice comes from data which show that CFC-trained mutant mice receiving intra-hippocampus injections of DAPT, a gamma secretase inhibitor which reduced Aβ levels, show regular formation of hippocampal spines with no longer compensatory formation of spines the BLA. Therefore, this is the first study to provide evidence of neural compensation consisting of enhanced synaptic activity in brain regions spared by Aβ load. Furthermore, it unravels an activity-mediated mechanism by which neuronal activation produced during CFC encoding triggers Aβ oligomerization in the hippocampus and prevents synaptic rearrangements in this region. Indeed, the observation that learning activates compensatory circuits allowing mutant mice to maintain an intact memory delineates entirely novel therapeutic avenues in the AD field. Considering that “compensatory circuits” recruit regions unaffected by Aβ load, their stimulation might be more beneficial to prolong cognitive efficiency than stimulation of disrupted “canonical circuits.”

      Conclusion Is There an Ideal Background for Overexpressing Mutant APP?

      Although it may appear trivial, the first requirement for the recipient background of an APP mutation is to exhibit sufficiently elevated episodic memory capacities likely to be significantly altered by the mutation. At the first sight, B6 mice appear appropriate given their optimal episodic memory scores, even though their outstandingly functional hippocampus is a priori not representative of the natural genetic heterogeneity of AD patients and even represents an obstacle to the natural evolution toward procedural memory (Passino et al., 2002) which becomes rapidly predominant in AD patients (Eldridge et al., 2002) and AD rat models (Ammassari-Teule et al., 2002). This limitation, however, is not fully overcome by inserting mutations in mixed backgrounds. For example, expression of APPswe in B6/SJL mice generates five mutant phenotypes (black, white-belly agouti, albino, tan with pink eyes, and silver with pink eyes), with the three later ones showing poor contextual memory due to vision problem. These observations highlight the importance of controlling the sensory phenotype of strains and substrains of mice, which can result in the loss of function (deafness, Zheng et al., 1999; blindness, Brown and Wong, 2007), as well as a gain of function (resistance to noise-induced hearing loss, Street et al., 2014; enhancement of olfactory conditioning in mice with vision defects, Brown and Wong, 2007).

      The second requirement is to choose a strain showing a cognitive, even mild, deficit at a sufficient early age to have the performance of the wild-type counterpart unaffected by aging. Apparently, C57 mice align again with this criterion. For example, Tg2576 in a C57 background shows a CFC deficit associated with a decrease in hippocampal spines (D'Amelio et al., 2011) and the presence of Aβ oligomers already at 3 months of age (Pignataro et al., 2019), whereas the Tg2576 mutation in a B6/SJL background is still considered as a late AD model.

      In addition, these observations raise another equally important question, namely, the choice of experimental protocols allowing to anticipate neural dysfunctions at a stage where no, or mild, cognitive impairment is observed to start therapies when maximal effectiveness can be expected.

      Alternative Strategies: Incorporating Genetic Diversity Into Mouse Models of AD

      The fact that inbred mouse strains do not reflect the phenotypic variability observed in natural populations limits a priori the face validity of AD models. One strategy alternative to the insertion of human-like mutations in a single genome is increasing the genetic diversity of recipient mouse backgrounds to generate well-differentiated phenotypes carrying the same mutation. For example, Onos et al. (2019) B6 mice expressing APPswe and PS1de9 (APP/PS1) transgenes were backcrossed for six generations with three wild-derived strains (CAST/EiJ, WSB/EiJ, and PWK/PhJ). As expected, they obtained large phenotypic differences between mice substrains as far as cognitive ability, neurodegeneration, plaque load, cerebrovascular health, and cerebral amyloid angiopathy were concerned. Transcriptional analyses revealed, however, that the “strain” factor was the largest source of variation suggesting a potential risk of this breeding method, that is, the possibility that wild-type genomes include AD risk genes. For example, crossing B6 males with deletion of Cacna1c and Tcf7l2 genes associated with multiple psychiatric diseases with wild-type females from 30 inbred laboratory strains resulted in highly variable, sometimes opposing, effects (Sittig et al., 2016). Thus, if the translatability of data obtained by studying mutations in one single inbred genotype is limited, highly diversified backgrounds require careful genotypic/phenotypic characterization prior to insertion of mutations. With the same objective of differentiating AD phenotypes, the utilization of recombinant inbred (RI) strains overcame issues relating to unknown wild-type-derived genomes since they were established to detect gene segregation and linkage and for identifying associations between behavior and quantitative trait loci (QTL) accounting for variations in behavior (Plomin et al., 1994). RI strains were obtained by crossing two inbred parental strains (e.g., B6JxDJ) giving raise to F1 or F2 generations maintained under a strict inbreeding regimen. After at least 20 generations, individuals in each RI lines were found to be genetically homogeneous, whereas families of RI lines exhibited genetic diversity. Mapping of quantitative trait loci with small or very large effects allowed to build up genetic reference panels providing databases on genotypes x phenotype interactions (Peirce et al., 2004). Taking advantage of these databases, Neuner et al. (2019) created the AD-BXD panel of transgenic mouse strains. This panel was established by crossing 5xFAD female mice to male mice from the BJxDJ genetic reference panel until 27 F1 AD-BxD strains were generated. The 5xFAD mutation being hemizygous, half of mice carried no transgene and served as isogenic control for mutations. This method considerably extended AD phenotypes for traits including “age onset of symptoms” and “acceleration of memory decline” but also allowed to identify variations in genes regulation. Specifically, the expression of genes controlling neural activity, structure, and function was decreased, whereas the expression of immune response genes was increased. This approach appears undeniably insightful for personalized medicine if followed by systems neuroscience analyses estimating structure–function relationships in the subset of strains of interest for the trait under examination (e.g., early vs. late AD onset, abrupt vs. progressive emergence of cognitive alterations).

      Author Contributions

      The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

      Funding

      This work was funded by the Italian Ministry of Health (5XMille–2018).

      Conflict of Interest

      The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Publisher's Note

      All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

      References Abramov U. Puussaar T. Raud S. Kurrikoff K. Vasar E. (2008). Behavioural differences between C57BL/6 and 129S6/SvEv strains are reinforced by environmental enrichment. Neurosci. Lett. 443, 223227. 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.07.07518687379 Ammassari-Teule M. Caprioli A. (1985). Spatial learning and memory, maze running strategies and cholinergic mechanisms in two inbred strains of mice. Behav. Brain Res. 17, 916. 10.1016/0166-4328(85)90003-84041225 Ammassari-Teule M. Middei S. Passino E. Restivo L. (2002). Enhanced procedural learning following beta-amyloid protein (1-42) infusion in the rat. NeuroReport 13, 16791682. 10.1097/00001756-200209160-0002212352626 Ammassari-Teule M. Passino E. Restivo L. de Marsanich B. (2000). Fear conditioning in C57/BL/6 and DBA/2 mice: variability in nucleus accumbens function according to the strain predisposition to show contextual- or cue-based responding. Eur. J. Neurosci. 12, 44674474. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.01333.x11122357 Atchley W. R. Fitch W. (1993). Genetic affinities of inbred mouse strains of uncertain origin. Mol. Biol. Evol. 10, 11501169.8277849 Bagg H. J.. (1920). “Individual differences and family resemblances in animal behavior,” in Archives of Psychology N°43, eds. R. S. Woodworth (New-York, NY; The Science Press).11494642 Baglietto-Vargas D. Forner S. Cai L. Martini A. C. Trujillo-Estrada L. Swarup V. . (2021). Generation of a humanized Abeta expressing mouse demonstrating aspects of Alzheimer's disease-like pathology. Nat. Commun. 12, 2421. 10.1038/s41467-021-22624-z33893290 Balogh S. A. Wehner J. M. (2003). Inbred mouse strain differences in the establishment of long-term fear memory. Behav. Brain Res. 140, 97106. 10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00279-612644283 Barber R. P. Vaughn J. Wimer R. E. Wimer C. C. (1974). Genetically associated variations in the distribution of dentate granule cell synapses upon the pyramidal cell dendrites in mouse hippocampus. J. Comp. Neurol. 156, 417434. 10.1002/cne.9015604044137683 Benitez D. P. Jiang S. Wood J. Wang R. Hall C. M. Peerboom C. . (2021). Knock-in models related to Alzheimer's disease: synaptic transmission, plaques and the role of microglia. Mol. Neurodegener. 16, 47. 10.1186/s13024-021-00457-034266459 Bottomly D. Walter N. A. Hunter J. E. Darakjian P. Kawane S. Buck K. J. . (2011). Evaluating gene expression in C57BL/6J and DBA/2J mouse striatum using RNA-Seq and microarrays. PLoS ONE 6, e17820. 10.1371/journal.pone.001782021455293 Bovet D. Bovet-Nitti F. Oliverio A. (1969). Genetic aspects of learning and memory in mice. Science 163, 139149. 10.1126/science.163.3863.1395762759 Brown R. E. Wong A. A. (2007). The influence of visual ability on learning and memory performance in 13 strains of mice. Learn Mem. 14, 134144. 10.1101/lm.47390717351136 Chakroborty S. Kim J. Schneider C. West A. R. Stutzmann G. E. (2015). Nitric oxide signaling is recruited as a compensatory mechanism for sustaining synaptic plasticity in Alzheimer's disease mice. J. Neurosci. 35, 68936902. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4002-14.201525926464 Chakroborty S. Kim J. Schneider C Jacobson C Molg,ó J. Stutzmann G. E. (2012). Early presynaptic and postsynaptic calcium signaling abnormalities mask underlying synaptic depression in presymptomatic Alzheimer's disease mice. J. Neurosci. 32, 83418353. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0936-12.201222699914 Chapillon P. Manneché C Belzung C. Caston J. (1999). Rearing environmental enrichment in two inbred strains of mice: 1. Effects on emotional reactivity. Behav. Genet. 29, 4146. 10.1023/A:102143790591310371757 Cohen N. J. Squire L. R. (1980). Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science 210, 207210. 10.1126/science.74143317414331 Crawley J. N. Belknap J. K. Collins A. Crabbe J. C. Frankel W. Henderson N. . (1997). Behavioral phenotypes of inbred mouse strains: implications and recommendations for molecular studies. Psychopharmacology 132, 107124. 10.1007/s0021300503279266608 Crusio W. E.. (1996). Gene-targeting studies: new methods, old problems. Trends Neurosci. 19, 186187. 10.1016/S0166-2236(96)20023-28723203 Cunningham C. L. Shields C. N. (2018). Effects of multi-modal cues on conditioned place preference in C57BL/6J and DBA/2J mice. Psychopharmacology 235, 35353543. 10.1007/s00213-018-5078-230343365 D'Amelio M. Cavallucci V. Middei S. Marchetti C. Pacioni S. Ferri A. . (2011). Caspase-3 triggers early synaptic dysfunction in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 6976. 10.1038/nn.270921151119 Dennenberg V. H.. (1959). Learning differences in two separated lines of mice. Science 130, 451452. 10.1126/science.130.3373.45113675770 Desmedt A. Garcia R. Jaffard R. (1998). Differential modulation of changes in hippocampal-septal synaptic excitability by the amygdala as a function of either elemental or contextual fear conditioning in mice. J. Neurosci. 18, 480487. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-01-00480.19989412524 Dickson P. E. Mittleman G. (2021). Environmental enrichment influences novelty reactivity, novelty preference, and anxiety via distinct genetic mechanisms in C57BL/6J and DBA/2J mice. Sci. Rep. 11, 3928. 10.1038/s41598-021-83574-633594184 Dickson P. E. Mittleman G. (2022). Working memory and pattern separation in founder strains of the BXD recombinant inbred mouse panel. Sci. Rep. 12, 69. 10.1038/s41598-021-03850-334996965 Dobzhansky T.. (1937). Genetics and the Origin of Species. New York, NY; Columbia University Press. Dudek B. C. Fuller J. L. (1978). Task-dependent genetic influences on behavioral response of mice (Mus musculus) to acetaldehyde. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 92, 749758. 10.1037/h0077506690294 Eldridge L. L. Masterman D. Knowlton B. J. (2002). Intact implicit habit learning in Alzheimer's disease. Behav. Neurosci. 116, 722726. 10.1037/0735-7044.116.4.72212148939 Forner S. Kawauchi S. Balderrama-Gutierrez G. Kramár E. A. Matheos D. P. Phan J. . (2021). Systematic phenotyping and characterization of the 5xFAD mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Sci. Data. 8, 270. 10.1038/s41597-021-01054-y34654824 Fuller J. L. Thompson W. R. (1960). Behavior Genetics. New York, NY: John Wiley and Sons. Gerlai R.. (1996). Gene-targeting studies of mammalian behavior: is it the mutation or the background genotype? Trends Neurosci. 19, 177181. 10.1016/S0166-2236(96)20020-78723200 Gerlai R.. (2001a). LTP: variation between inbred mouse strains. Trends Neurosci. 24, 75. 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01750-111164925 Gerlai R.. (2001b). Gene targeting: technical confounds and potential solutions in behavioral brain research. Behav. Brain. Res. 125, 1321. 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00282-011682088 Goode T. D. Leong K. C. Goodman J. Maren S. Packard M. G. (2016). Enhancement of striatum-dependent memory by conditioned fear is mediated by beta-adrenergic receptors in the basolateral amygdala. Neurobiol. Stress 3, 7482. 10.1016/j.ynstr.2016.02.00427981180 Hellstrom K. E.. (1963). Differential behavior of transplanted mouse lymphoma lines in genetically compatible homozygous and F1 hybrid mice. Nature 199, 614. 10.1038/199614a014074545 Hsiao K. Chapman P. Nilsen S. Eckman C. Harigaya Y. Younkin S. . (1996). Correlative memory deficits, Abeta elevation, and amyloid plaques in transgenic mice. Science. 274, 99102. 10.1126/science.274.5284.998810256 Ingram D. K. Corfman T. P. (1980). An overview of neurobiological comparisons in mouse strains. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 4, 421435. 10.1016/0149-7634(80)90032-96110196 Jackson H. M. Onos K. D. Pepper K. W. Graham L. C. Akeson E. C. Byers C. . (2015). DBA/2J genetic background exacerbates spontaneous lethal seizures but lessens amyloid deposition in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. PLoS ONE 10, e0125897. 10.1371/journal.pone.012589725933409 Jones M. W. Peckham H. M. Errington M. L. Bliss T. V. Routtenberg A. (2001). Synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus of awake C57BL/6 and DBA/2 mice: interstrain differences and parallels with behavior. Hippocampus 11, 391396. 10.1002/hipo.105311530843 Jun H. Bramian A. Soma S. Saito T. Saido T. C. Igarashi K. M. (2020). Disrupted place cell remapping and impaired grid cells in a knockin model of Alzheimer's disease. Neuron. 107, 10951112.e6. 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.06.02332697942 Lalonde R. Dumont M. Staufenbiel M. Sturchler-Pierrat C. Strazielle C. (2002). Spatial learning, exploration, anxiety, and motor coordination in female APP23 transgenic mice with the Swedish mutation. Brain Res. 956, 3644. 10.1016/S0006-8993(02)03476-512426044 Lassalle J. M. Halley H. Daumas S. Verret L. Francés B. (2008). Effects of the genetic background on cognitive performances of Tg2576 mice. Behav. Brain Res. 191, 104110. 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.03.01718433892 Lipp H. P. Schwegler H. Heimrich B. Driscol P. (1988). lnfrapyramidal mossy fibers and two-way avoidance learning: developmental modification of hippocampal circuitry and adult behavior of rats and mice. J. Neurosci. 8, 19051921. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-06-01905.19883385481 Little C. C.. (1915). The inheritance of cancer. Science 42, 494495. 10.1126/science.42.1084.49417770909 Matsuyama S. Namgung U. Routtenberg A. (1997). Long-term potentiation persistence greater in C57BL/6 than DBA/2 mice: predicted on basis of protein kinase C levels and learning performance. Brain Res. 763, 127130. 10.1016/S0006-8993(97)00444-79272837 McDonald R. J. White N. M. (1993). A triple dissociation of memory systems: hippocampus, amygdala, and dorsal striatum. Behav. Neurosci. 107, 322. 10.1037/0735-7044.107.1.324341708 McDonald R. J. White N. M. (1995). Information acquired by the hippocampus interferes with acquisition of the amygdala-based conditioned-cue preference in the rat. Hippocampus 5, 189197. 10.1002/hipo.4500503057550614 Middei S. Restivo L. Sgobio C. Passino E. Ammassari-Teule M. (2004). Reversible inactivation of hippocampus and dorsolateral striatum in C57BL/6 and DBA/2 inbred mice failed to show interaction between memory systems in these genotypes. Behav. Brain Res. 154, 527534. 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.03.01815313042 Middei S. Roberto A. Berretta N. Panico M. B. Lista S. Bernardi G. . (2010). Learning discloses abnormal structural and functional plasticity at hippocampal synapses in the APP23 mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Learn Me. 17, 236240. 10.1101/lm.174831020404004 Neuner S. M. Heuer S. E. Huentelman M. J. O'Connell K. M. S. Kaczorowski C. C. (2019). Harnessing genetic complexity to enhance translatability of Alzheimer's disease mouse models: a path toward precision medicine. Neuron 101, 399411.e5. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.11.04030595332 Nguyen P. V. Abel T Kandel ER Bourtchouladze R. (2000). Strain-dependent differences in LTP and hippocampus-dependent memory in inbred mice. Learn Mem. 7, 170179. 10.1101/lm.7.3.17010837506 Nie T. Abel T. (2001). Fear conditioning in inbred mouse strains: an analysis of the time course of memory. Behav. Neurosci. 115, 951956. 10.1037/0735-7044.115.4.95111508735 Nilsson P. Saito T. Saido T. C. (2014). New mouse model of Alzheimer's. ACS Chem. Neurosci. 5, 499502. 10.1021/cn500105p24852598 O'Keefe J. Nadel L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford, UK; Oxford University Press. Oliverio A. Castellano C. Messeri P. (1972). Genetic analysis of avoidance, maze, and wheel-running behaviors in the mouse. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 79, 459473. 10.1037/h00328355054480 Olton D. S. Branch M. Best P. J. (1978). Spatial correlates of hippocampal unit activity. Exp. Neurol. 58, 387409. 10.1016/0014-4886(78)90096-17286118 Onos K. D. Uyar A. Keezer K. J. Jackson H. M. Preuss C. Acklin C. J. . (2019). Enhancing face validity of mouse models of Alzheimer's disease with natural genetic variation. PLoS Genet. 15, e1008155. 10.1371/journal.pgen.100815531150388 Packard M. G. Hirsh R. White N. M. (1989). Differential effects of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on two radial maze tasks: evidence for multiple memory systems. J. Neurosci. 9, 14651472. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-05-01465.19892723738 Packard M. G. McGaugh J. L. (1996). Inactivation of hippocampus or caudate nucleus with lidocaine differentially affects expression of place and response learning. Neurobiol. Learn Mem. 65, 6572. 10.1006/nlme.1996.00078673408 Packard M. G. White N. M. (1991). Dissociation of hippocampus and caudate nucleus memory systems by posttraining intracerebral injection of dopamine agonists. Behav. Neurosci. 105, 295306. 10.1037/0735-7044.105.2.2951675062 Paré D. Quirk G. J. Ledoux J. E. (2004). New vistas on amygdala networks in conditioned fear. J Neurophysiol. 92:19. 10.1152/jn.00153.200415212433 Passino E. Middei S. Restivo L. Bertaina-Anglade V. Ammassari-Teule M. (2002). Genetic approach to variability of memory systems: analysis of place vs. response learning and fos-related expression in hippocampal and striatal areas of C57BL/6 and DBA/2 mice. Hippocampus 12, 6375. 10.1002/hipo.1000711918290 Paylor R. Baskall L. Wehner J. M. (1993). Behavioral dissociations between C57BL/6 and DBA/2 mice on learning and memory tasks: a hippocampal-dysfunction hypothesis. Psychobiology 21, 1126. 10.3758/BF03327122 Paylor R. Tracy R. Wehner J. Rudy J. W. (1994). DBA/2 and C57BL/6 mice differ in contextual fear but not auditory fear conditioning. Behav. Neurosci. 108, 810817. 10.1037/0735-7044.108.4.8107986374 Peirce J. L. Lu L. Gu J. Silver L. M. Williams R. W. (2004). A new set of BXD recombinant inbred lines from advanced intercross populations in mice. BMC Genet. 5, 7. 10.1186/1471-2156-5-715117419 Phillips R. G. LeDoux J. E. (1992). Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behav. Neurosci. 106, 274285. 10.1037/0735-7044.106.2.2741590953 Pignataro A. Meli G. Pagano R. Fontebasso V. Battistella R. Conforto G. . (2019). Activity-induced amyloid-β oligomers drive compensatory synaptic rearrangements in brain circuits controlling memory of presymptomatic Alzheimer's disease mice. Biol. Psychiatry 86, 185195. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.10.01830528194 Pignataro A. Middei S. Borreca A. Ammassari-Teule M. (2013). Indistinguishable pattern of amygdala and hippocampus rewiring following tone or contextual fear conditioning in C57BL/6 mice. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:156. 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.0015624194705 Plomin R. McClearn G. E. Smith D. L. Vignetti S. Chorney M. J. Chorney, K. . (1994). DNA markers associated with high versus low IQ: The IQ quantitative trait loci (QTL) project. Behav Genet 24, 107118 . 10.1007/BF010678158024528 Renzi P. Sansone M. (1971). Discriminated lever-press avoidance behavior in mice. Commun. Behav. Biol. 6, 315321.4648616 Restivo L. Passino E. Middei S. Ammassari-Teule M. (2002). The strain-specific involvement of nucleus accumbens in latent inhibition might depend on differences in processing configural- and cue-based information between C57BL/6 and DBA mice. Brain Res. Bull 57, 3539. 10.1016/S0361-9230(01)00634-711827735 Restivo L. Roman F. S. Ammassari-Teule M. Marchetti E. (2006). Simultaneous olfactory discrimination elicits a strain-specific increase in dendritic spines in the hippocampus of inbred mice. Hippocampus 16, 472479. 10.1002/hipo.2017416502390 Río-Ȧlamos C. Oliveras I. Cañete T. Blázquez G Martínez-Membrives E Tobeña, A. Fernández-Teruel A. (2015). Neonatal handling decreases unconditioned anxiety, conditioned fear, and improves two-way avoidance acquisition: a study with the inbred Roman high (RHA-I)- and low-avoidance (RLA-I) rats of both sexes. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9,174. 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.0017426217201 Risinger F. O. Cunningham C. L. (1995). Genetic differences in ethanol-induced conditioned taste aversion after ethanol preexposure. Alcohol 12, 535539. 10.1016/0741-8329(95)00040-28590615 Rustay N. R. Cronin E. A. Curzon P. Markosyan S. Bitner R. S. Ellis T. . (2010). Mice expressing the Swedish APP mutation on a 129 genetic background demonstrate consistent behavioral deficits and pathological markers of Alzheimer's disease. Brain Res. 1311, 136147 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.11.04019944081 Saito T. Matsuba Y Mihira N Takano, J. Nilsson P. Itohara S. Iwata N. . (2014). Single App knock-in mouse models of Alzheimer's disease. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 661663. 10.1038/nn.369724728269 Sakakibara Y. Sekiya M. Saito T. Saido T. C Iijima KM (2019). Amyloid-β plaque formation and reactive gliosis are required for induction of cognitive deficits in App knock-in mouse models of Alzheimer's disease. BMC Neurosci. 20, 13. 10.1186/s12868-019-0496-630894120 Sato K. Watamura N. Fujioka R. Mihira N. Sekiguchi M. Nagata K. . (2021). A third-generation mouse model of Alzheimer's disease shows early and increased cored plaque pathology composed of wild-type human amyloid β peptide. J. Biol. Chem. 297, 101004. 10.1016/j.jbc.2021.10100434329683 Schwegler H. Crusio W. E. Lipp H. Heimrich B. (1988). Water-maze learning in the mouse correlates with variation in hippocampal morphology. Behav. Genet. 18, 153165. 10.1007/BF010678373377730 Selden N. R. Everitt B. J. Jarrard L. E. Robbins T. W. (1991). Complementary roles for the amygdala and hippocampus in aversive conditioning to explicit and contextual cues. Neuroscience 42, 335350. 10.1016/0306-4522(91)90379-31832750 Selkoe D. J.. (2002). Alzheimer's disease is a synaptic failure. Science 298, 78991. 10.1126/science.107406912399581 Sil A. Erfani A. Lamb N. Copland R. Riedel G. Platt B. J. (2022). Sex differences in behavior and molecular pathology in the 5xFAD model. Alzheimers Dis. 85, 755777. 10.3233/JAD-21052334864660 Singhal G. Morgan J. Jawahar M. C. Corrigan F. Jaehne E. J. Toben C. . (2019). The effects of short-term and long-term environmental enrichment on locomotion, mood-like behavior, cognition and hippocampal gene expression. Behav. Brain Res. 368, 111917. 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.11191731004685 Sittig L. J. Carbonetto P. Engel K. A. Krauss K. S. Barrios-Camacho C. M. Palmer A. A. (2016). Genetic background limits generalizability of genotype-phenotype relationships. Neuron 91, 1253129. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.08.01327618673 Sterniczuk R. Antle M. C. LaFerla F. M. Dyck R. H. (2010). Characterization of the 3xTg-AD mouse model of Alzheimer's disease: part 2. Behavioral and cognitive changes. Brain Res. 1348, 149155. 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.06.01120558146 Stiedl O. Radulovic J. Lohmann R. Birkenfeld K. Palve M. Kammermeier J. . (1999). Strain and substrain differences in context- and tone-dependent fear conditioning of inbred mice. Behav. Brain Res. 104, 112. 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00047-911125727 Street V. A. Kujawa S. G. Manichaikul A. Broman K. W. Kallman J. C. Shilling D. J. . (2014). Resistance to noise-induced hearing loss in 129S6 and MOLF mice: identification of independent, overlapping, and interacting chromosomal regions. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 15, 72138. 10.1007/s10162-014-0472-x24952082 Sturchler-Pierrat C. Staufenbiel M. (2000). Pathogenic mechanisms of Alzheimer's disease analyzed in the APP23 transgenic mouse model. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 920, 134139. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2000.tb06915.x11193142 Sultana R. Ogundele O. M. Lee C. C. (2019). Contrasting characteristic behaviours among common laboratory mouse strains. R. Soc. Open. Sci. 6,190574. 10.1098/rsos.19057431312505 Sutoko S. Masuda A. Kandori A. Sasaguri H. Saito T. Saido T. C. . (2021). Early identification of Alzheimer's disease in mouse models: application of deep neural network algorithm to cognitive behavioral parameters. iScience 24, 102198. 10.1016/j.isci.2021.10219833733064 Thinus-Blanc C. Save E. Rossi-Arnaud C. Tozzi A. Ammassari-Teule M. (1996). The differences shown by C57BL/6 and DBA/2 inbred mice in detecting spatial novelty are subserved by a different hippocampal and parietal cortex interplay. Behav. Brain Res. 80, 3340. 10.1016/0166-4328(96)00016-28905126 Upchurch M. Wehner J. M. (1988). Differences between inbred strains of mice in Morris water maze performance. Behav. Genet. 18, 5568. 10.1007/BF010670753365197 Upchurch M. Wehner J. M. (1989). Inheritance of spatial learning ability in inbred mice: a classical genetic analysis. Behav. Neurosci. 103, 12518. 10.1037/0735-7044.103.6.12512610918 van de Weerd H. A. Baumans V. Koolhaas J. M. van Zutphen L. F. (2004). Strain specific behavioural response to environmental enrichment in the mouse. J. Exp. Anim. Sci. 36, 117127. 10.1177/026119290403202s147948063 Vicari E. M.. (1929). Modes of inheritance of reaction time and degrees of learning in mice. J. Exp. Zool. 54, 3188. 10.1002/jez.1400540104 Vloeberghs E. Van Dam D. D'Hooge R. Staufenbiel M. De Deyn P. P. (2006). APP23 mice display working memory impairment in the plus-shaped water maze. Neurosci. Lett. 40, 610. 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.07.06016959424 Wehner J. M. Silva A. (1996). Importance of strain differences in evaluations of learning and memory processes in null mutants. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2, 243248. Wehner J. M. Sleight S. Upchurch M. (1990). Hippocampal protein kinase C activity is reduced in poor spatial learners. Brain Res. 523, 181187. 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91485-Y2400904 Weinberger S. B. Koob G. F. Martinez J. L. (1992). Differences in one-way active avoidance learning in mice from three inbred strains. Behav. Gene. 22, 381391. 10.1007/BF010669961596257 Wolfer D. Lipp H. P. (2000). Dissecting the behaviour of transgenic mice: is it the mutation, the genetic background, or the environment? Exp. Physiol. 85, 627634. 10.1111/j.1469-445X.2000.02095.x11187958 Zheng Q. Y. Johnson K. R. Erway L. C. (1999). Assessment of hearing in 80 inbred strains of mice by ABR threshold analyses. Hear Res. 13, 94107. 10.1016/S0378-5955(99)00003-910320101
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016www.hefqbx.com.cn
      www.fcchain.com.cn
      www.goldbeauty.com.cn
      meifubui.com.cn
      gamala.com.cn
      lianbangzg.com.cn
      hnzz666.org.cn
      www.nhchain.com.cn
      ufnews.com.cn
      www.osmxjy.org.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p